Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 40
Filter
1.
Evol Anthropol ; 31(4): 175-198, 2022 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35485603

ABSTRACT

We present evidence that people in small-scale mobile hunter-gatherer societies cooperated in large numbers to produce collective goods. Foragers engaged in large-scale communal hunts and constructed shared capital facilities; they made shared investments in improving the local environment; and they participated in warfare, formed enduring alliances, and established trading networks. Large-scale collective action often played a crucial role in subsistence. The provision of public goods involved the cooperation of many individuals, so each person made only a small contribution. This evidence suggests that large-scale cooperation occurred in the Pleistocene societies that encompass most of human evolutionary history, and therefore it is unlikely that large-scale cooperation in Holocene food producing societies results from an evolved psychology shaped only in small-group interactions. Instead, large-scale human cooperation needs to be explained as an adaptation, likely rooted in distinctive features of human biology, grammatical language, increased cognitive ability, and cumulative cultural adaptation.


Subject(s)
Biological Evolution , Cooperative Behavior , Social Behavior/history , Cognition , History, Ancient , Humans , Societies , Warfare
2.
Behav Brain Sci ; 45: e172, 2022 09 13.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36098443

ABSTRACT

In theory, observed correlations between genetic information and behaviour might be useful to members of the WEIRD (western, educated, industrialized, rich, and democratic) populations. Guiding young people to choose educational opportunities that best match their abilities would benefit both the individual and society. In practice, however, such choices are far more profoundly limited by the culture people have inherited than their genes.


Subject(s)
Culture , Adolescent , Genetics , Humans
3.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 114(23): 6068-6073, 2017 06 06.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28533363

ABSTRACT

Human behavior is strongly affected by culturally transmitted norms and values. Certain norms are internalized (i.e., acting according to a norm becomes an end in itself rather than merely a tool in achieving certain goals or avoiding social sanctions). Humans' capacity to internalize norms likely evolved in our ancestors to simplify solving certain challenges-including social ones. Here we study theoretically the evolutionary origins of the capacity to internalize norms. In our models, individuals can choose to participate in collective actions as well as punish free riders. In making their decisions, individuals attempt to maximize a utility function in which normative values are initially irrelevant but play an increasingly important role if the ability to internalize norms emerges. Using agent-based simulations, we show that norm internalization evolves under a wide range of conditions so that cooperation becomes "instinctive." Norm internalization evolves much more easily and has much larger effects on behavior if groups promote peer punishment of free riders. Promoting only participation in collective actions is not effective. Typically, intermediate levels of norm internalization are most frequent but there are also cases with relatively small frequencies of "oversocialized" individuals willing to make extreme sacrifices for their groups no matter material costs, as well as "undersocialized" individuals completely immune to social norms. Evolving the ability to internalize norms was likely a crucial step on the path to large-scale human cooperation.


Subject(s)
Cooperative Behavior , Cultural Evolution , Social Norms , Altruism , Computer Simulation , Ethnic Violence/psychology , Ethnic Violence/statistics & numerical data , Humans , Interpersonal Relations , Models, Psychological , Punishment , Social Behavior
4.
Behav Brain Sci ; 43: e171, 2020 08 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32772971

ABSTRACT

What promised to be a refreshing addition to cumulative cultural evolution, by moving the focus from cultural transmission to technological innovation, falls flat through a lack of thoroughness, explanatory power, and data. A comprehensive theory of cumulative cultural change must carefully integrate all existing evidence in a cohesive multi-level account. We argue that the manuscript fails to do so convincingly.


Subject(s)
Cultural Evolution , Humans , Inventions
5.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 111(17): 6159-64, 2014 Apr 29.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24753572

ABSTRACT

The domestication of plants and animals marks one of the most significant transitions in human, and indeed global, history. Traditionally, study of the domestication process was the exclusive domain of archaeologists and agricultural scientists; today it is an increasingly multidisciplinary enterprise that has come to involve the skills of evolutionary biologists and geneticists. Although the application of new information sources and methodologies has dramatically transformed our ability to study and understand domestication, it has also generated increasingly large and complex datasets, the interpretation of which is not straightforward. In particular, challenges of equifinality, evolutionary variance, and emergence of unexpected or counter-intuitive patterns all face researchers attempting to infer past processes directly from patterns in data. We argue that explicit modeling approaches, drawing upon emerging methodologies in statistics and population genetics, provide a powerful means of addressing these limitations. Modeling also offers an approach to analyzing datasets that avoids conclusions steered by implicit biases, and makes possible the formal integration of different data types. Here we outline some of the modeling approaches most relevant to current problems in domestication research, and demonstrate the ways in which simulation modeling is beginning to reshape our understanding of the domestication process.


Subject(s)
Animals, Domestic/growth & development , Crops, Agricultural/growth & development , Narration , Animals , Humans , Hybridization, Genetic , Models, Biological
6.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 111(17): 6139-46, 2014 Apr 29.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24757054

ABSTRACT

It is difficult to overstate the cultural and biological impacts that the domestication of plants and animals has had on our species. Fundamental questions regarding where, when, and how many times domestication took place have been of primary interest within a wide range of academic disciplines. Within the last two decades, the advent of new archaeological and genetic techniques has revolutionized our understanding of the pattern and process of domestication and agricultural origins that led to our modern way of life. In the spring of 2011, 25 scholars with a central interest in domestication representing the fields of genetics, archaeobotany, zooarchaeology, geoarchaeology, and archaeology met at the National Evolutionary Synthesis Center to discuss recent domestication research progress and identify challenges for the future. In this introduction to the resulting Special Feature, we present the state of the art in the field by discussing what is known about the spatial and temporal patterns of domestication, and controversies surrounding the speed, intentionality, and evolutionary aspects of the domestication process. We then highlight three key challenges for future research. We conclude by arguing that although recent progress has been impressive, the next decade will yield even more substantial insights not only into how domestication took place, but also when and where it did, and where and why it did not.


Subject(s)
Animals, Domestic/genetics , Crops, Agricultural/genetics , Animals , Biological Evolution , Environment , Geography , Spatio-Temporal Analysis
7.
Nature ; 463(7283): E8-9; discussion E9-10, 2010 Feb 18.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20164866

ABSTRACT

Wild et al. argue that the evolution of reduced virulence can be understood from the perspective of inclusive fitness, obviating the need to evoke group selection as a contributing causal factor. Although they acknowledge the mathematical equivalence of the inclusive fitness and multilevel selection approaches, they conclude that reduced virulence can be viewed entirely as an individual-level adaptation by the parasite. Here we show that their model is a well-known special case of the more general theory of multilevel selection, and that the cause of reduced virulence resides in the opposition of two processes: within-group and among-group selection. This distinction is important in light of the current controversy among evolutionary biologists in which some continue to affirm that natural selection centres only and always at the level of the individual organism or gene, despite mathematical demonstrations that evolutionary dynamics must be described by selection at various levels in the hierarchy of biological organization.


Subject(s)
Genetic Fitness/physiology , Models, Biological , Parasites/genetics , Parasites/pathogenicity , Selection, Genetic/physiology , Animals , Virulence/genetics , Virulence/physiology
8.
Proc Biol Sci ; 281(1776): 20132864, 2014 Feb 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24352950

ABSTRACT

Copying others appears to be a cost-effective way of obtaining adaptive information, particularly when flexibly employed. However, adult humans differ considerably in their propensity to use information from others, even when this 'social information' is beneficial, raising the possibility that stable individual differences constrain flexibility in social information use. We used two dissimilar decision-making computer games to investigate whether individuals flexibly adjusted their use of social information to current conditions or whether they valued social information similarly in both games. Participants also completed established personality questionnaires. We found that participants demonstrated considerable flexibility, adjusting social information use to current conditions. In particular, individuals employed a 'copy-when-uncertain' social learning strategy, supporting a core, but untested, assumption of influential theoretical models of cultural transmission. Moreover, participants adjusted the amount invested in their decision based on the perceived reliability of personally gathered information combined with the available social information. However, despite this strategic flexibility, participants also exhibited consistent individual differences in their propensities to use and value social information. Moreover, individuals who favoured social information self-reported as more collectivist than others. We discuss the implications of our results for social information use and cultural transmission.


Subject(s)
Decision Making/physiology , Imitative Behavior , Individuality , Personality , Adult , California , Female , Games, Experimental , Humans , Information Dissemination , Male , Surveys and Questionnaires
9.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 108 Suppl 2: 10918-25, 2011 Jun 28.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21690340

ABSTRACT

In the last 60,000 y humans have expanded across the globe and now occupy a wider range than any other terrestrial species. Our ability to successfully adapt to such a diverse range of habitats is often explained in terms of our cognitive ability. Humans have relatively bigger brains and more computing power than other animals, and this allows us to figure out how to live in a wide range of environments. Here we argue that humans may be smarter than other creatures, but none of us is nearly smart enough to acquire all of the information necessary to survive in any single habitat. In even the simplest foraging societies, people depend on a vast array of tools, detailed bodies of local knowledge, and complex social arrangements and often do not understand why these tools, beliefs, and behaviors are adaptive. We owe our success to our uniquely developed ability to learn from others. This capacity enables humans to gradually accumulate information across generations and develop well-adapted tools, beliefs, and practices that are too complex for any single individual to invent during their lifetime.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Psychological , Cultural Evolution , Learning , Animals , Environment , Humans
11.
Behav Brain Sci ; 37(3): 280-1, 2014 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24970427

ABSTRACT

Smaldino makes a solid contribution to the literature on the evolution of human social organization by pointing out that group-level-traits (GLTs) often emerge from the interactions of group members in such a way that their effects are not easily partitioned into individual selection. However, we argue that he too readily dismisses institutional analysis as a tool for understanding these traits.


Subject(s)
Cooperative Behavior , Cultural Evolution , Group Processes , Selection, Genetic , Humans
12.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 107 Suppl 2: 8985-92, 2010 May 11.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20445092

ABSTRACT

The use of socially learned information (culture) is central to human adaptations. We investigate the hypothesis that the process of cultural evolution has played an active, leading role in the evolution of genes. Culture normally evolves more rapidly than genes, creating novel environments that expose genes to new selective pressures. Many human genes that have been shown to be under recent or current selection are changing as a result of new environments created by cultural innovations. Some changed in response to the development of agricultural subsistence systems in the Early and Middle Holocene. Alleles coding for adaptations to diets rich in plant starch (e.g., amylase copy number) and to epidemic diseases evolved as human populations expanded (e.g., sickle cell and G6PD deficiency alleles that provide protection against malaria). Large-scale scans using patterns of linkage disequilibrium to detect recent selection suggest that many more genes evolved in response to agriculture. Genetic change in response to the novel social environment of contemporary modern societies is also likely to be occurring. The functional effects of most of the alleles under selection during the last 10,000 years are currently unknown. Also unknown is the role of paleoenvironmental change in regulating the tempo of hominin evolution. Although the full extent of culture-driven gene-culture coevolution is thus far unknown for the deeper history of the human lineage, theory and some evidence suggest that such effects were profound. Genomic methods promise to have a major impact on our understanding of gene-culture coevolution over the span of hominin evolutionary history.


Subject(s)
Biological Evolution , Genomics , Alleles , Animals , Cultural Characteristics , Environment , Fossils , Hominidae , Humans , Linkage Disequilibrium , Models, Genetic
13.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 106(42): 17671-4, 2009 Oct 20.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19822753

ABSTRACT

Whether competition among large groups played an important role in human social evolution is dependent on how variation, whether cultural or genetic, is maintained between groups. Comparisons between genetic and cultural differentiation between neighboring groups show how natural selection on large groups is more plausible on cultural rather than genetic variation.


Subject(s)
Biological Evolution , Culture , Social Behavior , Altruism , Ethnicity/genetics , Ethnicity/psychology , Ethnopsychology , Humans , Models, Genetic , Models, Psychological , Paleontology , Selection, Genetic
14.
Behav Brain Sci ; 35(1): 20-1, 2012 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22289309

ABSTRACT

Experiments are not models of cooperation; instead, they demonstrate the presence of the ethical and other-regarding predispositions that often motivate cooperation and the punishment of free-riders. Experimental behavior predicts subjects' cooperation in the field. Ethnographic studies in small-scale societies without formal coercive institutions demonstrate that disciplining defectors is both essential to cooperation and often costly to the punisher.


Subject(s)
Cooperative Behavior , Game Theory , Models, Psychological , Punishment/psychology , Social Behavior , Humans
15.
Evol Hum Sci ; 4: e51, 2022.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37588907

ABSTRACT

Human decision-making is controlled by various factors including material cost-benefit considerations, values and beliefs, social influences, cognitive factors and errors. Among social influences, those by external authorities (e.g. educational, cultural, religious, political, administrative, etc.) are particularly important owing to their potential reach and power. To better understand the effects of 'soft' power of authorities we develop a unifying theoretical framework integrating material, cognitive and social forces controlling the joint dynamics of individual actions and beliefs. We apply our approach to three different phenomena: evolution of food sharing in small-scale societies, participation in political protests and effects of priming social identity in behavioural experiments. For each of these applications, we show that our approach leads to different (or simpler) explanations of human behaviour than alternatives. We highlight the type of measurements which can be helpful in developing practical applications of our approach. We identify and explicitly characterise the degree of mismatch between individual actions and attitudes. We assert that the effects of external authorities, of changing beliefs and of differences between people must be studied empirically, included in mathematical models, and accounted for when developing different policies aiming to modify or sustain human behaviour.

16.
Science ; 372(6544)2021 05 21.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34016754

ABSTRACT

Charles Darwin's The Descent of Man, published 150 years ago, laid the grounds for scientific studies into human origins and evolution. Three of his insights have been reinforced by modern science. The first is that we share many characteristics (genetic, developmental, physiological, morphological, cognitive, and psychological) with our closest relatives, the anthropoid apes. The second is that humans have a talent for high-level cooperation reinforced by morality and social norms. The third is that we have greatly expanded the social learning capacity that we see already in other primates. Darwin's emphasis on the role of culture deserves special attention because during an increasingly unstable Pleistocene environment, cultural accumulation allowed changes in life history; increased cognition; and the appearance of language, social norms, and institutions.


Subject(s)
Biological Evolution , Cultural Evolution , Social Evolution , Animals , Brain/anatomy & histology , Brain/growth & development , Gorilla gorilla , Humans , Organ Size , Primates/anatomy & histology , Primates/growth & development , Primates/physiology , Social Norms
19.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 375(1803): 20190498, 2020 07 20.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32475331

ABSTRACT

Humans evolved from an ape ancestor that was highly intelligent, moderately social and moderately dependent on cultural adaptations for subsistence technology (tools). By the late Pleistocene, humans had become highly dependent on culture for subsistence and for rules to organize a complex social life. Adaptation by cultural traditions transformed our life history, leading to an extended juvenile period to learn subsistence and social skills, post-reproductive survival to help conserve and transmit skills, a dependence on social support for mothers of large-brained, very dependent and nutrient-demanding offspring, males devoting substantial effort to provisioning rather than mating, and the cultivation of large social networks to tap pools in information unavailable to less social species. One measure of the success of the exploitation of culture is that the minimum inter-birth interval of humans is nearly half that of our ape relatives. Another measure is the wide geographical distribution of humans compared with other apes, based on subsistence systems adapted to fine-scale spatial environmental variation. An important macro-evolutionary question is why our big-brained, culture-intensive life-history strategy evolved so recently and in only our lineage. We suggest that increasing spatial and temporal variation in the Pleistocene favoured cultural adaptations. This article is part of the theme issue 'Life history and learning: how childhood, caregiving and old age shape cognition and culture in humans and other animals'.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Biological , Biological Evolution , Cultural Evolution , Hominidae/psychology , Life History Traits , Animals , Cognition , Humans , Learning , Social Behavior
20.
J Theor Biol ; 257(2): 331-9, 2009 Mar 21.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19135062

ABSTRACT

Human migration is nonrandom. In small scale societies of the past, and in the modern world, people tend to move to wealthier, safer, and more just societies from poorer, more violent, less just societies. If immigrants are assimilated, such nonrandom migration can increase the occurrence of culturally transmitted beliefs, values, and institutions that cause societies to be attractive to immigrants. Here we describe and analyze a simple model of this process. This model suggests that long run outcomes depend on the relative strength of migration and local adaptation. When local adaption is strong enough to preserve cultural variation among groups, cultural variants that make societies attractive always predominate, but never drive alternative variants to extinction. When migration predominates, outcomes depend both on the relative attractiveness of alternative variants and on the initial sizes of societies that provide and receive immigrants.


Subject(s)
Altruism , Biological Evolution , Emigration and Immigration , Anthropology , Culture , Humans , Models, Biological , Population Dynamics , Socioeconomic Factors
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL