Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 43
Filtrar
1.
J Neurosci ; 43(22): 4033-4046, 2023 05 31.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37142429

RESUMO

Dexterous object manipulation depends critically on information about forces normal and tangential to the fingerpads, and also on torque associated with object orientation at grip surfaces. We investigated how torque information is encoded by human tactile afferents in the fingerpads and compared them to 97 afferents recorded in monkeys (n = 3; 2 females) in our previous study. Human data included slowly-adapting Type-II (SA-II) afferents, which are absent in the glabrous skin of monkeys. Torques of different magnitudes (3.5-7.5 mNm) were applied in clockwise and anticlockwise directions to a standard central site on the fingerpads of 34 human subjects (19 females). Torques were superimposed on a 2, 3, or 4 N background normal force. Unitary recordings were made from fast-adapting Type-I (FA-I, n = 39), and slowly-adapting Type-I (SA-I, n = 31) and Type-II (SA-II, n = 13) afferents supplying the fingerpads via microelectrodes inserted into the median nerve. All three afferent types encoded torque magnitude and direction, with torque sensitivity being higher with smaller normal forces. SA-I afferent responses to static torque were inferior to dynamic stimuli in humans, while in monkeys the opposite was true. In humans this might be compensated by the addition of sustained SA-II afferent input, and their capacity to increase or decrease firing rates with direction of rotation. We conclude that the discrimination capacity of individual afferents of each type was inferior in humans than monkeys which could be because of differences in fingertip tissue compliance and skin friction.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT We investigated how individual human tactile nerve fibers encode rotational forces (torques) and compared them to their monkey counterparts. Human hands, but not monkey hands, are innervated by a tactile neuron type (SA-II afferents) specialized to encode directional skin strain yet, so far, torque encoding has only been studied in monkeys. We find that human SA-I afferents were generally less sensitive and less able to discriminate torque magnitude and direction than their monkey counterparts, especially during the static phase of torque loading. However, this shortfall in humans could be compensated by SA-II afferent input. This indicates that variation in afferent types might complement each other signaling different stimulus features possibly providing computational advantage to discriminate stimuli.


Assuntos
Dedos , Tato , Feminino , Humanos , Torque , Tato/fisiologia , Dedos/fisiologia , Pele/inervação , Mãos , Mecanorreceptores/fisiologia , Neurônios Aferentes/fisiologia
2.
J Physiol ; 602(9): 2089-2106, 2024 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38544437

RESUMO

When manipulating objects, humans begin adjusting their grip force to friction within 100 ms of contact. During motor adaptation, subjects become aware of the slipperiness of touched surfaces. Previously, we have demonstrated that humans cannot perceive frictional differences when surfaces are brought in contact with an immobilised finger, but can do so when there is submillimeter lateral displacement or subjects actively make the contact movement. Similarly, in, we investigated how humans perceive friction in the absence of intentional exploratory sliding or rubbing movements, to mimic object manipulation interactions. We used a two-alternative forced-choice paradigm in which subjects had to reach and touch one surface followed by another, and then indicate which felt more slippery. Subjects correctly identified the more slippery surface in 87 ± 8% of cases (mean ± SD; n = 12). Biomechanical analysis of finger pad skin displacement patterns revealed the presence of tiny (<1 mm) localised slips, known to be sufficient to perceive frictional differences. We tested whether these skin movements arise as a result of natural hand reaching kinematics. The task was repeated with the introduction of a hand support, eliminating the hand reaching movement and minimising fingertip movement deviations from a straight path. As a result, our subjects' performance significantly declined (66 ± 12% correct, mean ± SD; n = 12), suggesting that unrestricted reaching movement kinematics and factors such as physiological tremor, play a crucial role in enhancing or enabling friction perception upon initial contact. KEY POINTS: More slippery objects require a stronger grip to prevent them from slipping out of hands. Grip force adjustments to friction driven by tactile sensory signals are largely automatic and do not necessitate cognitive involvement; nevertheless, some associated awareness of grip surface slipperiness under such sensory conditions is present and helps to select a safe and appropriate movement plan. When gripping an object, tactile receptors provide frictional information without intentional rubbing or sliding fingers over the surface. However, we have discovered that submillimeter range lateral displacement might be required to enhance or enable friction sensing. The present study provides evidence that such small lateral movements causing localised partial slips arise and are an inherent part of natural reaching movement kinematics.


Assuntos
Fricção , Movimento , Humanos , Masculino , Fenômenos Biomecânicos , Adulto , Feminino , Movimento/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem , Braço/fisiologia , Percepção do Tato/fisiologia , Dedos/fisiologia , Força da Mão/fisiologia , Tato/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia
3.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 118(49)2021 12 07.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34857635

RESUMO

Humans efficiently estimate the grip force necessary to lift a variety of objects, including slippery ones. The regulation of grip force starts with the initial contact and takes into account the surface properties, such as friction. This estimation of the frictional strength has been shown to depend critically on cutaneous information. However, the physical and perceptual mechanism that provides such early tactile information remains elusive. In this study, we developed a friction-modulation apparatus to elucidate the effects of the frictional properties of objects during initial contact. We found a correlation between participants' conscious perception of friction and radial strain patterns of skin deformation. The results provide insights into the tactile cues made available by contact mechanics to the sensorimotor regulation of grip, as well as to the conscious perception of the frictional properties of an object.


Assuntos
Fricção/fisiologia , Percepção do Tato/fisiologia , Tato/fisiologia , Adulto , Feminino , Dedos/fisiologia , Força da Mão/fisiologia , Humanos , Masculino , Fenômenos Mecânicos , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Pele , Fenômenos Fisiológicos da Pele , Propriedades de Superfície , Resistência à Tração/fisiologia
4.
J Neurophysiol ; 128(4): 1074-1084, 2022 Oct 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36102518

RESUMO

The perceived intensity of a vibrotactile stimulus is thought to depend on single-neuron firing rates (rate coding) and the number of active afferents (population coding). Unaddressed until now is whether the temporal relation of individual spikes also conveys information about tactile intensity. We used cutaneous electro-tactile stimulation to investigate how the temporal structure of a fixed number of spikes in a 1-s train influenced the perception of intensity. Four mean spike rates spanning the flutter and vibratory hum range (36 Hz, 60 Hz; 120 Hz, 180 Hz) were tested, with spikes grouped into a regular pattern, or bursts of 2-6 spikes spaced 3 ms apart. To link a putative neural code to perception, perceived intensity was assessed in 16 human participants (aged 20-45; 4 females) using the psychophysical paradigm of magnitude estimation. Compound sensory nerve action potentials were recorded to assess any stimulus variation in afferent recruitment. The temporal structuring of a fixed number of spikes into periodic bursts of multiple spikes altered perceived intensity as a function of burst spike count. The largest increase was seen at 36 Hz, where the bursts of six spikes were rated 2.1 times stronger than the regularly spaced spikes [95% confidence interval (CI): 1.9-2.3]. The true increase is likely larger as temporal structuring of spikes into bursts had some negative effect on afferent recruitment. We conclude that the perceived intensity can be modulated by changing temporal features of afferent discharge even when normalized for the number of recruited afferents.NEW & NOTEWORTHY Structuring a fixed number of spikes into temporal burst patterns evoke gradations of perceived intensity with burst spike count, emphasizing the importance of spike timing in primary afferents for shaping perception. This forms the basis for new strategies in communicating a range of intensity information to users of neural interfaces by simply varying the timing of spikes in nonspecific primary afferents using fixed-charge electric pulses, without requiring alterations in stimulation current or mean pulse frequency.


Assuntos
Neurônios , Tato , Potenciais de Ação/fisiologia , Potenciais Evocados , Feminino , Humanos , Neurônios/fisiologia , Neurônios Aferentes/fisiologia , Tato/fisiologia
5.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 33(12): 2427-2439, 2021 11 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34424986

RESUMO

Sensory suppression refers to the phenomenon that sensory input generated by our own actions, such as moving a finger to press a button to hear a tone, elicits smaller neural responses than sensory input generated by external agents. This observation is usually explained via the internal forward model in which an efference copy of the motor command is used to compute a corollary discharge, which acts to suppress sensory input. However, because moving a finger to press a button is accompanied by neural processes involved in preparing and performing the action, it is unclear whether sensory suppression is the result of movement planning, movement execution, or both. To investigate this, in two experiments, we compared ERPs to self-generated tones that were produced by voluntary, semivoluntary, or involuntary button-presses, with externally generated tones that were produced by a computer. In Experiment 1, the semivoluntary and involuntary button-presses were initiated by the participant or experimenter, respectively, by electrically stimulating the median nerve in the participant's forearm, and in Experiment 2, by applying manual force to the participant's finger. We found that tones produced by voluntary button-presses elicited a smaller N1 component of the ERP than externally generated tones. This is known as N1-suppression. However, tones produced by semivoluntary and involuntary button-presses did not yield significant N1-suppression. We also found that the magnitude of N1-suppression linearly decreased across the voluntary, semivoluntary, and involuntary conditions. These results suggest that movement planning is a necessary condition for producing sensory suppression. We conclude that the most parsimonious account of sensory suppression is the internal forward model.


Assuntos
Eletroencefalografia , Potenciais Evocados , Dedos , Humanos , Movimento
6.
J Neurophysiol ; 125(2): 687-692, 2021 02 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33439792

RESUMO

Perceived frequency of vibrotactile stimuli can be divided into two distinctive cutaneous sensations-flutter (<60 Hz) and vibratory hum (>60 Hz), mediated by two different tactile afferent types [fast adapting type I (FA1) and fast adapting type II (FA2), respectively]. We recently demonstrated a novel form of neural coding in the human tactile system, where frequency perception of stimulus pulses grouped into periodic bursts in the flutter range depended on the duration of the silent gap between bursts, rather than the periodicity or mean impulse rate. Here, we investigated whether this interburst interval could also explain the perceived frequency of electrocutaneous pulse patterns delivered at frequencies above the flutter range. At stimulus rates of 50 to 190 pulses/s, the burst gap model correctly predicted the perceived frequency. This shows that the burst gap code represents a general coding strategy that spans the range of frequencies traditionally attributed to two different tactile channels.NEW & NOTEWORTHY We present evidence for a generalized frequency processing strategy on tactile afferent inputs that is shared across a broad range of frequencies extending beyond the flutter range, supporting the notion that spike timing has an important role in shaping tactile perception.


Assuntos
Percepção do Tato , Tato , Adolescente , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Mecanorreceptores/classificação , Mecanorreceptores/fisiologia , Pele/citologia , Pele/inervação , Córtex Somatossensorial/citologia , Córtex Somatossensorial/fisiologia , Vibração , Adulto Jovem
7.
J Neurophysiol ; 125(3): 809-823, 2021 03 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33439786

RESUMO

Perception of the frictional properties of a surface contributes to the multidimensional experience of exploring various materials; we slide our fingers over a surface to feel it. In contrast, during object manipulation, we grip objects without such intended exploratory movements. Given that we are aware of the slipperiness of objects or tools that are held in the hand, we investigated whether the initial contact between the fingertip skin and the surface of the object is sufficient to provide this consciously perceived frictional information. Using a two-alternative forced-choice protocol, we examined human capacity to detect frictional differences using touch, when two otherwise structurally identical surfaces were brought in contact with the immobilized finger perpendicularly or under an angle (20° or 30°) to the skin surface (passive touch). An ultrasonic friction reduction device was used to generate three different frictions over each of three flat surfaces with different surface structure: 1) smooth glass, 2) textured surface with dome-shaped features, and 3) surface with sharp asperities (sandpaper). Participants (n = 12) could not reliably indicate which of the two surfaces was more slippery under any of these conditions. In contrast, when slip was induced by moving the surface laterally by a total of 5 mm (passive slip), participants could clearly perceive frictional differences. Thus making contact with the surface, even with moderate tangential forces, was not enough to perceive frictional differences, instead conscious perception required a sufficient size slip.NEW & NOTEWORTHY This study contributes to understanding how frictional information is obtained and used by the brain. When the skin is contacting surfaces of identical topography but varying frictional properties, the deformation pattern is different; however, available sensory cues did not get translated into perception of frictional properties unless a sufficiently large lateral movement was present. These neurophysiological findings may inform how to design and operate haptic devices relying on friction modulation principles.


Assuntos
Fricção , Movimento , Desempenho Psicomotor , Percepção do Tato , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Feminino , Dedos/fisiologia , Humanos , Masculino , Robótica/instrumentação , Tato , Ultrassom/instrumentação , Adulto Jovem
8.
J Neurophysiol ; 121(4): 1143-1149, 2019 04 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30699044

RESUMO

Studies on anesthetized animals have revealed that nociceptors can excite fusimotor neurons and thereby change the sensitivity of muscle spindles to stretch; such nociceptive reflexes have been suggested to underlie the mechanisms that lead to chronic musculoskeletal pain syndromes. However, the validity of the "vicious cycle" hypothesis in humans has yielded results contrasting with those found in animals. Given that spindle firing rates are much lower in humans than in animals, it is possible that some of the discrepancies between human experimental data and those obtained in animals could be explained by differences in background fusimotor drive when the leg muscles are relaxed. We examined the effects of tonic muscle pain during voluntary contractions of the ankle dorsiflexors. Unitary recordings were obtained from 10 fusimotor-driven muscle spindle afferents (6 primary, 4 secondary) supplying the ankle dorsiflexors via a microelectrode inserted percutaneously into the common peroneal nerve. A series of 1-min weak contractions was performed at rest and during 1 h of muscle pain induced by intramuscular infusion of 5% hypertonic saline into the tibialis anterior muscle. We did not observe any statistically significant increases in muscle spindle firing rates of six afferents followed during tonic muscle pain, although discharge variability increased slightly. Furthermore, a participant's capacity to maintain a constant level of force, while relying on proprioceptive feedback in the absence of visual feedback, was not compromised during pain. We conclude that nociceptive inputs from contracting muscle do not excite fusimotor neurons during voluntary isometric contractions in humans. NEW & NOTEWORTHY Data obtained in the cat have shown that muscle pain causes a marked increase in the firing of muscle spindles, attributed to a nociceptor-driven fusimotor reflex. However, our studies of muscle spindles in relaxed leg muscles failed to find any effect on spindle discharge. Here we showed that experimental muscle pain failed to increase the firing of muscle spindle afferents during weak voluntary contractions, when fusimotor drive sufficient to increase their firing is present.


Assuntos
Contração Isométrica , Fusos Musculares/fisiologia , Mialgia/fisiopatologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Tornozelo/fisiologia , Tornozelo/fisiopatologia , Retroalimentação Sensorial , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Fusos Musculares/fisiopatologia , Nociceptividade , Nervo Fibular/fisiologia , Nervo Fibular/fisiopatologia , Reflexo
9.
J Neurophysiol ; 117(4): 1489-1498, 2017 04 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28077660

RESUMO

Muscle spindles provide exquisitely sensitive proprioceptive information regarding joint position and movement. Through passively driven length changes in the muscle-tendon unit (MTU), muscle spindles detect joint rotations because of their in-parallel mechanical linkage to muscle fascicles. In human microneurography studies, muscle fascicles are assumed to follow the MTU and, as such, fascicle length is not measured in such studies. However, under certain mechanical conditions, compliant structures can act to decouple the fascicles, and, therefore, the spindles, from the MTU. Such decoupling may reduce the fidelity by which muscle spindles encode joint position and movement. The aim of the present study was to measure, for the first time, both the changes in firing of single muscle spindle afferents and changes in muscle fascicle length in vivo from the tibialis anterior muscle (TA) during passive rotations about the ankle. Unitary recordings were made from 15 muscle spindle afferents supplying TA via a microelectrode inserted into the common peroneal nerve. Ultrasonography was used to measure the length of an individual fascicle of TA. We saw a strong correlation between fascicle length and firing rate during passive ankle rotations of varying rates (0.1-0.5 Hz) and amplitudes (1-9°). In particular, we saw responses observed at relatively small changes in muscle length that highlight the sensitivity of the TA muscle to small length changes. This study is the first to measure spindle firing and fascicle dynamics in vivo and provides an experimental basis for further understanding the link between fascicle length, MTU length, and spindle firing patterns.NEW & NOTEWORTHY Muscle spindles are exquisitely sensitive to changes in muscle length, but recordings from human muscle spindle afferents are usually correlated with joint angle rather than muscle fascicle length. In this study, we monitored both muscle fascicle length and spindle firing from the human tibialis anterior muscle in vivo. Our findings are the first to measure these signals in vivo and provide an experimental basis for exploring this link further.


Assuntos
Movimento/fisiologia , Fusos Musculares/fisiologia , Músculo Esquelético/fisiologia , Propriocepção/fisiologia , Tendões/fisiologia , Adulto , Articulação do Tornozelo/inervação , Teorema de Bayes , Biofísica , Estimulação Elétrica , Eletromiografia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Fusos Musculares/diagnóstico por imagem , Músculo Esquelético/diagnóstico por imagem , Ultrassonografia , Adulto Jovem
10.
Neurocase ; 22(1): 95-102, 2016.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25965510

RESUMO

Well-organized somatotopic representation of the hand is required to interpret input from cutaneous mechanoreceptors. Previous reports have identified patients with various distortions of somatotopic representation after stroke. Importantly, those patients were investigated years after the stroke, indicating that afferent signal regained access to the cortical circuits; however, further plastic changes, which would re-establish somatotopic order and ability to correctly localize tactile stimuli, did not follow. Thus, it was not known whether somatotopic organization could be restored in such patients and whether there is a potential for new rehabilitation strategies. This is the first case report demonstrating normalization of somatotopic representation.


Assuntos
Recuperação de Função Fisiológica/fisiologia , Córtex Somatossensorial/fisiopatologia , Acidente Vascular Cerebral/fisiopatologia , Tato/fisiologia , Mapeamento Encefálico , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade
11.
J Neurophysiol ; 114(1): 474-84, 2015 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25948866

RESUMO

Dexterous manipulation is not possible without sensory information about object properties and manipulative forces. Fundamental neuroscience has been unable to demonstrate how information about multiple stimulus parameters may be continuously extracted, concurrently, from a population of tactile afferents. This is the first study to demonstrate this, using spike trains recorded from tactile afferents innervating the monkey fingerpad. A multiple-regression model, requiring no a priori knowledge of stimulus-onset times or stimulus combination, was developed to obtain continuous estimates of instantaneous force and torque. The stimuli consisted of a normal-force ramp (to a plateau of 1.8, 2.2, or 2.5 N), on top of which -3.5, -2.0, 0, +2.0, or +3.5 mNm torque was applied about the normal to the skin surface. The model inputs were sliding windows of binned spike counts recorded from each afferent. Models were trained and tested by 15-fold cross-validation to estimate instantaneous normal force and torque over the entire stimulation period. With the use of the spike trains from 58 slow-adapting type I and 25 fast-adapting type I afferents, the instantaneous normal force and torque could be estimated with small error. This study demonstrated that instantaneous force and torque parameters could be reliably extracted from a small number of tactile afferent responses in a real-time fashion with stimulus combinations that the model had not been exposed to during training. Analysis of the model weights may reveal how interactions between stimulus parameters could be disentangled for complex population responses and could be used to test neurophysiologically relevant hypotheses about encoding mechanisms.


Assuntos
Dedos/inervação , Dedos/fisiologia , Mecanorreceptores/fisiologia , Tato/fisiologia , Animais , Macaca nemestrina , Nervo Mediano/fisiologia , Modelos Neurológicos , Estimulação Física/instrumentação , Estimulação Física/métodos , Análise de Regressão , Torque
12.
J Neurophysiol ; 114(4): 2249-57, 2015 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26269550

RESUMO

It is not known how changes in skin mechanics affect the responses of cutaneous mechanoreceptors in the finger pads to compression forces. We used venous occlusion to change the stiffness of the fingers and investigated whether this influenced the firing of low-threshold mechanoreceptors to surfaces of differing stiffness. Unitary recordings were made from 10 slowly adapting type I (SAI), 10 fast adapting type I (FAI) and 9 slowly adapting type II (SAII) units via tungsten microelectrodes inserted into the median nerve at the wrist. A servo-controlled stimulator applied ramp-and-hold forces (1, 2, and 4 N) at a constant loading and unloading rate (2 N/s) via a flat 2.5-cm-diameter silicone disk over the center of the finger pad. Nine silicone disks (objects), varying in compliance, were used. Venous occlusion, produced by inflating a sphygmomanometer cuff around the upper arm to 40 ± 5 mmHg, was used to induce swelling of the fingers and increase the compliance of the finger pulp. Venous occlusion had no effect on the firing rates of the SAI afferents, nor on the slopes of the relationship between mean firing rate and object compliance at each amplitude, but did significantly reduce the slopes for the FAI afferents. Although the SAII afferents possess a poor capacity to encode changes in object compliance, mean firing rates were significantly lower during venous occlusion. The finding that venous occlusion had no effect on the firing properties of SAI afferents indicates that these afferents preserve their capacity to encode changes in object compliance, despite changes in skin mechanics.


Assuntos
Fenômenos Biomecânicos/fisiologia , Dedos/fisiologia , Nervo Mediano/fisiologia , Neurônios Aferentes/fisiologia , Fenômenos Fisiológicos da Pele , Tato/fisiologia , Potenciais de Ação , Adolescente , Adulto , Elasticidade , Feminino , Dedos/inervação , Humanos , Masculino , Microeletrodos , Estimulação Física , Pele/inervação , Pressão Venosa/fisiologia , Punho/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
13.
BMC Neurosci ; 16: 52, 2015 Aug 13.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26268809

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: A myriad of studies have argued that tactile sensibility is underpinned exclusively by large myelinated mechanoreceptors. However, the functional significance of their slow-conducting counterparts, termed C-low threshold mechanoreceptors (C-LTMRs), remains largely unexplored. We recently showed the emergence of brush- and vibration-evoked allodynia in human hairy and glabrous skin during background muscle pain. The allodynia persisted following the preferential blockade of myelinated fibres but was abolished by the preferential blockade of cutaneous C fibres, thereby suggesting a pathway involving hairy skin C-LTMRs and their functional counterparts in glabrous skin in this phenomenon. In the present study, we tested the effects of preferential A- and C-fibre conduction blocks and pharmacological blockade of T-type calcium channel Cav3.2 (expressed selectively on small-fibre LTMRs) on monofilament detection thresholds in healthy participants by compression, low-dose intradermal anaesthesia (xylocaine 0.25 %) and selective T-channel antagonist, TTA-A2. RESULTS: We found that all participants could detect monofilament contacts (as low as 1.6 mN) within the innocuous tactile range regardless of the preferential blockade of myelinated fibres. Furthermore, during the compression block no subject reported a switch in modality from touch to pain. That is, the low-force monofilament contacts were always perceived as non-painful. However, there was a small but significant elevation of monofilament thresholds (~2 mN) in the glabrous skin following the compression block. Importantly, no differences were found in the thresholds across hairy and glabrous regions while the myelinated fibres were conducting or not. The preferential blockade of C fibres in the glabrous skin (with myelinated fibres intact) also resulted in a small but significant elevation of tactile thresholds. Furthermore, the use of T-channel blocker in the glabrous skin during compression block of myelinated fibres resulted in complete abolition of monofilament sensibility within the innocuous tactile range (tested up to ~20 mN). CONCLUSIONS: These observations suggest that C-LTMRs need not be regarded as a redundant tactile system, but appear to complement normal large-myelinated-fibre tactile function. Convergent findings in glabrous and hairy skin lend support for an underlying system of innocuous mechanoreception with Cav3.2-expressing unmyelinated fibres.


Assuntos
Benzenoacetamidas/farmacologia , Bloqueadores dos Canais de Cálcio/farmacologia , Canais de Cálcio Tipo T/metabolismo , Lidocaína/farmacologia , Mecanorreceptores/efeitos dos fármacos , Piridinas/farmacologia , Bloqueadores do Canal de Sódio Disparado por Voltagem/farmacologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Mãos/inervação , Humanos , Masculino , Mecanorreceptores/metabolismo , Bloqueio Nervoso , Percepção da Dor/efeitos dos fármacos , Percepção da Dor/fisiologia , Estimulação Física , Limiar Sensorial/efeitos dos fármacos , Limiar Sensorial/fisiologia , Pele/efeitos dos fármacos , Pele/inervação , Percepção do Tato/efeitos dos fármacos , Percepção do Tato/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
14.
J Neurophysiol ; 111(6): 1308-17, 2014 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24371291

RESUMO

We undertook a neurophysiological investigation of the responses of low-threshold mechanoreceptors in the human finger pad to surfaces of differing softness. Unitary recordings were made from 26 slowly adapting type I (SAI), 17 fast-adapting type I (FAI), and 9 slowly adapting type II (SAII) afferents via tungsten microelectrodes inserted into the median nerve at the wrist. A servo-controlled stimulator applied ramp-and-hold forces (1, 2, 4 N) at a constant loading and unloading rate (2 N/s) via a flat silicone disc over the center of the finger pad. Nine discs were used, which linearly increased in stiffness across the range. Population responses of the SAI afferents showed the greatest sensitivity to compliance, with a steep monotonic increase in mean firing rate with increasing stiffness (decreasing compliance) of the surface during the loading and plateau (but not unloading) phases. FAI afferents also showed a linear increase in firing during the loading but not unloading phase, although the slope was significantly lower than that of the SAI afferents at all amplitudes. Conversely, SAII afferents were influenced by object compliance only in certain conditions. Given their high density in the finger pads and their linear relationship between firing rate and object compliance during the loading and plateau phases, SAI afferents (together with FAI afferents during the loading phase) are ideally suited to contributing information on surface compliance to the overall estimation of softness, but the SAII afferents appear to play only a minor role.


Assuntos
Potenciais de Ação , Dedos/inervação , Neurônios Aferentes/fisiologia , Limiar Sensorial , Tato , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Dedos/fisiologia , Humanos , Masculino , Mecanorreceptores/fisiologia , Nervo Mediano/citologia , Nervo Mediano/fisiologia , Tempo de Reação
15.
J Neurophysiol ; 112(10): 2382-7, 2014 Nov 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25143540

RESUMO

We simultaneously compared the sensitivity of single primary afferent neurons supplying the glabrous skin of the hand and the psychophysical amplitude discrimination thresholds in human subjects for a set of vibrotactile stimuli delivered to the receptive field. All recorded afferents had a dynamic range narrower than the range of amplitudes across which the subjects could discriminate. However, when the vibration amplitude was chosen to be within the steepest part of the afferent's stimulus-response function the response of single afferents, defined as the spike count over the vibration duration (500 ms), was often more sensitive in discriminating vibration amplitude than the perceptual judgment of the participants. We quantified how the neuronal performance depended on the integration window: for short windows the neuronal performance was inferior to the performance of the subject. The neuronal performance progressively improved with increasing spike count duration and reached a level significantly above that of the subjects when the integration window was 250 ms or longer. The superiority in performance of individual neurons over observers could reflect a nonoptimal integration window or be due to the presence of noise between the sensory periphery and the cortical decision stage. Additionally, it could indicate that the range of perceptual sensitivity comes at the cost of discrimination through pooling across neurons with different response functions.


Assuntos
Discriminação Psicológica/fisiologia , Neurônios Aferentes/fisiologia , Percepção do Tato/fisiologia , Vibração , Potenciais de Ação , Feminino , Mãos/fisiologia , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulação Física , Psicofísica
16.
Exp Brain Res ; 232(4): 1309-15, 2014 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24477764

RESUMO

We recently showed that long-lasting muscle pain, induced by intramuscular infusion of hypertonic saline, evoked two patterns of cardiovascular responses across subjects: one group showed parallel increases in muscle sympathetic nerve activity (MSNA), blood pressure, and heart rate, while the other group showed parallel decreases. Given that MSNA is consistent day to day, we tested the hypothesis that individuals who show increases in MSNA during experimental muscle pain will show consistent responses over time. MSNA was recorded from the peroneal nerve, together with blood pressure and heart rate, during an intramuscular infusion of hypertonic saline causing pain for an hour in 15 subjects on two occasions, 2-27 weeks apart. Pain intensity ratings were not significantly different between the first (5.8 ± 0.4/10) and second (6.1 ± 0.2) recording sessions. While four subjects showed significant decreases in the first session (46.6 ± 9.2% of baseline) and significant increases in the second (159.6 ± 8.9%), in 11 subjects, there was consistency in the changes in MSNA over time: either a sustained decrease (55.6 ± 6.8%, n = 6) or a sustained increase (143.5 ± 6.1%, n = 5) occurred in both recording sessions. There were no differences in pain ratings between sessions for any subjects. We conclude that the changes in MSNA during long-lasting muscle pain are consistent over time in the majority of individuals, reflecting the importance of studying interindividual differences in physiology.


Assuntos
Fibras Adrenérgicas/fisiologia , Músculo Esquelético/fisiologia , Mialgia/fisiopatologia , Adolescente , Fibras Adrenérgicas/efeitos dos fármacos , Adulto , Pressão Sanguínea/efeitos dos fármacos , Pressão Sanguínea/fisiologia , Feminino , Frequência Cardíaca/efeitos dos fármacos , Frequência Cardíaca/fisiologia , Humanos , Masculino , Músculo Esquelético/efeitos dos fármacos , Mialgia/induzido quimicamente , Mialgia/diagnóstico , Solução Salina Hipertônica/toxicidade , Sistema Nervoso Simpático/efeitos dos fármacos , Sistema Nervoso Simpático/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
17.
Front Neurosci ; 18: 1125597, 2024.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38894940

RESUMO

In neural prostheses, intensity modulation of a single channel (i.e., through a single stimulating electrode) has been achieved by increasing the magnitude or width of each stimulation pulse, which risks eliciting pain or paraesthesia; and by changing the stimulation rate, which leads to concurrent changes in perceived frequency. In this study, we sought to render a perception of tactile intensity and frequency independently, by means of temporal pulse train patterns of fixed magnitude, delivered non-invasively. Our psychophysical study exploits a previously discovered frequency coding mechanism, where the perceived frequency of stimulus pulses grouped into periodic bursts depends on the duration of the inter-burst interval, rather than the mean pulse rate or periodicity. When electrical stimulus pulses were organised into bursts, perceived intensity was influenced by the number of pulses within a burst, while perceived frequency was determined by the time between the end of one burst envelope and the start of the next. The perceived amplitude was modulated by 1.6× while perceived frequency was varied independently by 2× within the tested range (20-40 Hz). Thus, the sensation of intensity might be controlled independently from frequency through a single stimulation channel without having to vary the injected electrical current. This can form the basis for improving strategies in delivering more complex and natural sensations for prosthetic hand users.

18.
Exp Physiol ; 98(6): 1125-32, 2013 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23417691

RESUMO

Experimental pain induced in animals has shown that noxious stimulation of group III and IV afferents increases the firing of muscle spindles via a reflex excitation of fusimotor (γ) motoneurones. Chronic muscle pain has been hypothesized to develop as a result of a vicious cycle involving this mechanism. In order to explore the effects of long-lasting muscle pain on the fusimotor system, single unit muscle spindle afferents were recorded from 15 subjects. Afferent activity was recorded from foot and ankle extensor muscles whilst infusing hypertonic saline into the tibialis anterior muscle of the ipsilateral leg, producing moderate-strong pain lasting for ∼60 min. A change in fusimotor drive was inferred by observing changes in the mean discharge rate of spontaneously active muscle spindle afferents. Homonymous and heteronymous muscles remained relaxed and showed no increase in activity, arguing against any fusimotor-driven increase in motor activity, and there was no net change in the firing of muscle spindle afferents. We conclude that long-lasting stimulation of group III and IV afferents fails to excite fusimotor neurones and increase muscle spindle discharge. Accordingly, the vicious cycle theory has no functional basis for the development of myalgia in human subjects.


Assuntos
Perna (Membro)/fisiologia , Neurônios Motores gama/fisiologia , Músculo Esquelético/fisiologia , Músculo Esquelético/fisiopatologia , Mialgia/fisiopatologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Animais , Estimulação Elétrica/métodos , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Contração Muscular/fisiologia , Fusos Musculares/fisiologia , Reflexo/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
19.
Am J Physiol Heart Circ Physiol ; 302(3): H826-36, 2012 Feb 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22114133

RESUMO

Assessment of spontaneous slow waves in the peripheral blood volume using the photoplethysmogram (PPG) has shown potential clinical value, but the physiological correlates of these fluctuations have not been fully elucidated. This study addressed the contribution of arterial pressure and muscle sympathetic nerve activity (MSNA) in beat-to-beat PPG variability in resting humans under spontaneous breathing conditions. Peripheral PPG waveforms were measured from the fingertip, earlobe, and toe in young and healthy individuals (n = 13), together with the arterial pressure waveform, electrocardiogram, respiration, and direct measurement of MSNA by microneurography. Cross-spectral coherence analysis revealed that among the PPG waveforms, low-frequency fluctuations (0.04-0.15 Hz) in the ear PPG had the highest coherence with arterial pressure (0.71 ± 0.15) and MSNA (0.44 ± 0.18, with a peak of 0.71 ± 0.16 at 0.10 ± 0.03 Hz). The normalized midfrequency powers (0.08-0.15 Hz), with an emphasis on the 0.1-Hz region, were positively correlated between MSNA and the ear PPG (r = 0.77, P = 0.002). Finger and toe PPGs had lower coherence with arterial pressure (0.35 ± 0.10 and 0.30 ± 0.11, respectively) and MSNA (0.33 ± 0.10 and 0.26 ± 0.10, respectively) in the LF band but displayed higher coherence between themselves (0.54 ± 0.09) compared with the ear (P < 0.001), which may suggest the dominance of regional vasomotor activities and a common sympathetic influence in the glabrous skin. These findings highlight the differential mechanisms governing PPG waveform fluctuations across different body sites. Spontaneous PPG variability in the ear includes a major contribution from arterial pressure and MSNA, which may provide a rationale for its clinical utility.


Assuntos
Pressão Sanguínea/fisiologia , Volume Sanguíneo/fisiologia , Músculo Esquelético/irrigação sanguínea , Músculo Esquelético/inervação , Fotopletismografia/métodos , Sistema Nervoso Simpático/fisiologia , Adulto , Determinação do Volume Sanguíneo/métodos , Determinação do Volume Sanguíneo/normas , Feminino , Dedos/irrigação sanguínea , Humanos , Modelos Lineares , Masculino , Fotopletismografia/normas , Valores de Referência , Dedos do Pé/irrigação sanguínea , Adulto Jovem
20.
Exp Physiol ; 97(10): 1084-92, 2012 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22581744

RESUMO

We recently showed that acute muscle pain, induced by bolus intramuscular injection of hypertonic saline, causes a sustained increase in muscle sympathetic nerve activity (MSNA) and a modest increase in blood pressure and heart rate. However, it is not known whether long-lasting (tonic) pain, which more closely resembles chronic pain, causes a sustained increase in MSNA and blood pressure. We tested this hypothesis by recording MSNA in 12 healthy subjects. Tonic pain was induced for ~60 min by slow intramuscular infusion of hypertonic saline (7%) into the ipsilateral tibialis anterior muscle. Pain was sustained at a tolerable level (5/10 to 6/10 on a visual analog scale). Seven subjects showed progressive increases in mean MSNA amplitude during tonic pain, increasing to 154 ± 17% (SEM) at 45 min and remaining essentially constant for the duration of the infusion. In these subjects, blood pressure and heart rate also increased. Conversely, for the other five subjects MSNA showed a progressive decline, with a peak fall of 67 ± 11% at 40 min; blood pressure and heart rate also fell in these subjects. We conclude that tonic muscle pain has long-lasting effects on the sympathetic control of blood pressure, causing a sustained increase in some subjects yet a sustained decrease in others. This may have implications for individual differences in the cardiovascular consequences of chronic pain.


Assuntos
Pressão Sanguínea/fisiologia , Frequência Cardíaca/fisiologia , Músculo Esquelético/inervação , Dor/fisiopatologia , Sistema Nervoso Simpático/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Pressão Sanguínea/efeitos dos fármacos , Fenômenos Fisiológicos Cardiovasculares/efeitos dos fármacos , Sistema Cardiovascular/efeitos dos fármacos , Sistema Cardiovascular/fisiopatologia , Feminino , Frequência Cardíaca/efeitos dos fármacos , Humanos , Individualidade , Injeções Intramusculares , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Músculo Esquelético/efeitos dos fármacos , Dor/induzido quimicamente , Solução Salina Hipertônica/farmacologia , Sistema Nervoso Simpático/efeitos dos fármacos , Adulto Jovem
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA