Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 10 de 10
Filtrar
Mais filtros

Base de dados
País/Região como assunto
Tipo de documento
País de afiliação
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process ; 25(2): 211-24, 1999 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10331920

RESUMO

In 2 studies, the authors investigated whether evaluative conditioning (EC) is an associative phenomenon. Experiment 1 compared a standard EC paradigm with nonpaired and no-treatment control conditions. EC effects were obtained only when the conditioned stimulus (CS) and unconditioned stimulus (UCS) were rated as perceptually similar. However, similar EC effects were obtained in both control groups. An earlier failure to obtain EC effects was reanalyzed in Experiment 2. Conditioning-like effects were found when comparing a CS with the most perceptually similar UCSs used in the procedure but not when analyzing a CS rating with respect to the UCS with which it was paired during conditioning. The implications are that EC effects found in many studies are not due to associative learning and that the special characteristics of EC (conditioning without awareness and resistance to extinction) are probably nonassociative artifacts of the EC paradigm.


Assuntos
Condicionamento Psicológico , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Extinção Psicológica , Expressão Facial , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Rememoração Mental , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Distribuição Aleatória
2.
J Psychosom Res ; 57(4): 399-407, 2004 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15518676

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: This study aimed to explore cross-sectional and longitudinal associations between pathological worry and fatigue in a working population. METHODS: In employees with very low or very high fatigue levels, psychometrics of the Penn State Worry Questionnaire (PSWQ; measuring pathological worry) and the Checklist Individual Strength (CIS; measuring fatigue) were examined and their cross-sectional and longitudinal associations were explored. RESULTS: Pathological worry and fatigue can be measured as different constructs. However, pathological worry and fatigue were also associated on a cross-sectional level. Pathological worry predicted fatigue level 10 months later, but this association disappeared after adjustment for the cross-sectional association between pathological worry and fatigue. CONCLUSION: Although they can be measured as different constructs, pathological worry and fatigue seem to be associated. When studying longitudinal relations between pathological worry and fatigue, their cross-sectional association should be taken into account. Pathological worry might not be a risk factor for fatigue per se, but might act more like a mediating factor.


Assuntos
Transtornos de Ansiedade/epidemiologia , Fadiga/epidemiologia , Doenças Profissionais/epidemiologia , Adulto , Transtornos de Ansiedade/psicologia , Estudos de Coortes , Comorbidade , Estudos Transversais , Feminino , Humanos , Estudos Longitudinais , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Países Baixos , Doenças Profissionais/psicologia , Inventário de Personalidade/estatística & dados numéricos , Estudos Prospectivos , Psicometria , Fatores de Risco , Estatística como Assunto
3.
Behav Res Ther ; 41(11): 1277-93, 2003 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14527528

RESUMO

Field, Argyris and Knowles (Behav Res Ther 39 (2001) 1259), and Field, Hamilton, Knowles and Plews (Behav Res Thera 41 (2003) 113) have developed a prospective paradigm for testing Rachman's (Behav Res Ther 15 (1977) 375) proposition that fear information is important in the development of fears and phobias in children. Despite this paradigm being an advance on retrospective reports, the research so far has been restricted to self-reported fear beliefs measured after the information is given. This gives rise to two possible shortcomings: (1) the effects could simply reflect demand characteristics resulting from children conforming to the experimental demands, and (2) although fear information changes beliefs, this might not translate into the behavioural change that would be expected if this information has a powerful effect relevant to the development of pathological fear. This paper describes an experiment that attempts to address these concerns by improving Field et al.'s (2001, 2003) basic paradigm but with the addition of two measures: (1) a behavioural measure of avoidance, and (2) an implicit attitude task that should not be susceptible to deliberate attempts to conform to experimental demands. The result showed that negative and positive information have dramatic, and opposite, effects on self-reported fear beliefs, behavioural avoidance and implicit attitudes. There were no effects of gender on any of these results. This study fully supports Rachman's model and suggests that past work does not merely reflect demand characteristics and that fear information increases behavioural avoidance as well as fear beliefs.


Assuntos
Atitude , Aprendizagem da Esquiva , Medo/psicologia , Animais , Criança , Feminino , Humanos , Aprendizagem , Masculino , Marsupiais , Testes Psicológicos , Psicologia da Criança
4.
Behav Res Ther ; 39(11): 1259-76, 2001 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11686263

RESUMO

Rachman's theory [The conditioning theory of fear insition: a critical examination. Behav. Res. Ther. 15 (1977) 375-387] of fear acquisition suggests that fears and phobias can be acquired through three pathways: direct conditioning, vicarious learning and information/instruction. Although retrospective studies have provided some evidence for these pathways in the development of phobias during childhood [see King, Gullone, & Ollendick, Etiology of childhood phobias: current status of Rachman's three pathway's theory. Behav. Res. Ther. 36 (1998) 297-309 for a review], these studies have relied on long-term past memories of adult phobics or their parents. The current study was aimed towards developing a paradigm in which the plausibility of Rachman's indirect pathways could be investigated prospectively. In Experiment 1, children aged between 7 and 9 were presented with two types of information about novel stimuli (two monsters): video information and verbal information in the form of a story. Fear-related beliefs about the monsters changed significantly as a result of verbal information but not video information. Having established an operational paradigm, Experiment 2 looked at whether the source of verbal information had an effect on changes in fear-beliefs. Using the same paradigm, information about the monsters was provided by either a teacher, an adult stranger or a peer, or no information was given. Again, verbal information significantly changed fear-beliefs, but only when the information came from an adult. The role of information in the acquisition of fear and maintenance of avoidant behaviour is discussed with reference to modern conditioning theories of fear acquisition.


Assuntos
Condicionamento Clássico , Medo , Rememoração Mental , Transtornos Fóbicos/psicologia , Teoria Psicológica , Adulto , Aprendizagem da Esquiva , Criança , Feminino , Humanos , Comportamento Imitativo , Masculino , Desenvolvimento da Personalidade , Socialização
5.
Behav Res Ther ; 41(1): 113-23, 2003 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12488124

RESUMO

This paper presents a first attempt to develop a prospective paradigm to test Rachman's (Behav. Res. Ther. 15 (1977) 375) theory of fear acquisition for social fears. Following the prospective paradigm for animal fears developed by Field et al. (Behav. Res. Ther. 39 (2001) 1259) an attempt is made to adapt this paradigm to look at the effect of fear information in the development of social fears. A large group of normal children (N=135) who were at an age (10-13 years) at which social concerns are most pertinent were tested using this paradigm. They were given positive, negative or neutral information about three social situations: public speaking, eating in public, and meeting a new group of children. Children's fear beliefs were measured before and after the information was given and the information was given by a teacher, a same age peer or no information was given (a control). The results indicate that although information can change social fear beliefs it is dependent upon the type of social activity and who provides the information. The implications of these initial results for our understanding of both the role of fear information in the development of social fear beliefs, and the limitations of this current paradigm are discussed.


Assuntos
Condicionamento Psicológico , Medo/psicologia , Transtornos Fóbicos/psicologia , Adolescente , Criança , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Transtornos Fóbicos/etiologia , Estudos Prospectivos , Ajustamento Social
6.
Psychol Methods ; 6(2): 161-80, 2001 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11411440

RESUMO

The efficacy of the Hedges and colleagues, Rosenthal-Rubin, and Hunter-Schmidt methods for combining correlation coefficients was tested for cases in which population effect sizes were both fixed and variable. After a brief tutorial on these meta-analytic methods, the author presents two Monte Carlo simulations that compare these methods for cases in which the number of studies in the meta-analysis and the average sample size of studies were varied. In the fixed case the methods produced comparable estimates of the average effect size; however, the Hunter-Schmidt method failed to control the Type I error rate for the associated significance tests. In the variable case, for both the Hedges and colleagues and Hunter-Schmidt methods, Type I error rates were not controlled for meta-analyses including 15 or fewer studies and the probability of detecting small effects was less than .3. Some practical recommendations are made about the use of meta-analysis.


Assuntos
Metanálise como Assunto , Método de Monte Carlo , Psicometria , Humanos , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes
7.
N Z Med J ; 114(1142): 471-4, 2001 Oct 26.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11760242

RESUMO

AIM: To estimate the burden of visual impairment attributable to smoking in New Zealand. METHODS: Review of Medline-indexed literature on the relationship between smoking and eye disease and use of relevant New Zealand morbidity and smoking prevalence data. RESULTS: The international literature indicates there is strong evidence that smoking is a major cause of eye disease and blindness--particularly for cataracts and age-related macular degeneration (AMD). Using the most relevant international risk estimates, we estimated that 1335 people who are registered blind in New Zealand have AMD attributable to current and past smoking (26.8% of all AMD cases in the 55 years plus age-group). It was also estimated that 31 of the registered cases of visual impairment due to cataract and 396 hospitalisations for cataract surgery per year, are attributable to smoking. While subject to various methodological limitations, these estimates are probably under-estimates of the true burden of eye disease attributable to smoking. CONCLUSIONS: Smoking is a major cause of untreatable visual impairment and also a significant reason for cataract surgery in New Zealand. There is a need for more intensive tobacco control activities in New Zealand.


Assuntos
Oftalmopatias/etiologia , Fumar/efeitos adversos , Catarata/epidemiologia , Catarata/etiologia , Oftalmopatias/epidemiologia , Humanos , Incidência , Degeneração Macular/epidemiologia , Degeneração Macular/etiologia , Nova Zelândia , Fatores de Risco
8.
J Anxiety Disord ; 25(5): 697-705, 2011 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21489749

RESUMO

Using an experimental bias modification task, an interpretation bias towards or away from threat was induced about animal or social situations in a sample of 103 children split into a young (7-10 years) and old age group (11-15 years). Children rapidly learned to select outcomes of ambiguous situations which were congruent with their assigned modification condition. Following positive modification, children's threat interpretation biases significantly decreased, while threat biases increased (non-significantly) after negative modification. Bias modification effects also varied as a function of age with children appearing particularly vulnerable to acquiring biases about stimuli that were congruent with the normative fears for their age group. Weak age-related modification-congruent effects on younger but not older children's anxiety vulnerability in response to a behavioral task were also observed. However, no consistent effects of bias modification on avoidance behavior were found.


Assuntos
Ansiedade/terapia , Terapia Comportamental , Medo/psicologia , Transtornos Fóbicos/terapia , Adolescente , Afeto , Ansiedade/psicologia , Criança , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Transtornos Fóbicos/psicologia , Comportamento Social , Resultado do Tratamento
9.
Conscious Cogn ; 9(1): 13-36, 2000 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10753490

RESUMO

There is good evidence that, in general, autonomic conditioning in humans occurs only when subjects can verbalize the contingencies of conditioning. However, one form of conditioning, evaluative conditioning (EC), seems exceptional in that a growing body of evidence suggests that it can occur without conscious contingency awareness. As such, EC offers a unique insight into what role contingency awareness might play in associative learning. Despite this evidence, there are reasons to doubt that evaluative conditioning can occur without conscious awareness. This paper aims to critically review the EC literature and to draw some parallels to what is known about autonomic conditioning. In doing so, some important general issues about measuring contingency awareness are raised. These issues are illustrated with a brief report of an experiment in which a sensitive measure of contingency awareness is compared against a commonly used measure.


Assuntos
Sistema Nervoso Autônomo , Condicionamento Psicológico , Estado de Consciência , Condicionamento Clássico , Humanos , Modelos Psicológicos
10.
Conscious Cogn ; 10(4): 559-66; discussion 567-73, 2001 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11790043

RESUMO

Fulcher and Hammerl's (2001) important exploration of the role of contingency awareness in evaluative conditioning (EC) raises a lot of issues for discussion: (1) what boundaries, if any, exist between EC and affective learning paradigms?; (2) if EC does occur without awareness does this mean it is nonpropositional learning?; (3) is EC driven by stimulus-response (S-R), rather than stimulus-stimulus (S-S), associations and if so should it then surprise us that contingency awareness is not important?; and (4) if S-R associations are at the heart of EC, should we automatically assume EC is part of a different learning mechanism to autonomic Pavlovian conditioning (Field, 2000a, 2000b)? This article, after a critical review of Fulcher and Hammerl's work, discusses these issues with reference to what can be realistically inferred about the mechanisms underlying EC.


Assuntos
Conscientização , Cognição , Condicionamento Psicológico , Aprendizagem por Associação , Humanos
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA