Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 80
Filtrar
Mais filtros

Base de dados
País/Região como assunto
Tipo de documento
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Psychol Sci ; 35(7): 780-797, 2024 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38728320

RESUMO

It is commonly assumed that inner speech-the experience of thought as occurring in a natural language-is a human universal. Recent evidence, however, suggests that the experience of inner speech in adults varies from near constant to nonexistent. We propose a name for a lack of the experience of inner speech-anendophasia-and report four studies examining some of its behavioral consequences. We found that adults who reported low levels of inner speech (N = 46) had lower performance on a verbal working memory task and more difficulty performing rhyme judgments compared with adults who reported high levels of inner speech (N = 47). Task-switching performance-previously linked to endogenous verbal cueing-and categorical effects on perceptual judgments were unrelated to differences in inner speech.


Assuntos
Memória de Curto Prazo , Fala , Humanos , Masculino , Feminino , Adulto , Memória de Curto Prazo/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem , Julgamento/fisiologia , Pensamento/fisiologia
2.
Child Dev ; 95(2): 497-514, 2024.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37728552

RESUMO

The present study tested the hypothesis that verbal labels support category induction by providing compact hypotheses. Ninety-seven 4- to 6-year-old children (M = 63.2 months; 46 female, 51 male; 77% White, 8% more than one race, 4% Asian, and 3% Black; tested 2018) and 90 adults (M = 20.1 years; 70 female, 20 male) in the Midwestern United States learned novel categories with features that were easy (e.g., "red") or difficult (e.g., "mauve") to name. Adults (d = 1.06) and-to a lesser extent-children (d = 0.57; final training block) learned categories composed of more nameable features better. Children's knowledge of difficult-to-name color words predicted their learning for categories with difficult-to-name features. Rule-based category learning may be supported by the emerging ability to form verbal hypotheses.


Assuntos
Aprendizagem , Aprendizagem Verbal , Adulto , Humanos , Masculino , Feminino , Criança , Meio-Oeste dos Estados Unidos
3.
Behav Res Methods ; 56(3): 1640-1655, 2024 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37081237

RESUMO

Iconic words and signs are characterized by a perceived resemblance between aspects of their form and aspects of their meaning. For example, in English, iconic words include peep and crash, which mimic the sounds they denote, and wiggle and zigzag, which mimic motion. As a semiotic property of words and signs, iconicity has been demonstrated to play a role in word learning, language processing, and language evolution. This paper presents the results of a large-scale norming study for more than 14,000 English words conducted with over 1400 American English speakers. We demonstrate the utility of these ratings by replicating a number of existing findings showing that iconicity ratings are related to age of acquisition, sensory modality, semantic neighborhood density, structural markedness, and playfulness. We discuss possible use cases and limitations of the rating dataset, which is made publicly available.


Assuntos
Idioma , Semântica , Humanos , Desenvolvimento da Linguagem , Aprendizagem Verbal , Som
4.
Behav Brain Sci ; 46: e281, 2023 Sep 28.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37766628

RESUMO

There are towns in which language-of-thought (LoT) is the best game. But do we live in one? I go through three properties that characterize the LoT hypothesis: Discrete constituents, role-filler independence, and logical operators, and argue that in each case predictions from the LoT hypothesis are a poor fit to actual human cognition. As a hypothesis of what human cognition ought to be like, LoT departs from empirical reality.

5.
Psychol Sci ; 33(1): 33-47, 2022 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34939508

RESUMO

We investigated how gender is represented in children's books using a novel 200,000-word corpus comprising 247 popular, contemporary books for young children. Using adult human judgments and word co-occurrence data, we quantified gender biases of words in individual books and in the whole corpus. We found that children's books contain many words that adults judge as gendered. Semantic analyses based on co-occurrence data yielded word clusters related to gender stereotypes (e.g., feminine: emotions; masculine: tools). Co-occurrence data also indicated that many books instantiate gender stereotypes identified in other research (e.g., girls are better at reading, and boys are better at math). Finally, we used large-scale data to estimate the gender distribution of the audience for individual books, and we found that children are more often exposed to stereotypes for their own gender. Together, the data suggest that children's books may be an early source of gender associations and stereotypes.


Assuntos
Livros , Estereotipagem , Adulto , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Matemática , Leitura , Sexismo
6.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 221: 105449, 2022 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35550281

RESUMO

Children's early language knowledge-typically assessed using standardized word comprehension tests or through parental reports-has been positively linked to a variety of later outcomes, from reasoning tests to academic performance to income and health. To better understand the mechanisms behind these links, we examined whether knowledge of certain "seed words"-words with high inductive potential-is positively associated with inductive reasoning. This hypothesis stems from prior work on the effects of language on categorization suggesting that certain words may be important for helping people to deploy categorical hypotheses. Using a longitudinal design, we assessed 36 2- to 4-year-old children's knowledge of 333 words of varying levels of generality (e.g., toy vs. pinwheel, number vs. five). We predicted that adjusting for overall vocabulary, knowledge of more general words (e.g., toy, number) would predict children's performance on inductive reasoning tasks administered 6 months later (i.e., a subset of the Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scales for Early Childhood-Fifth Edition [SB-5] and Woodcock-Johnson Tests of Cognitive Abilities [WJ] concept formation tasks). This prediction was confirmed for one of the measures of inductive reasoning (i.e., the SB-5 but not the WJ) and notably for the task considered to be less reliant on language. Although our experimental design demonstrates only a correlational relationship between seed word knowledge and inductive reasoning ability, our results are consistent with the possibility that early knowledge of certain seed words facilitates performance on putatively nonverbal reasoning tasks.


Assuntos
Linguagem Infantil , Vocabulário , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Humanos , Idioma , Desenvolvimento da Linguagem , Testes de Linguagem
8.
Behav Brain Sci ; 45: e169, 2022 09 13.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36098407

RESUMO

Cognitive scientists and psychometricians are unaccustomed to thinking about culture, often treating their measures - memory, vocabulary, intelligence - as natural kinds. Relying on these measures, behavioral geneticists likewise seem to not wonder about their origin and cultural provenance. I argue that complex human traits - the sort we are most interested in measuring - are cultural products. We can measure them and their heritability, but to conclude that what we have measured is unbound to a time and place is hubris.


Assuntos
Cognição , Inteligência , Humanos
9.
J Vis ; 21(13): 13, 2021 12 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34967860

RESUMO

Human visual recognition is outstandingly robust. People can recognize thousands of object classes in the blink of an eye (50-200 ms) even when the objects vary in position, scale, viewpoint, and illumination. What aspects of human category learning facilitate the extraction of invariant visual features for object recognition? Here, we explore the possibility that a contributing factor to learning such robust visual representations may be a taxonomic hierarchy communicated in part by common labels to which people are exposed as part of natural language. We did this by manipulating the taxonomic level of labels (e.g., superordinate-level [mammal, fruit, vehicle] and basic-level [dog, banana, van]), and the order in which these training labels were used during learning by a Convolutional Neural Network. We found that training the model with hierarchical labels yields visual representations that are more robust to image transformations (e.g., position/scale, illumination, noise, and blur), especially when images were first trained with superordinate labels and then fine-tuned with basic labels. We also found that Superordinate-label followed by Basic-label training best predicts functional magnetic resonance imaging responses in visual cortex and behavioral similarity judgments recorded while viewing naturalistic images. The benefits of training with superordinate labels in the earlier stages of category learning is discussed in the context of representational efficiency and generalization.


Assuntos
Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos , Córtex Visual , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Redes Neurais de Computação , Estimulação Luminosa
10.
Behav Res Methods ; 52(5): 2053-2070, 2020 10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32166609

RESUMO

Does the format in which we experience our moment-to-moment thoughts vary from person to person? Many people claim that their thinking takes place in an inner voice and that using language outside of interpersonal communication is a regular experience for them. Other people disagree. We present a novel measure, the Internal Representation Questionnaire (IRQ) designed to assess people's subjective mode of internal representations, and to quantify individual differences in "modes of thinking" along multiple factors in a single questionnaire. Exploratory factor analysis identified four factors: Internal Verbalization, Visual Imagery, Orthographic Imagery, and Representational Manipulation. All four factors were positively correlated with one another, but accounted for unique predictions. We describe the properties of the IRQ and report a test of its ability to predict patterns of interference in a speeded word-picture verification task. Taken together, the results suggest that self-reported differences in how people internally represent their thoughts relates to differences in processing familiar images and written words.


Assuntos
Idioma , Pensamento , Humanos , Imaginação , Inquéritos e Questionários
11.
Proc Biol Sci ; 285(1874)2018 03 14.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29514962

RESUMO

People have long pondered the evolution of language and the origin of words. Here, we investigate how conventional spoken words might emerge from imitations of environmental sounds. Does the repeated imitation of an environmental sound gradually give rise to more word-like forms? In what ways do these forms resemble the original sounds that motivated them (i.e. exhibit iconicity)? Participants played a version of the children's game 'Telephone'. The first generation of participants imitated recognizable environmental sounds (e.g. glass breaking, water splashing). Subsequent generations imitated the previous generation of imitations for a maximum of eight generations. The results showed that the imitations became more stable and word-like, and later imitations were easier to learn as category labels. At the same time, even after eight generations, both spoken imitations and their written transcriptions could be matched above chance to the category of environmental sound that motivated them. These results show how repeated imitation can create progressively more word-like forms while continuing to retain a resemblance to the original sound that motivated them, and speak to the possible role of human vocal imitation in explaining the origins of at least some spoken words.


Assuntos
Comportamento Imitativo , Idioma , Som , Humanos
12.
Dev Sci ; 21(3): e12572, 2018 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28523758

RESUMO

Iconicity - the correspondence between form and meaning - may help young children learn to use new words. Early-learned words are higher in iconicity than later learned words. However, it remains unclear what role iconicity may play in actual language use. Here, we ask whether iconicity relates not just to the age at which words are acquired, but also to how frequently children and adults use the words in their speech. If iconicity serves to bootstrap word learning, then we would expect that children should say highly iconic words more frequently than less iconic words, especially early in development. We would also expect adults to use iconic words more often when speaking to children than to other adults. We examined the relationship between frequency and iconicity for approximately 2000 English words. Replicating previous findings, we found that more iconic words are learned earlier. Moreover, we found that more iconic words tend to be used more by younger children, and adults use more iconic words when speaking to children than to other adults. Together, our results show that young children not only learn words rated high in iconicity earlier than words low in iconicity, but they also produce these words more frequently in conversation - a pattern that is reciprocated by adults when speaking with children. Thus, the earliest conversations of children are relatively higher in iconicity, suggesting that this iconicity scaffolds the production and comprehension of spoken language during early development.


Assuntos
Compreensão , Desenvolvimento da Linguagem , Fala/fisiologia , Aprendizagem Verbal/fisiologia , Adulto , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Idioma , Masculino
13.
14.
J Neurosci ; 35(25): 9329-35, 2015 Jun 24.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26109657

RESUMO

People use language to shape each other's behavior in highly flexible ways. Effects of language are often assumed to be "high-level" in that, whereas language clearly influences reasoning, decision making, and memory, it does not influence low-level visual processes. Here, we test the prediction that words are able to provide top-down guidance at the very earliest stages of visual processing by acting as powerful categorical cues. We investigated whether visual processing of images of familiar animals and artifacts was enhanced after hearing their name (e.g., "dog") compared with hearing an equally familiar and unambiguous nonverbal sound (e.g., a dog bark) in 14 English monolingual speakers. Because the relationship between words and their referents is categorical, we expected words to deploy more effective categorical templates, allowing for more rapid visual recognition. By recording EEGs, we were able to determine whether this label advantage stemmed from changes to early visual processing or later semantic decision processes. The results showed that hearing a word affected early visual processes and that this modulation was specific to the named category. An analysis of ERPs showed that the P1 was larger when people were cued by labels compared with equally informative nonverbal cues-an enhancement occurring within 100 ms of image onset, which also predicted behavioral responses occurring almost 500 ms later. Hearing labels modulated the P1 such that it distinguished between target and nontarget images, showing that words rapidly guide early visual processing.


Assuntos
Sinais (Psicologia) , Idioma , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Estimulação Acústica , Eletroencefalografia , Potenciais Evocados/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Processamento de Sinais Assistido por Computador
15.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 110(35): 14196-201, 2013 Aug 27.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23940323

RESUMO

Linguistic labels (e.g., "chair") seem to activate visual properties of the objects to which they refer. Here we investigated whether language-based activation of visual representations can affect the ability to simply detect the presence of an object. We used continuous flash suppression to suppress visual awareness of familiar objects while they were continuously presented to one eye. Participants made simple detection decisions, indicating whether they saw any image. Hearing a verbal label before the simple detection task changed performance relative to an uninformative cue baseline. Valid labels improved performance relative to no-label baseline trials. Invalid labels decreased performance. Labels affected both sensitivity (d') and response times. In addition, we found that the effectiveness of labels varied predictably as a function of the match between the shape of the stimulus and the shape denoted by the label. Together, the findings suggest that facilitated detection of invisible objects due to language occurs at a perceptual rather than semantic locus. We hypothesize that when information associated with verbal labels matches stimulus-driven activity, language can provide a boost to perception, propelling an otherwise invisible image into awareness.


Assuntos
Conscientização , Idioma , Visão Ocular , Sinais (Psicologia) , Audição , Humanos , Percepção Visual
16.
Behav Brain Sci ; 39: e251, 2016 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28355864

RESUMO

I applaud Firestone & Scholl (F&S) in calling for more rigor. But, although F&S are correct that some published work on top-down effects suffers from confounds, their sweeping claim that there are no top-down effects on perception is premised on incorrect assumptions. F&S's thesis is wrong. Perception is richly and interestingly influenced by cognition.


Assuntos
Cognição , Percepção , Humanos
17.
Behav Brain Sci ; 39: e66, 2016 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27562311

RESUMO

The Now-or-Never bottleneck has important consequence for understanding why languages have the structures they do. However, not addressed by C&C is that the bottleneck may interact with who is doing the learning: While some languages are mostly learned by infants, others have a large share of adult learners. We argue that such socio-demographic differences extend and qualify C&C's thesis.


Assuntos
Idioma , Aprendizagem , Demografia , Dissidências e Disputas , Humanos , Desenvolvimento da Linguagem
19.
Open Mind (Camb) ; 8: 950-971, 2024.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39170795

RESUMO

What determines whether two people represent something in a similar way? We examined the role of verbal labels in promoting representational alignment. Across two experiments, three groups of participants sorted novel shapes from two visually dissimilar categories. Prior to sorting, participants in two of the groups were pre-exposed to the shapes using a simple visual matching task designed to reinforce the visual category structure. In one of these groups, participants additionally heard one of two nonsense category labels accompanying the shapes. Exposure to these redundant labels led people to represent the shapes in a more categorical way, which led to greater alignment between sorters. We found this effect of label-induced alignment despite the two categories being highly visually distinct and despite participants in both pre-exposure conditions receiving identical visual experience with the shapes. Experiment 2 replicated this basic result using more even more stringent testing conditions. The results hint at the possibly extensive role that labels may play in aligning people's mental representations.

20.
Trends Cogn Sci ; 28(6): 489-491, 2024 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38632006

RESUMO

In a recent paper, Aceves and Evans computed information and semantic density measures for hundreds of languages, and showed that these measures predict the pace and breadth of ideas in communication. Here, we summarize their key findings and situate them in a broader debate about the adaptive nature of language.


Assuntos
Comunicação , Idioma , Humanos , Semântica
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA