Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 30
Filter
Add more filters

Affiliation country
Publication year range
1.
J Pers Assess ; 102(3): 297-308, 2020.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30657714

ABSTRACT

Missing data is a problem that occurs frequently in many scientific areas. The most sophisticated method for dealing with this problem is multiple imputation. Contrary to other methods, like listwise deletion, this method does not throw away information, and partly repairs the problem of systematic dropout. Although from a theoretical point of view multiple imputation is considered to be the optimal method, many applied researchers are reluctant to use it because of persistent misconceptions about this method. Instead of providing an(other) overview of missing data methods, or extensively explaining how multiple imputation works, this article aims specifically at rebutting these misconceptions, and provides applied researchers with practical arguments supporting them in the use of multiple imputation.


Subject(s)
Data Interpretation, Statistical , Research Design , Humans
2.
Prev Sci ; 17(2): 259-73, 2016 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26411312

ABSTRACT

This meta-analysis reports on the effectiveness of targeted interventions focusing on child care professionals to improve child care quality, caregiver interaction skills, and child social-emotional development. Within randomized controlled trials, interventions are moderately effective in improving overall caregiver-child interactions (k = 19, Hedges' g = 0.35) and in improving child care quality on the classroom level (k = 11; Hedges' g = 0.39), the caregiver level (k = 10; Hedges' g = 0.44), and the child level (k = 6; Hedges' g = 0.26). Based on these findings, the implementation of evidence-based targeted interventions on a larger scale than currently exists may lead to better social-emotional development for children under the age of 5 years. There remains, however, an urgent need for more and larger randomized controlled trials with a solid design and high quality measures in order to shed more light on which child care components for which children are most critical in supporting children's socio-emotional development.


Subject(s)
Caregivers , Child Care/standards , Quality Improvement , Child, Preschool , Databases, Factual , Humans , Infant , Program Evaluation , Randomized Controlled Trials as Topic
3.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 130: 193-208, 2015 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25462041

ABSTRACT

During early childhood, girls outperform boys on key dimensions of cognitive functions, including inhibitory control, sustained attention, and working memory. The role of parenting in these sex differences is unknown despite evidence that boys are more sensitive to the effects of the early environment. In this study, we measured parental sensitivity at 14 and 36 months of age, and children's cognitive and executive functions (sustained attention, inhibitory control, and forward/backward memory) at 52 months of age, in a longitudinal cohort (N=752). Boys scored significantly lower than girls on inhibitory control (more Go/NoGo "commission errors") and short-term memory (forward color recall task), but boys did not differ from girls on attention (Go/NoGo "omission errors") or working memory (backward color recall task). In stratified analyses, parental sensitivity at 36 months of age was negatively associated with number of errors of commission (p=.05) and omission (p=.02) in boys, whereas child's age was the only significant predictor of commission and omission errors in girls. A combined analysis of both sexes confirmed an interaction between sex and parenting for omission errors (p=.03). The results indicate that sex differences in cognitive functions are evident in preschoolers, although not across all dimensions we assessed. Boys appear to be more vulnerable to early parenting effects, but only in association with omission errors (attention) and not with the other cognitive function dimensions.


Subject(s)
Attention , Executive Function , Memory, Short-Term , Parent-Child Relations , Parenting/psychology , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Male , Netherlands , Neuropsychological Tests/statistics & numerical data , Sex Factors
4.
Dev Psychobiol ; 57(1): 35-49, 2015 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25251423

ABSTRACT

This study is the first to investigate the longitudinal effects of gene-environment interplay between DRD4 genotype and maternal sensitivity on child externalizing behavior. Multiple measures of maternal sensitivity (14, 36, and 48 months) and externalizing behavior (18 months, 36 months, and 5 years) were assessed in a large cohort study (N = 548). Early maternal insensitivity (14 months) was associated with early externalizing behavior (18 months) in a for better and for worse manner, but only in children with at least one DRD4 7-repeat, consistent with a differential susceptibility model. Later insensitivity (48 months) predicted externalizing behavior at age 5 independent of DRD4 genotype. A structural equation model including all measures across time supported the differential susceptibility model: The overall effect of early maternal sensitivity on later externalizing behavior was significant only for children with a DRD4 7-repeat allele. The results highlight the importance of studying gene-environment interactions across development.


Subject(s)
Child Behavior/psychology , Mother-Child Relations/psychology , Receptors, Dopamine D4/genetics , Adult , Age Factors , Alleles , Child Development , Child, Preschool , Gene-Environment Interaction , Humans , Infant , Minisatellite Repeats/genetics , Mothers/psychology , Prospective Studies , Receptors, Dopamine D4/physiology
5.
Attach Hum Dev ; 17(3): 241-56, 2015.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25912711

ABSTRACT

We examined whether differences in adult attachment representations could be predicted from early and later maternal sensitivity, controlling for early and later assessments of attachment. In this longitudinal study on 190 adoptees, attachment at 23 years was measured with the Attachment Script Assessment. Maternal sensitivity was observed in infancy and at seven and 14 years. Attachment was also measured in infancy and at 14 years. Higher maternal sensitivity in infancy predicted more secure attachment in infancy and more secure attachment representations in young adulthood. Higher maternal sensitivity in middle childhood also predicted more secure attachment representations in young adulthood. There was no continuity of attachment from infancy to young adulthood, but attachment in adolescence and young adulthood were significantly related. Even in genetically unrelated families, maternal sensitivity in early and middle childhood predicts attachment representations in young adults, confirming the importance of sensitive parenting for human development.


Subject(s)
Adoption/psychology , Child Development , Mother-Child Relations/psychology , Object Attachment , Adolescent , Child , Female , Humans , Infant , Infant, Newborn , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Young Adult
6.
J Youth Adolesc ; 43(4): 528-40, 2014 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23828726

ABSTRACT

Internalizing symptoms such as withdrawn and anxious-depressed behavior are common in adolescence. This prospective longitudinal study helps to gain insight into the development of internalizing behavior, focusing on the role of early parent-child interaction while ruling out genetic similarity as a confounder. More specifically, the central question addressed in this study was whether parental sensitivity and child inhibited temperament predict children's withdrawn and anxious-depressed behavior in middle childhood and adolescence. We followed 160 early-adopted children (53 % girls) from infancy to adolescence. Structural equation modeling was used to test relationships both prospectively and concurrently. The results revealed that more sensitive parenting in infancy and middle childhood predicted less inhibited behavior in adolescence, which in turn predicted fewer internalizing problems in adolescence. The findings suggest that maternal sensitivity lowers adolescents' inhibited behavior and decreases the risk for adolescents' internalizing problem behavior indirectly through lower levels of inhibition. Supporting sensitive parenting in the years before adolescence may protect children from developing inhibited behavior and internalizing behavior problems in adolescence.


Subject(s)
Adolescent Behavior/psychology , Child Behavior Disorders/diagnosis , Parent-Child Relations , Parenting/psychology , Adolescent , Child , Child Behavior Disorders/psychology , Female , Humans , Inhibition, Psychological , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Surveys and Questionnaires , United Kingdom
7.
J Child Lang ; 41(5): 963-84, 2014 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24067295

ABSTRACT

When bilingual children enter formal reading education, host language proficiency becomes increasingly important. This study investigated the relation between socioeconomic status (SES), maternal language use, reading input, and vocabulary in a sample of 111 six-year-old children of first- and second-generation Turkish immigrant parents in the Netherlands. Mothers reported on their language use with the child, frequency of reading by both parents, and availability of children's books in the ethnic and the host language. Children's Dutch and Turkish vocabulary were tested during a home visit. SES was related to maternal language use and to host language reading input. Reading input mediated the relation between SES and host language vocabulary and between maternal language use and host language vocabulary, whereas only maternal language use was related to ethnic language vocabulary. During transition to formal reading education, one should be aware that children from low-SES families receive less host language reading input.


Subject(s)
Ethnicity/statistics & numerical data , Minority Groups/statistics & numerical data , Mothers/psychology , Multilingualism , Reading , Vocabulary , Child , Child, Preschool , Emigrants and Immigrants/psychology , Emigrants and Immigrants/statistics & numerical data , Ethnicity/psychology , Humans , Minority Groups/psychology , Mothers/statistics & numerical data , Netherlands , Socioeconomic Factors , Surveys and Questionnaires , Turkey/ethnology
8.
Child Maltreat ; 29(1): 53-65, 2024 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36154718

ABSTRACT

The aim of this study was to investigate (a) the extent to which child maltreatment co-occurs with parental separation and (b) associations between different types of child maltreatment and various types of separation-associated interparental conflict. Professionals working with children (N = 785) reported each case of suspected child maltreatment they observed during a 3-month period and indicated whether parental divorce or separation was about to take place or had taken place. This resulted in 530 reported cases that matched the definitions of child maltreatment for which information on parental relationship status was available. Most of the maltreated children (60%) also experienced (impending) parental separation. In 69% of these cases child maltreatment was associated with parental separation. Particularly, cases of emotional neglect, and emotional abuse co-occurred with parental separation. In addition, four clusters of separation-associated interparental conflict were distinguished- No observed conflict, Non-physical conflict, Verbal and physical conflict, and Multiple conflict-which were associated with child and family characteristics and specific types of child maltreatment. The results of this study suggest that child maltreatment often co-occurs with parental separation, especially when there is a considerable amount of interparental conflict.


Subject(s)
Child Abuse , Divorce , Child , Humans , Divorce/psychology , Child Abuse/psychology , Parents , Family Conflict/psychology , Family Characteristics
9.
Child Abuse Negl ; 149: 106699, 2024 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38417291

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Understanding how child maltreatment is passed down from one generation to the next is crucial for the development of intervention and prevention strategies that may break the cycle of child maltreatment. Changes in emotion recognition due to childhood maltreatment have repeatedly been found, and may underly the intergenerational transmission of child maltreatment. OBJECTIVE: In this study we, therefore, examined whether the ability to recognize emotions plays a role in the intergenerational transmission of child abuse and neglect. PARTICIPANTS AND SETTING: A total of 250 parents (104 males, 146 females) were included that participated in a three-generation family study. METHOD: Participants completed an emotion recognition task in which they were presented with series of photographs that depicted the unfolding of facial expressions from neutrality to the peak emotions anger, fear, happiness, and sadness. Multi-informant measures were used to examine experienced and perpetrated child maltreatment. RESULTS: A history of abuse, but not neglect, predicted a shorter reaction time to identify fear and anger. In addition, parents who showed higher levels of neglectful behavior made more errors in identifying fear, whereas parents who showed higher levels of abusive behavior made more errors in identifying anger. Emotion recognition did not mediate the association between experienced and perpetrated child maltreatment. CONCLUSIONS: Findings highlight the importance of distinguishing between abuse and neglect when investigating the precursors and sequalae of child maltreatment. In addition, the effectiveness of interventions that aim to break the cycle of abuse and neglect could be improved by better addressing the specific problems with emotion processing of abusive and neglectful parents.


Subject(s)
Child Abuse , Extended Family , Male , Female , Humans , Child , Emotions , Child Abuse/psychology , Fear/psychology , Anger
10.
Stress ; 16(6): 711-5, 2013 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23786528

ABSTRACT

Quantification of cortisol in scalp hair seems a promising measurement for long-term cortisol levels, and thereby a biomarker for stress. We examined hair cortisol concentrations (HCC) in children when first entering elementary school. Participants were 42 children (45% boys) with a mean age of 4.2 years (SD = 0.42 months). Hair samples (≥5 cm) were collected 2 months after school entry. Hair analysis was conducted using two 2-cm long segments, reflecting the first 2 months of school attendance (the scalp-near segment) and 2 months prior to school entry. HCC were higher after school entry than before, especially for fearful children. Alterations in HCC were not moderated by experience in group daycare before school entry. Thus, HCC suggest that starting elementary school is accompanied by increased stress hormone levels in young (in particular fearful) children.


Subject(s)
Hair/chemistry , Hydrocortisone/analysis , Schools , Stress, Psychological , Biomarkers/analysis , Child Care , Child, Preschool , Fear , Female , Humans , Male
11.
Dev Psychobiol ; 55(5): 451-64, 2013 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22614783

ABSTRACT

Maternal discipline is an important predictor of child committed compliance. Maternal stress can affect both parenting and child development. In a large population-based cohort study (N = 613) we examined whether maternal discipline mediated the association between maternal stress during pregnancy and child compliance, and whether COMT or DRD4 polymorphisms moderated the association between maternal discipline and child compliance. Family-related and general stress were measured through maternal self-report and genetic material was collected through cord blood sampling at birth. Mother-child dyads were observed at 36 months in disciplinary tasks in which the child was not allowed to touch attractive toys. Maternal discipline and child compliance were observed in two different tasks and independently coded. The association between family stress during pregnancy and child committed compliance was mediated by maternal positive discipline. Children with more COMT Met alleles seemed more susceptible to maternal positive discipline than children with more COMT Val alleles.


Subject(s)
Catechol O-Methyltransferase/genetics , Child Behavior/physiology , Maternal Behavior/psychology , Parenting/psychology , Prenatal Exposure Delayed Effects/genetics , Stress, Psychological/genetics , Alleles , Child , Child Behavior/psychology , Child Development , Female , Genetic Predisposition to Disease , Genotype , Humans , Male , Mother-Child Relations , Polymorphism, Single Nucleotide , Pregnancy , Prenatal Exposure Delayed Effects/psychology , Stress, Psychological/psychology
12.
Child Psychiatry Hum Dev ; 44(6): 751-65, 2013 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23408268

ABSTRACT

The goal of this study is to clarify the relation between maternal sensitivity and internalizing problems during the preschool period. For this purpose, a longitudinal, bidirectional model was tested in two large prospective, population-based cohorts, the Generation R Study and the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development (NICHD SECCYD), including over 1,800 mother-child dyads in total. Maternal sensitivity was repeatedly observed in mother-child interaction tasks and information on child internalizing problems was obtained from maternal reports. Modest but consistent associations between maternal sensitivity and internalizing problems were found in both cohorts, confirming the importance of sensitive parenting for positive development in the preschool years. Pathways from maternal sensitivity to child internalizing problems were consistently observed but child-to-mother pathways were only found in the NICHD SECCYD sample.


Subject(s)
Behavioral Symptoms/psychology , Child Development/physiology , Mother-Child Relations , Mothers/psychology , Parenting/psychology , Adult , Child , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Infant , Male , Models, Psychological , Netherlands , Prospective Studies , United States
13.
Dev Psychol ; 59(4): 655-668, 2023 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36548041

ABSTRACT

This study applies a within-family, age-snapshot design to investigate differences between siblings in the development of compliance during the preschool years by disaggregating situational, within-family, and between-family effects. The aim of the study was to investigate the relation between sibling differences in compliance and the within-family factors birth order and differential parenting, as well as interactions between these factors. Using observational data of 311 Dutch families (self-identified as culturally Dutch) with 2 children when each child was 3 and 4 years old (firstborns: 36.2 months old; SD = 3.6; 48% girls, second-borns (2 years later): 36.67 months old; SD = .62; 47% girls) and both parents. Three-level cross-classified multilevel models showed main effects of observed sibling noncompliance and differential verbal discipline on noncompliance. In addition, second-born children were more compliant than their firstborn siblings, but only when the firstborn was disciplined physically more often than his/her younger sibling. The results provide evidence that birth-order effects may partially be explained by differential parenting and suggest that differences between siblings cannot be fully understood without taking into account the influence of both direct and indirect sibling effects. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).


Subject(s)
Birth Order , Siblings , Child , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Infant , Male , Ethnicity , Parenting , Parents
14.
J Pers Assess ; 94(1): 12-25, 2012.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22176263

ABSTRACT

This article is set up as a tutorial for nonlinear principal components analysis (NLPCA), systematically guiding the reader through the process of analyzing actual data on personality assessment by the Rorschach Inkblot Test. NLPCA is a more flexible alternative to linear PCA that can handle the analysis of possibly nonlinearly related variables with different types of measurement level. The method is particularly suited to analyze nominal (qualitative) and ordinal (e.g., Likert-type) data, possibly combined with numeric data. The program CATPCA from the Categories module in SPSS is used in the analyses, but the method description can easily be generalized to other software packages.


Subject(s)
Principal Component Analysis/methods , Nonlinear Dynamics
15.
Child Abuse Negl ; 103: 104439, 2020 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32126398

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: The third Netherlands' Prevalence study of Maltreatment of children and youth (NPM-2017) continues the tradition of periodically mapping the national prevalence of child maltreatment. OBJECTIVE: The NPM-2017 provides an update of the current prevalence rates of child maltreatment and of changes in its prevalence over the last 12 years. In addition, risk factors for child maltreatment and its co-occurrence with domestic violence were investigated. PARTICIPANTS AND SETTING: Prevalence data were based on cases reported to 'Safe at Home' organizations (former CPS agencies) and observations of professionals working with children (sentinels). METHODS: Sentinels (N = 785) filled out a form for each case of suspected child maltreatment that they observed within their professional sample during a three-month period. RESULTS: An overall prevalence estimate of child maltreatment in the Netherlands in 2017 of 26-37 per 1000 children was computed. The most important risk factors for child maltreatment were low parental education (RR=4.95), parental unemployment (RR = 3.64), immigrant status (RR = 3.61), and single parenthood (RR = 2.29). Neither prevalence rates nor risk factors changed significantly between 2005, 2010, and 2017. Finally, in 46 % of the reported families child maltreatment occurred in a context of domestic violence. CONCLUSIONS: The prevalence of co-occurring domestic violence may indicate that family violence observed within one dyad could be a marker for dysfunctional functioning of the family system. Child maltreatment remains a considerable problem in the Netherlands with a stable prevalence over the last 12 years and stability in characteristics that make families vulnerable for child maltreatment.


Subject(s)
Child Abuse/statistics & numerical data , Adolescent , Child , Child, Preschool , Cross-Sectional Studies , Domestic Violence/statistics & numerical data , Educational Status , Emigrants and Immigrants , Female , Humans , Infant , Infant, Newborn , Male , Netherlands/epidemiology , Parents , Prevalence , Risk Factors , Time Factors , Unemployment
16.
J Fam Psychol ; 34(4): 459-468, 2020 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31829668

ABSTRACT

To thrive as an individual and within society, children need to develop the ability to control their behavior. Using a twin design, we estimated the relative influence of genetic, shared, and unique environmental factors on hot and cool effortful control (EC). Furthermore, we investigated whether parental sensitivity in a play, task, or discipline context when the children were on average 3.78 years old, was differentially related to children's hot and cool EC 1 year later (Mage 4.77 years). We included 476 children from 238 twin pairs (48% boys, 58% monozygotic) and their primary parent. Hot EC (delay of gratification) was measured with the marshmallow test and cool EC (response inhibition) was measured with a stop-signal task. The behavioral genetics analyses showed that individual differences in hot and cool EC were mostly explained by unique environmental factors, whereas their association was mostly explained by shared environmental factors. Controlling for sensitivity in the other contexts, task sensitivity contributed to the prediction of cool EC, and sensitive discipline contributed to both cool and hot EC. Play sensitivity did not contribute to the prediction of hot or cool EC over and above parental sensitivity in the other contexts. Our study contributes to a better understanding of the unique and shared antecedents of hot and cool EC, suggesting parental sensitive discipline as a focus for preventive interventions targeting both hot and cool EC. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).


Subject(s)
Child Behavior/psychology , Parent-Child Relations , Parenting/psychology , Self-Control/psychology , Social Environment , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Male
17.
Child Maltreat ; 25(3): 289-299, 2020 08.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31773993

ABSTRACT

Child-driven genetic factors can contribute to negative parenting and may increase the risk of being maltreated. Experiencing childhood maltreatment may be partly heritable, but results of twin studies are mixed. In the current study, we used a cross-sectional extended family design to estimate genetic and environmental effects on experiencing child maltreatment. The sample consisted of 395 individuals (225 women; Mage = 38.85 years, rangeage = 7-88 years) from 63 families with two or three participating generations. Participants were oversampled for experienced maltreatment. Self-reported experienced child maltreatment was measured using a questionnaire assessing physical and emotional abuse, and physical and emotional neglect. All maltreatment phenotypes were partly heritable with percentages for h2 ranging from 30% (SE = 13%) for neglect to 62% (SE = 19%) for severe physical abuse. Common environmental effects (c2) explained a statistically significant proportion of variance for all phenotypes except for the experience of severe physical abuse (c2 = 9%, SE = 13%, p = .26). The genetic correlation between abuse and neglect was ρg = .73 (p = .02). Common environmental variance increased as socioeconomic status (SES) decreased (p = .05), but additive genetic and unique environmental variances were constant across different levels of SES.


Subject(s)
Adult Survivors of Child Abuse/psychology , Child Abuse/psychology , Family/psychology , Adult , Adult Survivors of Child Abuse/statistics & numerical data , Child , Child Abuse/statistics & numerical data , Cross-Sectional Studies , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Risk Factors
18.
Dev Psychobiol ; 50(6): 615-25, 2008 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18683185

ABSTRACT

This study explores the relation between variations in the serotonin transporter gene (5-HTTLPR; long vs. short allele), the child's attachment representation (assessed with the Attachment Story Completion Task, reflecting the security of the parent-child relationship), and electrodermal reactivity during a public speaking task, the Trier Social Stress Test for Children (TSST-C) in a sample of 92 7-year-old. Electrodermal reactivity during the TSST-C was not directly associated with variations in 5-HTT. However, there was a significant gene-environment interaction effect of 5-HTT and attachment security on electrodermal reactivity. Results are interpreted in terms of cumulative protection: Children with a secure attachment representation as well as long 5-HTT alleles appeared to be less stressed during the TSST-C.


Subject(s)
Galvanic Skin Response/genetics , Object Attachment , Polymorphism, Genetic/genetics , Serotonin Plasma Membrane Transport Proteins/genetics , Social Behavior , Stress, Psychological/diagnosis , Adaptation, Psychological/physiology , Alleles , Child , Child Behavior/psychology , Female , Humans , Internal-External Control , Male , Mothers/psychology , Mothers/statistics & numerical data , Netherlands , Parent-Child Relations , Personality Assessment/statistics & numerical data , Sex Distribution , Speech , Stress, Psychological/genetics , Task Performance and Analysis
19.
Psychol Methods ; 12(3): 336-58, 2007 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17784798

ABSTRACT

The authors provide a didactic treatment of nonlinear (categorical) principal components analysis (PCA). This method is the nonlinear equivalent of standard PCA and reduces the observed variables to a number of uncorrelated principal components. The most important advantages of nonlinear over linear PCA are that it incorporates nominal and ordinal variables and that it can handle and discover nonlinear relationships between variables. Also, nonlinear PCA can deal with variables at their appropriate measurement level; for example, it can treat Likert-type scales ordinally instead of numerically. Every observed value of a variable can be referred to as a category. While performing PCA, nonlinear PCA converts every category to a numeric value, in accordance with the variable's analysis level, using optimal quantification. The authors discuss how optimal quantification is carried out, what analysis levels are, which decisions have to be made when applying nonlinear PCA, and how the results can be interpreted. The strengths and limitations of the method are discussed. An example applying nonlinear PCA to empirical data using the program CATPCA (J. J. Meulman, W. J. Heiser, & SPSS, 2004) is provided.


Subject(s)
Models, Psychological , Child , Humans
20.
Psychol Methods ; 12(3): 359-79, 2007 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17784799

ABSTRACT

Principal components analysis (PCA) is used to explore the structure of data sets containing linearly related numeric variables. Alternatively, nonlinear PCA can handle possibly nonlinearly related numeric as well as nonnumeric variables. For linear PCA, the stability of its solution can be established under the assumption of multivariate normality. For nonlinear PCA, however, standard options for establishing stability are not provided. The authors use the nonparametric bootstrap procedure to assess the stability of nonlinear PCA results, applied to empirical data. They use confidence intervals for the variable transformations and confidence ellipses for the eigenvalues, the component loadings, and the person scores. They discuss the balanced version of the bootstrap, bias estimation, and Procrustes rotation. To provide a benchmark, the same bootstrap procedure is applied to linear PCA on the same data. On the basis of the results, the authors advise using at least 1,000 bootstrap samples, using Procrustes rotation on the bootstrap results, examining the bootstrap distributions along with the confidence regions, and merging categories with small marginal frequencies to reduce the variance of the bootstrap results.


Subject(s)
Empirical Research , Models, Psychological , Social Environment , Humans
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL