Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 7 de 7
Filter
1.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 108(52): E1470-4, 2011 Dec 27.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22160702

ABSTRACT

Relationships between primates and snakes are of widespread interest from anthropological, psychological, and evolutionary perspectives, but surprisingly, little is known about the dangers that serpents have posed to people with prehistoric lifestyles and nonhuman primates. Here, we report ethnographic observations of 120 Philippine Agta Negritos when they were still preliterate hunter-gatherers, among whom 26% of adult males had survived predation attempts by reticulated pythons. Six fatal attacks occurred between 1934 and 1973. Agta ate pythons as well as deer, wild pigs, and monkeys, which are also eaten by pythons, and therefore, the two species were reciprocally prey, predators, and potential competitors. Natural history data document snake predation on tree shrews and 26 species of nonhuman primates as well as many species of primates approaching, mobbing, killing, and sometimes eating snakes. These findings, interpreted within the context of snake and primate phylogenies, corroborate the hypothesis that complex ecological interactions have long characterized our shared evolutionary history.


Subject(s)
Biological Evolution , Ethnicity , Predatory Behavior/physiology , Primates/physiology , Snake Bites/epidemiology , Snakes/physiology , Animals , Female , Humans , Interviews as Topic , Male , Philippines/epidemiology , Phylogeny
2.
Sci Adv ; 6(26): eaax9070, 2020 06.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32637588

ABSTRACT

Human adaptation depends on the integration of slow life history, complex production skills, and extensive sociality. Refining and testing models of the evolution of human life history and cultural learning benefit from increasingly accurate measurement of knowledge, skills, and rates of production with age. We pursue this goal by inferring hunters' increases and declines of skill from approximately 23,000 hunting records generated by more than 1800 individuals at 40 locations. The data reveal an average age of peak productivity between 30 and 35 years of age, although high skill is maintained throughout much of adulthood. In addition, there is substantial variation both among individuals and sites. Within study sites, variation among individuals depends more on heterogeneity in rates of decline than in rates of increase. This analysis sharpens questions about the coevolution of human life history and cultural adaptation.

3.
J R Soc Interface ; 15(144)2018 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30021924

ABSTRACT

Monogamy appears to have become the predominant human mating system with the emergence of highly unequal agricultural populations that replaced relatively egalitarian horticultural populations, challenging the conventional idea-based on the polygyny threshold model-that polygyny should be positively associated with wealth inequality. To address this polygyny paradox, we generalize the standard polygyny threshold model to a mutual mate choice model predicting the fraction of women married polygynously. We then demonstrate two conditions that are jointly sufficient to make monogamy the predominant marriage form, even in highly unequal societies. We assess if these conditions are satisfied using individual-level data from 29 human populations. Our analysis shows that with the shift to stratified agricultural economies: (i) the population frequency of relatively poor individuals increased, increasing wealth inequality, but decreasing the frequency of individuals with sufficient wealth to secure polygynous marriage, and (ii) diminishing marginal fitness returns to additional wives prevent extremely wealthy men from obtaining as many wives as their relative wealth would otherwise predict. These conditions jointly lead to a high population-level frequency of monogamy.


Subject(s)
Marriage , Models, Theoretical , Socioeconomic Factors , Female , Humans , Male
4.
PLoS One ; 12(1): e0169181, 2017.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28081562

ABSTRACT

It has been hypothesised that facial traits such as masculinity and a healthy appearance may indicate heritable qualities in males (e.g. immunocompetence) and that, consequently, female preferences for such traits may function to increase offspring viability and health. However, the putative link between paternal facial features and offspring health has not previously been tested empirically in humans. Here we present data from two traditional societies with little or no access to modern medicine and family planning technologies. Data on offspring number and offspring survival were analysed for the Agta of the Philippines and the Maya of Belize, and archive facial photographs were assessed by observers for attractiveness and masculinity. While there was no association between attractiveness and offspring survival in either population, a quadratic relationship was observed between masculinity and offspring survival in both populations, such that intermediate levels of masculinity were associated with the lowest offspring mortality, with both high and low levels of masculinity being associated with increased mortality. Neither attractiveness nor masculinity were related to fertility (offspring number) in either population. We consider how these data may or may not reconcile with current theories of female preferences for masculinity in male faces and argue that further research and replication in other traditional societies should be a key priority for the field.


Subject(s)
Child Mortality , Infant Mortality , Masculinity , Mortality/ethnology , Physical Appearance, Body/ethnology , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Child, Preschool , Face , Female , Humans , Infant , Infant, Newborn , Male , Middle Aged , Philippines/epidemiology , Philippines/ethnology
5.
Am J Hum Biol ; 1(1): 59-72, 1989.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28514029

ABSTRACT

The Casiguran Agta constitute a Negrito hunter-gatherer society in northeastern Luzon. The hypothesis presented is that this population has suffered serious decline over the last half -century. Demographic data collected over a 24-year period are used to substantiate this hypothesis. Agta figures on crude death rate, rate of natural decrease, infant mortality, life expectancy at birth, and homicide are among the most extreme known for any human population. Reasons for this decline are described, with emphasis on the factor of homicide.

7.
Science ; 331(6022): 1286-9, 2011 Mar 11.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21393537

ABSTRACT

Contemporary humans exhibit spectacular biological success derived from cumulative culture and cooperation. The origins of these traits may be related to our ancestral group structure. Because humans lived as foragers for 95% of our species' history, we analyzed co-residence patterns among 32 present-day foraging societies (total n = 5067 individuals, mean experienced band size = 28.2 adults). We found that hunter-gatherers display a unique social structure where (i) either sex may disperse or remain in their natal group, (ii) adult brothers and sisters often co-reside, and (iii) most individuals in residential groups are genetically unrelated. These patterns produce large interaction networks of unrelated adults and suggest that inclusive fitness cannot explain extensive cooperation in hunter-gatherer bands. However, large social networks may help to explain why humans evolved capacities for social learning that resulted in cumulative culture.


Subject(s)
Cultural Evolution , Family , Population Groups , Residence Characteristics , Social Behavior , Adult , Cooperative Behavior , Cross-Cultural Comparison , Female , Humans , Male
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL