Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 26
Filter
Add more filters

Publication year range
1.
J Neurol Phys Ther ; 41(3): 164-172, 2017 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28628550

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND AND PURPOSE: Spontaneous visual recovery is rare after cortical blindness. While visual rehabilitation may improve performance, no visual therapy has been widely adopted, as clinical outcomes are variable and rarely translate into improvements in activities of daily living (ADLs). We explored the potential value of a novel rehabilitation approach "cognitive therapeutic exercises" for cortical blindness. CASE DESCRIPTION: The subject of this case study was 48-year-old woman with cortical blindness and tetraplegia after cardiac arrest. Prior to the intervention, she was dependent in ADLs and poorly distinguished shapes and colors after 19 months of standard visual and motor rehabilitation. Computed tomographic images soon after symptom onset demonstrated acute infarcts in both occipital cortices. INTERVENTION: The subject underwent 8 months of intensive rehabilitation with "cognitive therapeutic exercises" consisting of discrimination exercises correlating sensory and visual information. OUTCOMES: Visual fields increased; object recognition improved; it became possible to watch television; voluntary arm movements improved in accuracy and smoothness; walking improved; and ADL independence and self-reliance increased. Subtraction of neuroimaging acquired before and after rehabilitation showed that focal glucose metabolism increases bilaterally in the occipital poles. DISCUSSION: This study demonstrates feasibility of "cognitive therapeutic exercises" in an individual with cortical blindness, who experienced impressive visual and sensorimotor recovery, with marked ADL improvement, more than 2 years after ischemic cortical damage.Video Abstract available for additional insights from the authors (see Video, Supplemental Digital Content 1, available at: http://links.lww.com/JNPT/A173).


Subject(s)
Blindness, Cortical/psychology , Blindness, Cortical/rehabilitation , Cognitive Behavioral Therapy , Exercise Therapy , Activities of Daily Living , Blindness, Cortical/physiopathology , Female , Humans , Middle Aged , Recovery of Function , Vision, Ocular , Walking
2.
Psychogeriatrics ; 15(1): 58-61, 2015 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25515048

ABSTRACT

Anton-Babinski syndrome is a rare disease featuring bilateral cortical blindness and anosognosia with visual confabulation, but without dementia or any memory impairment. It has a unique neuropsychiatric presentation and should be highly suspected in those with odd visual loss and imaging evidence of occipital lobe injury. In the case discussed herein, a 90-year-old man presented with bilateral blindness, obvious anosognosia, and vivid visual confabulation, which he had had for 3 days. Brain computed tomography demonstrated recent hypodense infarctions at the bilateral occipital lobes. Thus, the patient was diagnosed with Anton-Babinski syndrome. Because of his age and the thrombolytic therapy during the golden 3 hours after ischemic stroke, the patient received aspirin therapy rather than tissue plasminogen activator or warfarin. He gradually realized he was blind during the following week, but died of pneumonia 1 month later. In the literature, it is difficult to establish awareness of blindness in patients with Anton-Babinski syndrome, but optimistically, in one report, a patient was aware of blindness within 2 weeks, without vision improvement. Our case illustrates that elderly patients with Anton-Babinski syndrome can partially recover and that 1 week is the shortest time for the establishment of awareness of blindness for sufferers without vision improvement.


Subject(s)
Blindness, Cortical/diagnosis , Aged, 80 and over , Awareness/physiology , Blindness/diagnosis , Blindness/physiopathology , Blindness/psychology , Blindness, Cortical/physiopathology , Blindness, Cortical/psychology , Cerebral Infarction/diagnosis , Cerebral Infarction/physiopathology , Cerebral Infarction/psychology , Diagnosis, Differential , Dominance, Cerebral/physiology , Humans , Male , Occipital Lobe/pathology , Occipital Lobe/physiopathology , Tomography, X-Ray Computed
4.
Brain ; 135(Pt 11): 3440-52, 2012 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23169923

ABSTRACT

Brain responses (from functional magnetic resonance imaging) and components of information processing were investigated in nine cortically blind observers performing a global direction discrimination task. Three of these subjects had responses in perilesional cortex in response to blind field stimulation, whereas the others did not. We used the EZ-diffusion model of decision making to understand how cortically blind subjects make a perceptual decision on stimuli presented within their blind field. We found that these subjects had slower accumulation of information in their blind fields as compared with their good fields and to intact controls. Within cortically blind subjects, activity in perilesional tissue, V3A and hMT+ was associated with a faster accumulation of information for deciding direction of motion of stimuli presented in the blind field. This result suggests that the rate of information accumulation is a critical factor in the degree of impairment in cortical blindness and varies greatly among affected individuals. Retraining paradigms that seek to restore visual functions might benefit from focusing on increasing the rate of information accumulation.


Subject(s)
Blindness, Cortical/physiopathology , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Motion Perception/physiology , Visual Cortex/physiology , Adult , Aged , Blindness, Cortical/psychology , Case-Control Studies , Decision Making/physiology , Female , Functional Neuroimaging/methods , Functional Neuroimaging/psychology , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/psychology , Male , Middle Aged , Models, Psychological , Stroke/complications , Stroke/physiopathology , Stroke/psychology , Time Factors
5.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 106(42): 17661-6, 2009 Oct 20.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19805044

ABSTRACT

The spontaneous tendency to synchronize our facial expressions with those of others is often termed emotional contagion. It is unclear, however, whether emotional contagion depends on visual awareness of the eliciting stimulus and which processes underlie the unfolding of expressive reactions in the observer. It has been suggested either that emotional contagion is driven by motor imitation (i.e., mimicry), or that it is one observable aspect of the emotional state arising when we see the corresponding emotion in others. Emotional contagion reactions to different classes of consciously seen and "unseen" stimuli were compared by presenting pictures of facial or bodily expressions either to the intact or blind visual field of two patients with unilateral destruction of the visual cortex and ensuing phenomenal blindness. Facial reactions were recorded using electromyography, and arousal responses were measured with pupil dilatation. Passive exposure to unseen expressions evoked faster facial reactions and higher arousal compared with seen stimuli, therefore indicating that emotional contagion occurs also when the triggering stimulus cannot be consciously perceived because of cortical blindness. Furthermore, stimuli that are very different in their visual characteristics, such as facial and bodily gestures, induced highly similar expressive responses. This shows that the patients did not simply imitate the motor pattern observed in the stimuli, but resonated to their affective meaning. Emotional contagion thus represents an instance of truly affective reactions that may be mediated by visual pathways of old evolutionary origin bypassing cortical vision while still providing a cornerstone for emotion communication and affect sharing.


Subject(s)
Blindness, Cortical/physiopathology , Blindness, Cortical/psychology , Emotions/physiology , Facial Expression , Kinesics , Nonverbal Communication/physiology , Nonverbal Communication/psychology , Aged , Electromyography , Facial Muscles/physiopathology , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Photic Stimulation , Visual Cortex/injuries , Visual Cortex/physiopathology , Visual Fields , Visual Pathways/physiopathology
6.
Nat Neurosci ; 10(2): 257-61, 2007 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17237774

ABSTRACT

The lack of an accepted measure of awareness has made claims that accurate decisions can be made without awareness controversial. Here we introduce a new objective measure of awareness, post-decision wagering. We show that participants fail to maximize cash earnings by wagering high following correct decisions in blindsight, the Iowa gambling task and an artificial grammar task. This demonstrates, without the uncertainties associated with the conventional subjective measures of awareness (verbal reports and confidence ratings), that the participants were not aware that their decisions were correct. Post-decision wagering may be used to study the neural correlates of consciousness.


Subject(s)
Awareness/physiology , Consciousness/physiology , Decision Making/physiology , Gambling/psychology , Unconscious, Psychology , Adult , Blindness, Cortical/psychology , Brain/physiology , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Female , Humans , Language Tests , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Verbal Behavior/physiology
7.
Neuropsychologia ; 128: 204-208, 2019 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30102905

ABSTRACT

Blindsight has been primarily and extensively studied by Lawrence Weiskrantz. Residual visual abilities following a hemispheric lesion leading to homonymous hemianopia encompass a variety of visual-perceptual and visuo-motor functions. Attention blindsight produces the more salient subjective experiences, especially for motion (Riddoch phenomenon). Action blindsight illustrates visuo-motor abilities despite the patients' feeling that they produce random movements. Perception blindsight seems to be the weakest residual function observed in blindsight, e.g. for wavelength sensitivity. Discriminating motion produced by isoluminant colours does not give rise to blindsight for motion but the outcome of the reciprocal test is not known. Here we tested whether moving stimuli could give rise to colour discrimination in a patient with homonymous hemianopia. It was found that even though the patient exhibited nearly perfect performances for motion direction discrimination his colour discrimination for the same moving stimulus remained at chance level. It is concluded that easily discriminated moving stimuli do not give rise to colour discrimination and implications for the 3 levels of blindsight taxonomy are discussed.


Subject(s)
Blindness, Cortical/psychology , Color Perception , Hemianopsia/psychology , Motion Perception , Adult , Aged , Attention , Discrimination, Psychological , Humans , Male , Psychomotor Performance , Stroke/complications , Stroke/psychology , Visual Perception , Young Adult
8.
Neuropsychologia ; 128: 150-165, 2019 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29753019

ABSTRACT

Patients with injury to early visual cortex or its inputs can display the Riddoch phenomenon: preserved awareness for moving but not stationary stimuli. We provide a detailed case report of a patient with the Riddoch phenomenon, MC. MC has extensive bilateral lesions to occipitotemporal cortex that include most early visual cortex and complete blindness in visual field perimetry testing with static targets. Nevertheless, she shows a remarkably robust preserved ability to perceive motion, enabling her to navigate through cluttered environments and perform actions like catching moving balls. Comparisons of MC's structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) data to a probabilistic atlas based on controls reveals that MC's lesions encompass the posterior, lateral, and ventral early visual cortex bilaterally (V1, V2, V3A/B, LO1/2, TO1/2, hV4 and VO1 in both hemispheres) as well as more extensive damage to right parietal (inferior parietal lobule) and left ventral occipitotemporal cortex (VO1, PHC1/2). She shows some sparing of anterior occipital cortex, which may account for her ability to see moving targets beyond ~15 degrees eccentricity during perimetry. Most strikingly, functional and structural MRI revealed robust and reliable spared functionality of the middle temporal motion complex (MT+) bilaterally. Moreover, consistent with her preserved ability to discriminate motion direction in psychophysical testing, MC also shows direction-selective adaptation in MT+. A variety of tests did not enable us to discern whether input to MT+ was driven by her spared anterior occipital cortex or subcortical inputs. Nevertheless, MC shows rich motion perception despite profoundly impaired static and form vision, combined with clear preservation of activation in MT+, thus supporting the role of MT+ in the Riddoch phenomenon.


Subject(s)
Blindness, Cortical/diagnostic imaging , Blindness, Cortical/psychology , Motion Perception , Visual Cortex/pathology , Brain Mapping , Cerebral Infarction/pathology , Cerebral Infarction/psychology , Contrast Sensitivity , Discrimination, Psychological , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Middle Aged , Neuroimaging , Psychophysics , Visual Perception
9.
Neuropsychologia ; 128: 315-324, 2019 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29237554

ABSTRACT

Training chronic, cortically-blind (CB) patients on a coarse [left-right] direction discrimination and integration (CDDI) task recovers performance on this task at trained, blind field locations. However, fine direction difference (FDD) thresholds remain elevated at these locations, limiting the usefulness of recovered vision in daily life. Here, we asked if this FDD impairment can be overcome by training CB subjects with endogenous, feature-based attention (FBA) cues. Ten CB subjects were recruited and trained on CDDI and FDD with an FBA cue or FDD with a neutral cue. After completion of each training protocol, FDD thresholds were re-measured with both neutral and FBA cues at trained, blind-field locations and at corresponding, intact-field locations. In intact portions of the visual field, FDD thresholds were lower when tested with FBA than neutral cues. Training subjects in the blind field on the CDDI task improved FDD performance to the point that a threshold could be measured, but these locations remained impaired relative to the intact field. FDD training with neutral cues resulted in better blind field FDD thresholds than CDDI training, but thresholds remained impaired relative to intact field levels, regardless of testing cue condition. Importantly, training FDD in the blind field with FBA lowered FDD thresholds relative to CDDI training, and allowed the blind field to reach thresholds similar to the intact field, even when FBA trained subjects were tested with a neutral rather than FBA cue. Finally, FDD training appeared to also recover normal integration thresholds at trained, blind-field locations, providing an interesting double dissociation with respect to CDDI training. In summary, mechanisms governing FBA appear to function normally in both intact and impaired regions of the visual field following V1 damage. Our results mark the first time that FDD thresholds in CB fields have been seen to reach intact field levels of performance. Moreover, FBA can be leveraged during visual training to recover normal, fine direction discrimination and integration performance at trained, blind-field locations, potentiating visual recovery of more complex and precise aspects of motion perception in cortically-blinded fields.


Subject(s)
Blindness, Cortical/psychology , Discrimination, Psychological , Adult , Aged , Attention , Blindness, Cortical/diagnostic imaging , Blindness, Cortical/rehabilitation , Cues , Female , Humans , Learning , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Middle Aged , Motion Perception , Orientation , Recovery of Function , Stroke/complications , Stroke/diagnostic imaging , Stroke Rehabilitation , Visual Cortex/diagnostic imaging , Visual Fields , Young Adult
10.
Neuropsychologia ; 128: 44-49, 2019 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28993236

ABSTRACT

The human brain can process facial expressions of emotions rapidly and without awareness. Several studies in patients with damage to their primary visual cortices have shown that they may be able to guess the emotional expression on a face despite their cortical blindness. This non-conscious processing, called affective blindsight, may arise through an intact subcortical visual route that leads from the superior colliculus to the pulvinar, and thence to the amygdala. This pathway is thought to process the crude visual information conveyed by the low spatial frequencies of the stimuli. In order to investigate whether this is the case, we studied a patient (TN) with bilateral cortical blindness and affective blindsight. An fMRI paradigm was performed in which fearful and neutral expressions were presented using faces that were either unfiltered, or filtered to remove high or low spatial frequencies. Unfiltered fearful faces produced right amygdala activation although the patient was unaware of the presence of the stimuli. More importantly, the low spatial frequency components of fearful faces continued to produce right amygdala activity while the high spatial frequency components did not. Our findings thus confirm that the visual information present in the low spatial frequencies is sufficient to produce affective blindsight, further suggesting that its existence could rely on the subcortical colliculo-pulvino-amygdalar pathway.


Subject(s)
Affect , Blindness, Cortical/psychology , Amygdala/diagnostic imaging , Amygdala/physiopathology , Blindness, Cortical/diagnostic imaging , Facial Expression , Facial Recognition , Fear/psychology , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Middle Aged , Stroke/complications , Stroke/psychology , Visual Cortex/diagnostic imaging , Visual Cortex/physiopathology , Visual Pathways/diagnostic imaging , Visual Pathways/physiopathology , Visual Perception
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL