Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 34
Filter
1.
Exp Brain Res ; 225(4): 491-8, 2013 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23371745

ABSTRACT

Some results in the literature suggest that crossmodal attention is very sensitive to the features of the experimental protocol. The current work examined the possible contribution of the asynchrony between the onset of the cue and the target (SOA) and the kind of task performed by the observer to the manifestation of crossmodal attentional effect. In a first experiment, a target (Gabor patch), whose spatial frequency had to be discriminated, was presented 133 or 159 ms after an auditory cue, in a close location on the same side or in a distant location on the opposite side. The crossmodal attentional effect was observed only for the 159 ms SOA. In a second experiment, the SOA was again 133 ms, but the location of the target had to be discriminated, instead of its spatial frequency. A crossmodal attentional effect was observed. The results of these two experiments indicate that crossmodal attentional effect depends on the SOA and the task. It takes longer to develop when the task requires the discrimination of the spatial frequency of the target than the discrimination of its location.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Acoustic Stimulation , Cues , Female , Humans , Male , Orientation/physiology , Photic Stimulation , Space Perception/physiology , Young Adult
2.
Psychol Res ; 75(1): 24-34, 2011 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20559654

ABSTRACT

There is evidence that automatic visual attention favors the right side. This study investigated whether this lateral asymmetry interacts with the right hemisphere dominance for visual location processing and left hemisphere dominance for visual shape processing. Volunteers were tested in a location discrimination task and a shape discrimination task. The target stimuli (S2) could occur in the left or right hemifield. They were preceded by an ipsilateral, contralateral or bilateral prime stimulus (S1). The attentional effect produced by the right S1 was larger than that produced by the left S1. This lateral asymmetry was similar between the two tasks suggesting that the hemispheric asymmetries of visual mechanisms do not contribute to it. The finding that it was basically due to a longer reaction time to the left S2 than to the right S2 for the contralateral S1 condition suggests that the inhibitory component of attention is laterally asymmetric.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Form Perception/physiology , Functional Laterality/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation/methods , Reaction Time/physiology , Task Performance and Analysis , Young Adult
3.
Braz J Med Biol Res ; 39(7): 957-68, 2006 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16862287

ABSTRACT

The influence of a peripheral cue represented by a gray ring on responsivity to a subsequent target varies. When a vertical line inside a ring was a go target and a white small ring inside a ring was a no-go target, reaction time was shorter at the same location relative to a different location. However, no reaction time difference between the two locations occurred when a white cross inside the ring, instead of the white vertical line inside the ring, was the go target. We investigated whether this last finding was due to a forward masking influence of the cue, a requirement of low attention for the discrimination or a lack of attention mobilization by the cue. In Experiment 1, the intensity of the cue was reduced in an attempt to reduce forward masking. In Experiment 2, the vertical line and the cross were presented in the same block of trials so as to be dealt with a common attentional strategy. In Experiments 3 and 4, the no-go target was a 45 masculine rotated cross inside a ring to increase the difficulty of the discrimination. No evidence was obtained that the cross was forward masked by the cue nor that it demanded less attention to be discriminated from the small ring. There was a facilitation of responsivity by the cue when the small ring was replaced by the rotated cross. The results suggest that when the discrimination to be performed is too easy the cue does not mobilize attention.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Cues , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Perceptual Masking/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male
4.
Behav Brain Res ; 24(1): 47-57, 1987 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3580115

ABSTRACT

Normal, blind, anosmic, and unilaterally blind and contralaterally anosmic albino rats were submitted to unilateral peripheral sensory (olfactory and/or visual) occlusion and observed in an initially unfamiliar arena. Next day, either the same sensory periphery (control subjects) or the contralateral one (experimental subjects) was occluded and a new observation in the arena was made. The duration of the exploratory behavior of control and experimental subjects, on each occasion, was compared. There was not a complete internasal-interocular transfer of long-term habituation of exploratory behavior, either when olfaction and vision were suppressed on opposite sides or when they were suppressed on the same side, but there was a complete internasal transfer of this habituation in blind animals and a complete interocular transfer of this habituation in anosmic animals. These results suggest that long-term habituation of the exploratory behavior elicited by one olfactory and one visual periphery activation and that of the exploratory behavior elicited by the other olfactory and the other visual periphery activation depend upon different representations of the stimulatory situation in the central nervous system. These representations would, however, have only a small number of elements which are not shared.


Subject(s)
Exploratory Behavior/physiology , Functional Laterality , Habituation, Psychophysiologic/physiology , Smell , Transfer, Psychology/physiology , Vision, Ocular , Animals , Blindness/physiopathology , Functional Laterality/physiology , Male , Olfaction Disorders/physiopathology , Rats , Smell/physiology , Synapses/physiology
5.
Brain Res ; 550(2): 197-204, 1991 Jun 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1884232

ABSTRACT

The trigeminal motor nucleus (MoV) contains the somata of the motoneurons that control jaw position and jaw movements. This nucleus is of neurochemical interest because it receives a dense serotonergic input. We examined the effects of application of serotonin or fluoxetine, a serotonin reuptake blocker, into this nucleus on the spontaneous or reflex (jaw-closure) electrical activity of the masseter muscle in behaving cats. Serotonin produced a clearcut enhancement of both spontaneous and reflex activities. This action was attenuated by previous systemic injection of the serotonin receptor antagonist methysergide. The effect was mimicked to a certain extent by fluoxetine. These data provide evidence that the serotonergic input to MoV exerts a general facilitatory influence on masseter motoneurons activity.


Subject(s)
Masseter Muscle/physiology , Motor Neurons/physiology , Reflex/drug effects , Serotonin/pharmacology , Trigeminal Nuclei/physiology , Animals , Cats , Electric Stimulation , Electromyography , Female , Male , Masseter Muscle/drug effects , Masseter Muscle/innervation , Motor Neurons/drug effects , Reference Values , Trigeminal Nuclei/drug effects
6.
Brain Res Bull ; 44(3): 297-305, 1997.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9323445

ABSTRACT

Nonspecific lesion and stimulation methods have suggested that the hypothalamus is critical for the expression of defensive behavior, although the organization of neural circuits mediating such behavior is unclear. In the rat hypothalamus, we found that increased Fos levels were restricted to specific cell groups following presentation of a stimulus (predator) known to elicit partly innate defensive responses. The dorsal premammillary nucleus showed the most striking increase in Fos levels, and cell body-specific chemical lesions therein virtually eliminated two major components of defensive behavior but increased exploratory behavior, suggesting that this caudal hypothalamic nucleus plays a critical role in the expression of behavioral responses sometimes critical for survival of the individual. We have previously shown that the Fos-responsive cell groups in the medial hypothalamus are interconnected in a neural system distinct from those mediating reproductive and ingestive behaviors.


Subject(s)
Escape Reaction/physiology , Fear/physiology , Mammillary Bodies/physiopathology , Proto-Oncogene Proteins c-fos/analysis , Animals , Cats , Denervation , Excitatory Amino Acid Agonists , Ibotenic Acid , Male , Mammillary Bodies/chemistry , Motivation , Predatory Behavior , Rats , Rats, Wistar , Sexual Behavior, Animal/physiology
7.
Braz J Med Biol Res ; 19(1): 89-95, 1986.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3801728

ABSTRACT

Albino rats were individually placed in an unfamiliar arena and their exploratory behavior was recorded for 1 h on two consecutive days. Subjects that were tested with the right (left) nostril occluded and retested with the left (right) nostril occluded explored the arena for the same period of time as subjects that were tested and retested with the right (left) nostril occluded, in both sessions. Subjects without one olfactory bulb that were tested with the contralateral nostril occluded and retested with the ipsilateral nostril occluded explored as subjects without one olfactory bulb that were tested and retested with the ipsilateral nostril occluded, in both sessions. Subjects that were tested with the right (left) eyelids sutured and retested with the left (right) eyelids sutured explored for the same time in the test session as subjects that were tested with the right and the left eyelids sutured and retested with the right or the left eyelids sutured and subjects that were tested and retested with the right (left) eyelids sutured. In the retest session, the first group of subjects explored for less time than the second and for the same time as the third one. A complete internasal transfer of habituation of exploratory behavior probably occurred. There was a complete interocular transfer of habituation of exploratory behavior. This indicates that there is no developed specification of the side of peripheral origin of the visual information in the neural representation of the stimulatory situation responsible for habituation of exploratory behavior.


Subject(s)
Central Nervous System/physiology , Exploratory Behavior , Habituation, Psychophysiologic , Olfactory Pathways/physiology , Transfer, Psychology , Visual Pathways/physiology , Animals , Behavior, Animal , Male , Rats
8.
Braz J Med Biol Res ; 30(1): 79-83, 1997 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9222408

ABSTRACT

There is a dense serotonergic projection from nucleus raphe pallidus and nucleus raphe obscurus to the trigeminal motors nucleus and serotonin exerts a strong facilitatory action on the trigeminal motoneurons. Some serotonergic neurons in these caudal raphe nuclei increase their discharged during feeding. The objective of the present study was to investigate the possibility that the activity of these serotonergic neurons is related to activity of masticatory muscles. Cats were implanted with microelectrodes and gross electrodes. Caudal raphe single neuron activity, electrocorticographic activity, and splenius, digastric and masseter electromyographic activities were recorded during active behaviors (feeding and grooming), during quiet waking and during sleep. Seven presumed serotonergic neurons were identified. These neurons showed a long duration action potential (> 2.0 msec) and discharged slowly (2-7 Hz) and very regularly (interspike interval coefficient of variation < 0.3) during quiet waking. The activity of these neurons decreased remarkably during fast wave sleep (78-100%). Six of these neurons showed tonic changes in their activity positively related to digastric and/or masseter muscles activity but not to splenius muscle activity during waking. These data are consistent with the hypothesis that serotonergic neurons in the caudal raphe nuclei play an important role in the control of jaw movements.


Subject(s)
Masseter Muscle/physiology , Masticatory Muscles/physiology , Neurons/physiology , Raphe Nuclei/physiology , Serotonin/physiology , Animals , Cats , Male
9.
Braz J Med Biol Res ; 37(8): 1161-74, 2004 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15273817

ABSTRACT

An auditory stimulus speeds up a digital response to a subsequent visual stimulus. This facilitatory effect has been related to the expectancy and the immediate arousal that would be caused by the accessory stimulus. The present study examined the relative contribution of these two influences. In a first and a third experiment a simple reaction time task was used. In a second and fourth experiment a go/no-go reaction time task was used. In each of these experiments, the accessory stimulus preceded the target stimulus by 200 ms for one group of male and female volunteers (GFix). For another group of similar volunteers (GVar) the accessory stimulus preceded the target stimulus by 200 ms in 25% of the trials, by 1000 ms in 25% of the trials and was not followed by the target stimulus in 50% of the trials (Experiments 1a and 1b) or preceded the target stimulus by 200 ms in 6% of the trials and by 1000 ms in 94% of the trials (Experiments 2a and 2b). There was a facilitatory effect of the accessory stimulus for GFix in the four experiments. There was also a facilitatory effect of the accessory stimulus at the 200-ms stimulus onset asynchrony for GVar in Experiments 1a and 1b but not in Experiments 2a and 2b. The facilitatory effects observed were larger in the go/no-go task than in the simple task. Taken together, these results suggest that expectancy is much more important than immediate arousal for the improvement of performance caused by an accessory stimulus.


Subject(s)
Acoustic Stimulation/methods , Arousal/physiology , Auditory Perception/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Attention/physiology , Female , Humans , Male
10.
Braz J Med Biol Res ; 34(7): 919-37, 2001 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11449311

ABSTRACT

We investigated the behavioral correlates of the activity of serotonergic and non-serotonergic neurons in the nucleus raphe pallidus (NRP) and nucleus raphe obscurus (NRO) of unanesthetized and unrestrained cats. The animals were implanted with electrodes for recording single unit activity, parietal oscillographic activity, and splenius, digastric and masseter electromyographic activities. They were tested along the waking-sleep cycle, during sensory stimulation and during drinking behavior. The discharge of the serotonergic neurons decreased progressively from quiet waking to slow wave sleep and to fast wave sleep. Ten different patterns of relative discharge across the three states were observed for the non-serotonergic neurons. Several non-serotonergic neurons showed cyclic discharge fluctuations related to respiration during one, two or all three states. While serotonergic neurons were usually unresponsive to the sensory stimuli used, many non-serotonergic neurons responded to these stimuli. Several non-serotonergic neurons showed a phasic relationship with splenius muscle activity during auditory stimulation. One serotonergic neuron showed a tonic relationship with digastric muscle activity during drinking behavior. A few non-serotonergic neurons exhibited a tonic relationship with digastric and/or masseter muscle activity during this behavior. Many non-serotonergic neurons exhibited a phasic relationship with these muscle activities, also during this behavior. These results suggest that the serotonergic neurons in the NRP and NRO constitute a relatively homogeneous population from a functional point of view, while the non-serotonergic neurons form groups with considerable functional specificity. The data support the idea that the NRP and NRO are implicated in the control of somatic motor output.


Subject(s)
Neurons/physiology , Raphe Nuclei/physiology , Serotonin/metabolism , Animals , Cats , Drinking Behavior/physiology , Electrodes, Implanted , Electromyography , Female , Male , Neurons/cytology , Raphe Nuclei/cytology , Reflex, Startle/physiology
11.
Braz J Med Biol Res ; 36(2): 247-61, 2003 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12563528

ABSTRACT

The early facilitatory effect of a peripheral spatially visual prime stimulus described in the literature for simple reaction time tasks has been usually smaller than that described for complex (go/no-go, choice) reaction time tasks. In the present study we investigated the reason for this difference. In a first and a second experiment we tested the participants in both a simple task and a go/no-go task, half of them beginning with one of these tasks and half with the other one. We observed that the prime stimulus had an early effect, inhibitory for the simple task and facilitatory for the go/no-go task, when the task was performed first. No early effect appeared when the task was performed second. In a third and a fourth experiment the participants were, respectively, tested in the simple task and in the go/no-go task for four sessions (the prime stimulus was presented in the second, third and fourth sessions). The early effects of the prime stimulus did not change across the sessions, suggesting that a habituatory process was not the cause for the disappearance of these effects in the first two experiments. Our findings are compatible with the idea that different attentional strategies are adopted in simple and complex reaction time tasks. In the former tasks the gain of automatic attention mechanisms may be adjusted to a low level and in the latter tasks, to a high level. The attentional influence of the prime stimulus may be antagonized by another influence, possibly a masking one.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Perceptual Masking/physiology , Photic Stimulation/methods , Reaction Time , Analysis of Variance , Cues , Extinction, Psychological , Female , Humans , Male
12.
Braz J Med Biol Res ; 31(10): 1313-8, 1998 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9876303

ABSTRACT

It is well known that saccadic reaction times (SRT) are reduced when the target is preceded by the offset of the fixation point (FP)--the gap effect. Some authors have proposed that the FP offset also allows the saccadic system to generate a separate population of SRT, the express saccades. Nevertheless, there is no agreement as to whether the gap effect and express responses are also present for manual reaction times (MRT). We tested the gap effect and the MRT distribution in two different conditions, i.e., simple and choice MRT. In the choice MRT condition, subjects need to identify the side of the stimulus and to select the appropriate response, while in the simple MRT these stages are not necessary. We report that the gap effect was present in both conditions (22 ms for choice MRT condition; 15 ms for simple MRT condition), but, when analyzing the MRT distributions, we did not find any clear evidence for express manual responses. The main difference in MRT distribution between simple and choice conditions was a shift towards shorter values for simple MRT.


Subject(s)
Reaction Time , Saccades , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation
13.
Braz J Med Biol Res ; 34(6): 803-13, 2001 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11378672

ABSTRACT

We investigated the dependency of the early facilitatory effect of a prime stimulus (S1) on the physical characteristics of the target stimulus (S2). A go-no go reaction time paradigm was used. The S1 was a gray ring and the S2s were a white vertical line, a white horizontal line, a white cross and a white small ring, all inside a white ring with the same dimensions as the S1. S1 onset-S2 onset asynchrony was 100 ms. The stimuli appeared randomly in any one of the quadrants of a monitor screen. The S2 could occur at the same position as the S1 or at a different one. We observed a strong facilitatory effect when the vertical line or the horizontal line was the go stimulus and no effect when the cross was the go stimulus. These results show that the features of the target stimulus can be decisive for the appearance of the facilitatory effect of a peripheral spatially noninformative prime stimulus.


Subject(s)
Attention , Photic Stimulation , Reaction Time , Vision, Ocular , Adult , Female , Fixation, Ocular , Humans , Male , Perceptual Masking
14.
Braz J Med Biol Res ; 37(7): 1063-9, 2004 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15264014

ABSTRACT

Previous studies have shown that saccadic eye responses but not manual responses were sensitive to the kind of warning signal used, with visual onsets producing longer saccadic latencies compared to visual offsets. The aim of the present study was to determine the effects of distinct warning signals on manual latencies and to test the premise that the onset interference, in fact, does not occur for manual responses. A second objective was to determine if the magnitude of the warning effects could be modulated by contextual procedures. Three experimental conditions based on the kind of warning signal used (visual onset, visual offset and auditory warning) were run in two different contexts (blocked and non-blocked). Eighteen participants were asked to respond to the imperative stimulus that would occur some milliseconds (0, 250, 500 or 750 ms) after the warning signal. The experiment consisted in three experimental sessions of 240 trials, where all the variables were counterbalanced. The data showed that visual onsets produced longer manual latencies than visual offsets in the non-blocked context (275 vs 261 ms; P < 0.001). This interference was obtained, however, only for short intervals between the warning and the stimulus, and was abolished when the blocked context was used (256 vs 255 ms; P = 0.789). These results are discussed in terms of bottom-up and top-down interactions, mainly those related to the role of attentional processing in cancelling out competitive interactions and suppressive influences of a distractor on the relevant stimulus.


Subject(s)
Fixation, Ocular/physiology , Photic Stimulation/methods , Reaction Time/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Cues , Humans , Saccades/physiology
15.
Braz J Med Biol Res ; 47(1): 80-9, 2014 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24345871

ABSTRACT

There is evidence that the left hemisphere is more competent for motor control than the right hemisphere. This study investigated whether this hemispheric asymmetry is expressed in the latency/duration of sequential responses performed by the left and/or right hands. Thirty-two right-handed young adults (16 males, 16 females; 18-25 years old) were tested in a simple or choice reaction time task. They responded to a left and/or right visual target by moving their left and/or right middle fingers between two keys on each side of the midline. Right hand reaction time did not differ from left hand reaction time. Submovement times were longer for the right hand than the left hand when the response was bilateral. Pause times were shorter for the right hand than the left hand, both when the responses were unilateral or bilateral. Reaction time results indicate that the putatively more efficient response preparation by the left hemisphere motor mechanisms is not expressed behaviorally. Submovement time and pause time results indicate that the putatively more efficient response execution by the left hemisphere motor mechanisms is expressed behaviorally. In the case of the submovements, the less efficient motor control of the left hand would be compensated by a more intense attention to this hand.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Functional Laterality/physiology , Hand/physiology , Orientation/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Young Adult
16.
Braz J Med Biol Res ; 45(11): 1037-44, 2012 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22930411

ABSTRACT

The occurrence of a weak auditory warning stimulus increases the speed of the response to a subsequent visual target stimulus that must be identified. This facilitatory effect has been attributed to the temporal expectancy automatically induced by the warning stimulus. It has not been determined whether this results from a modulation of the stimulus identification process, the response selection process or both. The present study examined these possibilities. A group of 12 young adults performed a reaction time location identification task and another group of 12 young adults performed a reaction time shape identification task. A visual target stimulus was presented 1850 to 2350 ms plus a fixed interval (50, 100, 200, 400, 800, or 1600 ms, depending on the block) after the appearance of a fixation point, on its left or right side, above or below a virtual horizontal line passing through it. In half of the trials, a weak auditory warning stimulus (S1) appeared 50, 100, 200, 400, 800, or 1600 ms (according to the block) before the target stimulus (S2). Twelve trials were run for each condition. The S1 produced a facilitatory effect for the 200, 400, 800, and 1600 ms stimulus onset asynchronies (SOA) in the case of the side stimulus-response (S-R) corresponding condition, and for the 100 and 400 ms SOA in the case of the side S-R non-corresponding condition. Since these two conditions differ mainly by their response selection requirements, it is reasonable to conclude that automatic temporal expectancy influences the response selection process.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Photic Stimulation/methods , Reaction Time/physiology , Acoustic Stimulation/methods , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Young Adult
17.
Braz J Med Biol Res ; 45(5): 425-35, 2012 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22473319

ABSTRACT

This study investigated the influence of cueing on the performance of untrained and trained complex motor responses. Healthy adults responded to a visual target by performing four sequential movements (complex response) or a single movement (simple response) of their middle finger. A visual cue preceded the target by an interval of 300, 1000, or 2000 ms. In Experiment 1, the complex and simple responses were not previously trained. During the testing session, the complex response pattern varied on a trial-by-trial basis following the indication provided by the visual cue. In Experiment 2, the complex response and the simple response were extensively trained beforehand. During the testing session, the trained complex response pattern was performed in all trials. The latency of the untrained and trained complex responses decreased from the short to the medium and long cue-target intervals. The latency of the complex response was longer than that of the simple response, except in the case of the trained responses and the long cue-target interval. These results suggest that the preparation of untrained complex responses cannot be completed in advance, this being possible, however, for trained complex responses when enough time is available. The duration of the 1st submovement, 1st pause and 2nd submovement of the untrained and the trained complex responses increased from the short to the long cue-target interval, suggesting that there is an increase of online programming of the response possibly related to the degree of certainty about the moment of target appearance.


Subject(s)
Conditioning, Psychological/physiology , Cues , Movement/physiology , Photic Stimulation/methods , Reaction Time/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Young Adult
18.
Braz J Med Biol Res ; 43(8): 745-58, 2010 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20658095

ABSTRACT

We recently demonstrated that automatic attention favors the right side of space and, in the present study, we investigated whether voluntary attention also favors this side. Six reaction time experiments were conducted. In each experiment, 12 new 18-25-year-old male right-handed individuals were tested. In Experiments 1, 2, 3 (a, b) and 4 (a, b), tasks with increasing attentional demands were used. In Experiments 1, 2, 3a, and 4a, attention was oriented to one or both sides by means of a central spatially informative visual cue. A left or right side visual target appeared 100, 300, or 500 ms later. Attentional effects were observed in the four experiments. In Experiments 2, 3a and 4a, these effects were greater when the cue indicated the right side than when it indicated the left side (respectively: 16 +/- 10 and 44 +/- 6 ms, P = 0.015, for stimulus onset asynchrony of 500 ms in Experiment 2; 38 +/- 10 and 70 +/- 7 ms, P = 0.011, for Experiment 3a, and 23 +/- 11 and 61 +/- 10 ms, P = 0.009, for Experiment 4a). In Experiments 3b and 4b, the central cue pointed to both sides and was said to be non-relevant for task performance. In these experiments right and left reaction times did not differ. The most conservative interpretation of the present findings is that voluntary attention orienting favors the right side of space, particularly when a difficult task has to be performed.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Functional Laterality/physiology , Orientation/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation , Young Adult
20.
Braz. j. med. biol. res ; 47(1): 80-89, 01/2014. tab, graf
Article in English | LILACS | ID: lil-697669

ABSTRACT

There is evidence that the left hemisphere is more competent for motor control than the right hemisphere. This study investigated whether this hemispheric asymmetry is expressed in the latency/duration of sequential responses performed by the left and/or right hands. Thirty-two right-handed young adults (16 males, 16 females; 18-25 years old) were tested in a simple or choice reaction time task. They responded to a left and/or right visual target by moving their left and/or right middle fingers between two keys on each side of the midline. Right hand reaction time did not differ from left hand reaction time. Submovement times were longer for the right hand than the left hand when the response was bilateral. Pause times were shorter for the right hand than the left hand, both when the responses were unilateral or bilateral. Reaction time results indicate that the putatively more efficient response preparation by the left hemisphere motor mechanisms is not expressed behaviorally. Submovement time and pause time results indicate that the putatively more efficient response execution by the left hemisphere motor mechanisms is expressed behaviorally. In the case of the submovements, the less efficient motor control of the left hand would be compensated by a more intense attention to this hand.


Subject(s)
Adolescent , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Young Adult , Attention/physiology , Functional Laterality/physiology , Hand/physiology , Orientation/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL