Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 8 de 8
Filter
2.
Laterality ; 21(1): 12-33, 2016.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26314871

ABSTRACT

The maternal preference to hold infants on the left rather than right side of the body was examined longitudinally, with attention to 4 explanations: maternal monitoring of infant state, maternal handedness, infant proximity to the mother's heartbeat, and preferred infant head position. The side and site of holding were measured over the first 12 weeks of the lives of 24 infants. Information about group and individual consistency in holding side allowed novel evaluation of the theories. A strong bias to hold on the left dropped below significance when the infants were aged 12 weeks and was limited to specific holding positions. Findings were generally consistent with the monitoring hypothesis, and little support was found for the 3 alternative explanations.


Subject(s)
Functional Laterality/physiology , Infant Behavior , Maternal Behavior/physiology , Mother-Child Relations , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Bias , Female , Humans , Infant , Infant Behavior/psychology , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Time Factors
3.
Sci Rep ; 10(1): 17385, 2020 10 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33060674

ABSTRACT

Evolution has endowed vertebrates with a divided brain that allows for processing of critical survival behaviours in parallel. Most humans possess a standard functional brain organisation for these ancient sensory-motor behaviours, favouring the right hemisphere for fight-or-flight processes and the left hemisphere for performing structured motor sequences. However, a significant minority of the population possess an organisational phenotype that represents crowding of function in one hemisphere, or a reversal of the standard functional organisation. Using behavioural biases as a proxy for brain organisation, results indicate that reversed brain organisation phenotype increases in populations with autism and is associated with weaker cognitive abilities. Moreover, this study revealed that left-handedness, alone, is not associated with decreased cognitive ability or autism. Rather, left-handedness acts as a marker for decreased cognitive performance when paired with the reversed brain phenotype. The results contribute to comparative research suggesting that modern human abilities are supported by evolutionarily old, lateralised sensory-motor processes. Systematic, longitudinal investigations, capturing genetic measures and brain correlates, are essential to reveal how cognition emerges from these foundational processes. Importantly, strength and direction of biases can act as early markers of brain organisation and cognitive development, leading to promising, novel practices for diagnoses and interventions.


Subject(s)
Autistic Disorder/physiopathology , Biological Evolution , Cognition , Psychomotor Performance , Autistic Disorder/psychology , Brain/physiopathology , Case-Control Studies , Child , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Infant , Male
4.
Cortex ; 118: 116-131, 2019 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29961539

ABSTRACT

A robust left side cradling bias (LCB) in humans is argued to reflect an evolutionarily old left visual field bias and right hemisphere dominance for processing social stimuli. A left visual field bias for face processing, invoked via the LCB, is known to reflect a human population-level right cerebral hemisphere specialization for processing social stimuli. We explored the relationship between cradling side biases, hand dominance and socio-communicative abilities. Four and five year old typically-developing children (N = 98) participated in a battery of manual motor tasks interspersed by cradling trials comprising a(n): infant human doll, infant primate doll, proto-face pillow and no-face pillow. Mean social and communication ability scores were obtained via a survey completed by each child's key teacher. We found a population-level LCB for holding an infant human doll that was not influenced by hand dominance, sex, age or experience of having a younger sibling. Children demonstrating a LCB, did however, obtain a significantly higher mean social ability score compared with their right side cradling counterparts. Like the infant human doll, the proto-face pillow's schematic face symbol was sufficient to elicit a population-level LCB. By contrast, the infant primate doll elicited a population-level right side cradling bias, influenced by both hand dominance and sex. The findings suggest that the LCB is present and visible early in development and is likely therefore, to represent evolutionarily old domain-specific organization and function of the right cerebral hemisphere. Additionally, results suggest that a LCB requires minimal triggering but can be reversed in some situations, possibly in response to species-type or levels of novelty or stress as perceived by the viewer. Patterns of behavioral biases within the context of social stimuli and their associations with cognitive ability are important for understanding how socio-communication abilities emerge in developing children.


Subject(s)
Cognition/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Social Behavior , Visual Perception/physiology , Bias , Biological Evolution , Cerebrum/physiology , Child , Female , Functional Laterality/physiology , Humans , Infant , Male
5.
Prog Brain Res ; 238: 377-403, 2018.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30097201

ABSTRACT

Cerebral lateralization and associated motor behaviors were historically thought to be characteristics unique to humans. Today, it is clear that these features are present and visible in other animal species. These shared attributes of brain and behavior suggest inheritance from a distant common ancestor. Population-level motor biases are likely to reflect an early evolutionary division of primary survival functions of the brain's left and right hemispheres. In modern humans, these features may provide a foundational platform for the development of higher cognitive functions, inextricably cementing the ties between the evolution and development of cognition. This chapter focuses on the links between a vertebrate-wide right hemisphere dominance for perceiving and producing social signals, left side motor biases (inclusive of visual field preferences), and the evolution and development of cognition in modern humans.


Subject(s)
Biological Evolution , Functional Laterality/physiology , Social Behavior , Animals , Brain/physiology , Humans
6.
Sci Rep ; 7(1): 1731, 2017 05 11.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28496110

ABSTRACT

This systematic review and meta-analysis was designed to investigate whether maternal psychological stress and recent life events are associated with an increased risk of miscarriage. A literature search was conducted to identify studies reporting miscarriage in women with and without history of exposure to psychological stress (the only exposure considered). The search produced 1978 studies; 8 studies were suitable for analysis. A meta-analysis was performed using a random-effects model with effect sizes weighted by the sampling variance. The risk of miscarriage was significantly higher in women with a history of exposure to psychological stress (OR 1.42, 95% CI 1.19-1.70). These findings remained after controlling for study type (cohort and nested case-control study OR 1.33 95% CI 1.14-1.54), exposure types (work stress OR 1.27, 95% CI 1.10-1.47), types of controls included (live birth OR 2.82 95% CI: 1.64-4.86). We found no evidence that publication bias or study heterogeneity significantly influenced the results. Our finding provides the most robust evidence to date, that prior psychological stress is harmful to women in early pregnancy.


Subject(s)
Abortion, Spontaneous/etiology , Abortion, Spontaneous/psychology , Stress, Psychological/complications , Confidence Intervals , Female , Humans , Odds Ratio , Pregnancy , Publication Bias
7.
Front Psychol ; 5: 879, 2014.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25161640

ABSTRACT

Despite a growing body of work examining the expression of infants' positive emotion in joint attention contexts, few studies have examined the moment-by-moment dynamics of emotional signaling by adults interacting with babies in these contexts. We invited 73 parents of infants (three fathers) to our laboratory, comprising parent-infant dyads with babies at 6 (n = 15), 9 (n = 15), 12 (n = 15), 15 (n = 14), and 18 (n = 14) months of age. Parents were asked to sit in a chair centered on the long axis of a room and to point to distant dolls (2.5 m) when the dolls were animated, while holding their children in their laps. We found that parents displayed the highest levels of smiling at the same time that they pointed, thus demonstrating affective/referential synchrony in their infant-directed communication. There were no discernable differences in this pattern among parents with children of different ages. Thus, parents spontaneously encapsulated episodes of joint attention with positive emotion.

8.
Dev Sci ; 7(1): 19-24, 2004 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15323114

ABSTRACT

Previous research has indicated that 70-85% of women and girls show a bias to hold infants, or dolls, to the left side of their body. This bias is not matched in males (e.g. deChateau, Holmberg & Winberg, 1978; Todd, 1995). This study tests an explanation of cradling preferences in terms of hemispheric specialization for the perception of facial emotional expression. Thirty-two right-handed participants were given a behavioural test of lateralization and a cradling task. Females, but not males, who cradled a doll on the left side were found to have significantly higher laterality quotients than right cradlers. Results indicate that women cradle on the side of the body that is contralateral to the hemisphere dominant for face and emotion processing and suggest a possible explanation of gender differences in the incidence of cradling.


Subject(s)
Choice Behavior/physiology , Functional Laterality/physiology , Maternal Behavior/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Chi-Square Distribution , Emotions/physiology , Facial Expression , Female , Humans , Male , Sex Factors
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL