Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 109
Filtrar
Mais filtros

Base de dados
Tipo de documento
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Hippocampus ; 28(7): 484-496, 2018 07.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29637657

RESUMO

Previous studies have suggested that spatial navigation can be achieved with at least two distinct learning processes, involving either cognitive map-like representations of the local environment, referred to as the "place strategy", or simple stimulus-response (S-R) associations, the "response strategy". A similar distinction between cognitive/behavioral processes has been made in the context of non-spatial, instrumental conditioning, with the definition of two processes concerning the sensitivity of a given behavior to the expected value of its outcome as well as to the response-outcome contingency ("goal-directed action" and "S-R habit"). Here we investigated whether these two versions of dichotomist definitions of learned behavior, one spatial and the other non-spatial, correspond to each other in a formal way. Specifically, we assessed the goal-directed nature of two navigational strategies, using a combination of an outcome devaluation procedure and a spatial probe trial frequently used to dissociate the two navigational strategies. In Experiment 1, rats trained in a dual-solution T-maze task were subjected to an extinction probe trial from the opposite start arm, with or without prefeeding-induced devaluation of the expected outcome. We found that a non-significant preference for the place strategy in the non-devalued condition was completely reversed after devaluation, such that significantly more animals displayed the use of the response strategy. The result suggests that the place strategy is sensitive to the expected value of the outcome, while the response strategy is not. In Experiment 2, rats with hippocampal lesions showed significant reliance on the response strategy, regardless of whether the expected outcome was devalued or not. The result thus offers further evidence that the response strategy conforms to the definition of an outcome-insensitive, habitual form of instrumental behavior. These results together attest a formal correspondence between two types of dual-process accounts of animal learning and behavior.


Assuntos
Comportamento de Escolha/fisiologia , Condicionamento Operante/fisiologia , Hipocampo/fisiologia , Aprendizagem em Labirinto/fisiologia , Motivação , Navegação Espacial/fisiologia , Animais , Extinção Psicológica , Hipocampo/lesões , Masculino , Ratos
2.
Neurobiol Learn Mem ; 153(Pt B): 118-130, 2018 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29596908

RESUMO

In a discrimination based on magnitude, the same stimulus is presented at two different magnitudes and an outcome, such as food, is signalled by one magnitude but not the other. The review presented in the first part of the article shows that, in general, such a discrimination is acquired more readily when the outcome is signalled by the larger rather than the smaller of the two magnitudes. This asymmetry is observed with magnitudes based on sound, odour, temporal duration, quantity, and physical length. The second part of the article, explores the implications of this pattern of results for the theory of discrimination learning presented by Pearce (1994). The asymmetry found with discriminations based on magnitude contradicts predictions derived from the original version of the theory, but it can be explained by a modified version. The asymmetry also has important implications for understanding how animals represent magnitudes.


Assuntos
Aprendizagem por Discriminação/fisiologia , Discriminação Psicológica/fisiologia , Animais , Estimulação Física/métodos
3.
Learn Behav ; 46(1): 23-37, 2018 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28597217

RESUMO

In three experiments, we investigated the contextual control of attention in human discrimination learning. In each experiment, participants initially received discrimination training in which the cues from Dimension A were relevant in Context 1 but irrelevant in Context 2, whereas the cues from Dimension B were irrelevant in Context 1 but relevant in Context 2. In Experiment 1, the same cues from each dimension were used in Contexts 1 and 2, whereas in Experiments 2 and 3, the cues from each dimension were changed across contexts. In each experiment, participants were subsequently shifted to a transfer discrimination involving novel cues from either dimension, to assess the contextual control of attention. In Experiment 1, measures of eye gaze during the transfer discrimination revealed that Dimension A received more attention than Dimension B in Context 1, whereas the reverse occurred in Context 2. Corresponding results indicating the contextual control of attention were found in Experiments 2 and 3, in which we used the speed of learning (associability) as an indirect marker of learned attentional changes. Implications of our results for current theories of learning and attention are discussed.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Aprendizagem por Discriminação/fisiologia , Transferência de Experiência/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Sinais (Psicologia) , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa , Adulto Jovem
4.
Eur Neurol ; 79(1-2): 64-67, 2018.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29241189

RESUMO

Edgar Douglas Adrian was an outstandingly brilliant, Nobel prize-winning neurophysiologist. He is remembered for developing the all-or-none principle of muscle contraction, and for explaining the minutiae of motor and sensory nerve transmission. He showed that the afferent effect in a neuron depends on the pattern in time of the impulses travelling in it, thereby providing a quantitative basis of nervous behaviour. With Sir Charles Sherrington, he was awarded the Nobel Prize in 1932 for discoveries on the functions of the neurons.


Assuntos
Neurofisiologia/história , Inglaterra , História do Século XX , Humanos , Prêmio Nobel
5.
Eur Neurol ; 79(3-4): 211-213, 2018.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29597201

RESUMO

Modern technologies have, to some degree, replaced the careful elicitation of neurological physical signs. Many 20th century texts and monographs were devoted to such clinical phenomena. Foremost among them were the assiduous writings of Robert Wartenberg who fled Hitler to enhance Neurology in San Francisco. His work and influences are outlined here.


Assuntos
Neurologia/história , História do Século XX , Humanos
6.
Eur Neurol ; 78(3-4): 196-199, 2017.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28898886

RESUMO

In the well-known story of the illness of King George III, what is often overlooked is the part played by Dr. Francis Willis, an inconspicuous doctor who with great success ran an asylum in Lincolnshire. In November 1788, he was called to attend the King whose mania was becoming uncontrollable. Because of his interventions there was a slow but marked improvement. The King's recovery in 1789 increased Willis' reputation and expanded his practice.


Assuntos
Transtorno Bipolar/história , Pessoas Famosas , História do Século XVIII , História do Século XIX , Hospitais Psiquiátricos , Humanos , Masculino , Médicos/história
7.
Eur Neurol ; 77(5-6): 303-306, 2017.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28456809

RESUMO

Samuel Thomas Soemmerring was a Prussian polymathic doctor with remarkable achievements in anatomy, draftsmanship and inventions. His naming of 12 pairs of cranial nerves in his graduation thesis is of particular importance. He also gave original descriptions of the macula, sensory pathways and of the substantia nigra. His non-medical contributions were diverse and included criticism of the guillotine, invention of a telegraphic system, and discoveries in palaeontology.


Assuntos
Nervos Cranianos , Neuroanatomia/história , História do Século XVIII , História do Século XIX , Humanos , Masculino
8.
Virol J ; 13: 55, 2016 Mar 31.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27036114

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Eurasian-origin and intercontinental reassortant highly pathogenic (HP) influenza A viruses (IAVs) were first detected in North America in wild, captive, and domestic birds during November-December 2014. Detections of HP viruses in wild birds in the contiguous United States and southern Canadian provinces continued into winter and spring of 2015 raising concerns that migratory birds could potentially disperse viruses to more northerly breeding areas where they could be maintained to eventually seed future poultry outbreaks. RESULTS: We sampled 1,129 wild birds on the Yukon-Kuskokwim Delta, Alaska, one of the largest breeding areas for waterfowl in North America, during spring and summer of 2015 to test for Eurasian lineage and intercontinental reassortant HP H5 IAVs and potential progeny viruses. We did not detect HP IAVs in our sample collection from western Alaska; however, we isolated five low pathogenic (LP) viruses. Four isolates were of the H6N1 (n = 2), H6N2, and H9N2 combined subtypes whereas the fifth isolate was a mixed infection that included H3 and N7 gene segments. Genetic characterization of these five LP IAVs isolated from cackling (Branta hutchinsii; n = 2) and greater white-fronted geese (Anser albifrons; n = 3), revealed three viral gene segments sharing high nucleotide identity with HP H5 viruses recently detected in North America. Additionally, one of the five isolates was comprised of multiple Eurasian lineage gene segments. CONCLUSIONS: Our results did not provide direct evidence for circulation of HP IAVs in the Yukon-Kuskokwim Delta region of Alaska during spring and summer of 2015. Prevalence and genetic characteristics of LP IAVs during the sampling period are concordant with previous findings of relatively low viral prevalence in geese during spring, non-detection of IAVs in geese during summer, and evidence for intercontinental exchange of viruses in western Alaska.


Assuntos
Genótipo , Vírus da Influenza A/classificação , Influenza Aviária/epidemiologia , Influenza Aviária/virologia , Vírus Reordenados/classificação , Alaska/epidemiologia , Animais , Aves , Monitoramento Epidemiológico , Vírus da Influenza A/genética , Vírus da Influenza A/isolamento & purificação , Influenza Aviária/transmissão , Epidemiologia Molecular , Vírus Reordenados/genética , Vírus Reordenados/isolamento & purificação , Análise de Sequência de DNA
9.
Eur Neurol ; 76(3-4): 175-181, 2016.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27658273

RESUMO

This historical essay outlines early ideas and clinical accounts of hysteria. It reproduces verbatim parts of a remarkable text of Thomas Sydenham. This provides the most detailed description of hysterical symptoms, contemporary treatment and particularly Sydenham's opinions about the nature of the disorder. His portrayal is compared to later and modern concepts and classification.


Assuntos
Transtornos Dissociativos/história , Histeria/história , Transtornos Somatoformes/história , Inglaterra , História do Século XVII , Humanos
10.
Proc Biol Sci ; 282(1802)2015 Mar 07.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25608880

RESUMO

Social learning strategies (SLSs) are rules specifying the conditions in which it would be adaptive for animals to copy the behaviour of others rather than to persist with a previously established behaviour or to acquire a new behaviour through asocial learning. In behavioural ecology, cultural evolutionary theory and economics, SLSs are studied using a 'phenotypic gambit'-from a purely functional perspective, without reference to their underlying psychological mechanisms. However, SLSs are described in these fields as if they were implemented by complex, domain-specific, genetically inherited mechanisms of decision-making. In this article, we suggest that it is time to begin investigating the psychology of SLSs, and we initiate this process by examining recent experimental work relating to three groups of strategies: copy when alternative unsuccessful, copy when model successful and copy the majority. In each case, we argue that the reported behaviour could have been mediated by domain-general and taxonomically general psychological mechanisms; specifically, by mechanisms, identified through conditioning experiments, that make associative learning selective. We also suggest experimental manipulations that could be used in future research to resolve more fully the question whether, in non-human animals, SLSs are mediated by domain-general or domain-specific psychological mechanisms.


Assuntos
Comportamento Animal , Comportamento Imitativo , Aprendizagem , Comportamento Social , Animais , Aprendizado Social
11.
Virol J ; 12: 151, 2015 Sep 28.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26411256

RESUMO

It is unknown how the current Asian origin highly pathogenic avian influenza H5 viruses arrived, but these viruses are now poised to become endemic in North America. Wild birds harbor these viruses and have dispersed them at regional scales. What is unclear is how the viruses may be moving from the wild bird reservoir into poultry holdings. Active surveillance of live wild birds is likely the best way to determine the true distribution of these viruses. We also suggest that sampling be focused on regions with the greatest risk for poultry losses and attempt to define the mechanisms of transfer to enhance biosecurity. Responding to the recent outbreaks of highly pathogenic avian influenza in North America requires an efficient plan with clear objectives and potential management outcomes.


Assuntos
Monitoramento Epidemiológico , Glicoproteínas de Hemaglutininação de Vírus da Influenza/análise , Vírus da Influenza A/isolamento & purificação , Influenza Aviária/epidemiologia , Influenza Aviária/virologia , Animais , Aves , América do Norte/epidemiologia , Sorogrupo
12.
Learn Behav ; 43(1): 72-82, 2015 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25537840

RESUMO

Pearce, Dopson, Haselgrove, and Esber (Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 38, 167-179, 2012) conducted a series of experiments with rats and pigeons in which the conditioned responding elicited by two types of redundant cue was compared. One of these redundant cues was a blocked cue X from A+ AX+ training, whereas the other was cue Y from a simple discrimination BY+ CY-. Greater conditioned responding was elicited by X than by Y; we refer to this difference as the redundancy effect. To test an explanation of this effect in terms of comparator theory (Denniston, Savastano, & Miller, 2001), a single group of rats in Experiment 1 received training of the form A+ AX+ BY+ CY-, followed by an A- Y+ discrimination. Responding to the individual cues was tested both before and after the latter discrimination. In addition to a replication of the redundancy effect during the earlier test, we observed stronger responding to B than to X, both during the earlier test and, in contradiction of the theory, after the A- Y+ discrimination. In Experiment 2, a blocking group received A+ AX+, a continuous group received AX+ BX-, and a partial group received AX± BX± training. Subsequent tests with X again demonstrated the redundancy effect, but also revealed a stronger response in the partial than in the continuous group. This pattern of results is difficult to explain with error-correction theories that assume that stimuli compete for associative strength during conditioning. We suggest, instead, that the influence of a redundant cue is determined by its relationship with the event with which it is paired, and by the attention it is paid.


Assuntos
Aprendizagem por Associação , Sinais (Psicologia) , Aprendizagem por Discriminação , Animais , Masculino , Ratos
13.
Eur Neurol ; 74(1-2): 18-21, 2015.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26111492

RESUMO

This paper sketches the early history of pituitary apoplexy, a disorder later fully described in 1950 by Brougham, Heusner and Adams. Haemorrhage or necrosis in an adenoma causes a characteristic sudden drowsiness, stupor or coma, headache and stiff neck, ocular palsy, and impaired acuity with visual field loss owing to optic nerve or chiasmal compression. The associated endocrinopathy and management are described.


Assuntos
Neurologia/história , Apoplexia Hipofisária/história , História do Século XVI , História do Século XVII , História do Século XVIII , História do Século XIX , História do Século XX , Humanos , Ilustração Médica/história
14.
Hippocampus ; 24(12): 1633-52, 2014 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25131441

RESUMO

Rats with lesions of the hippocampus or sham lesions were required in four experiments to escape from a square swimming pool by finding a submerged platform. Experiments 1 and 2 commenced with passive training in which rats were repeatedly placed on the platform in one corner-the correct corner-of a pool with distinctive walls. A test trial then revealed a strong preference for the correct corner in the sham but not the hippocampal group. Subsequent active training of being required to swim to the platform resulted in both groups acquiring a preference for the correct corner in the two experiments. In Experiments 3 and 4, rats were required to solve a discrimination between different panels pasted to the walls of the pool, by swimming to the middle of a correct panel. Hippocampal lesions prevented a discrimination being formed between panels of different lengths (Experiment 3), but not between panels showing lines of different orientations (Experiment 4); rats with sham lesions mastered both problems. It is suggested that an intact hippocampus is necessary for the formation of stimulus-goal associations that permit successful passive spatial leaning. It is further suggested that an intact hippocampus is not necessary for the formation of stimulus-response associations, except when they involve information about length or distance.


Assuntos
Aprendizagem por Associação/fisiologia , Discriminação Psicológica/fisiologia , Hipocampo/fisiologia , Aprendizagem Espacial/fisiologia , Animais , Agonistas de Aminoácidos Excitatórios/toxicidade , Objetivos , Hipocampo/efeitos dos fármacos , Hipocampo/fisiopatologia , Ácido Ibotênico/toxicidade , Masculino , Atividade Motora/fisiologia , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Orientação , Ratos , Natação/fisiologia
15.
Front Ecol Environ ; 12(10): 548-556, 2014 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32313510

RESUMO

Climate-related environmental changes have increasingly been linked to emerging infectious diseases in wildlife. The Arctic is facing a major ecological transition that is expected to substantially affect animal and human health. Changes in phenology or environmental conditions that result from climate warming may promote novel species assemblages as host and pathogen ranges expand to previously unoccupied areas. Recent evidence from the Arctic and subarctic suggests an increase in the spread and prevalence of some wildlife diseases, but baseline data necessary to detect and verify such changes are still lacking. Wild birds are undergoing rapid shifts in distribution and have been implicated in the spread of wildlife and zoonotic diseases. Here, we review evidence of current and projected changes in the abundance and distribution of avian diseases and outline strategies for future research. We discuss relevant climatic and environmental factors, emerging host-pathogen contact zones, the relationship between host condition and immune function, and potential wildlife and human health outcomes in northern regions.

16.
Hippocampus ; 23(12): 1162-78, 2013 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23749378

RESUMO

Three cohorts of rats with extensive hippocampal lesions received multiple tests to examine the relationships between particular forms of associative learning and an influential account of hippocampal function (the cognitive map hypothesis). Hippocampal lesions spared both the ability to discriminate two different digging media and to discriminate two different room locations in a go/no-go task when each location was approached from a single direction. Hippocampal lesions had, however, differential effects on a more complex task (biconditional discrimination) where the correct response was signaled by the presence or absence of specific cues. For all biconditional tasks, digging in one medium (A) was rewarded in the presence of cue C, while digging in medium B was rewarded in the presences of cue D. Such biconditional tasks are "configural" as no individual cue or element predicts the solution (AC+, AD-, BD+, and BC-). When proximal context cues signaled the correct digging choice, biconditional learning was seemingly unaffected by hippocampal lesions. Severe deficits occurred, however, when the correct digging choice was signaled by distal room cues. Also, impaired was the ability to discriminate two locations when each location was approached from two directions. A task demand that predicted those tasks impaired by hippocampal damage was the need to combine specific cues with their relative spatial positions ("structural learning"). This ability makes it possible to distinguish the same cues set in different spatial arrays. Thus, the hippocampus appears necessary for configural discriminations involving structure, discriminations that potentially underlie the creation of cognitive maps.


Assuntos
Aprendizagem por Associação/fisiologia , Discriminação Psicológica , Hipocampo/fisiologia , Inibição Psicológica , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Análise de Variância , Animais , Comportamento de Escolha/fisiologia , Estudos de Coortes , Sinais (Psicologia) , Hipocampo/lesões , Masculino , Aprendizagem em Labirinto , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Ratos
17.
NMR Biomed ; 26(9): 1152-7, 2013 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23401319

RESUMO

In previous work at 4.7 T, the individual components of biexponential (7) Li transverse (T2 ) spin relaxation in rat brain in vivo were tentatively identified with intra- and extracellular Li. The goal in this work was to estimate Li's compartmental distribution as a function of total Li concentration in brain from the biexponential decays. Here a localized, biexponential (7) Li T2 MR spin-relaxation study with isotopically enriched (7) LiCl is reported in rat brain in vivo at 7 T. Additionally, a simple linear interpolation using the biexponential T2 values to estimate intracellular Li from individual monoexponential T2 decays was assessed. Intracellular T2 was 14.8 ± 4.3 ms and extracellular T2 was 295 ± 61 ms. The fraction of intracellular brain Li ranged from 37.3 to 64.8% (mean 54.5 ± 6.7%) and did not correlate with total Li concentration. The estimated intracellular Li concentration ranged from 47 to 80% (mean 68.3 ± 8.5%) of the total brain Li concentration and was highly correlated with it. The monoexponential estimates of the intracellular-Li fractions and derived concentrations averaged about 15% higher than the corresponding biexponential estimates. This work supports the previous conclusion that a large fraction of Li in the brain is within the intracellular compartment.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/metabolismo , Lítio/metabolismo , Espectroscopia de Ressonância Magnética , Animais , Encéfalo/citologia , Lítio/análise , Masculino , Prótons , Ratos , Ratos Sprague-Dawley
18.
MAGMA ; 26(3): 337-43, 2013 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23053715

RESUMO

OBJECT: The relative amounts of choline (Cho), phosphocholine (PC), and glycerophosphocholine (GPC) may be sensitive indicators of breast cancer and the degree of malignancy. Here we implement some simple modifications to a previously developed (1)H NMR analysis of fine-needle-aspirate (FNA) biopsies designed to yield sufficient spectral resolution of Cho, PC, and GPC for usable relative quantitation of these metabolites. MATERIALS AND METHODS: FNA biopsies of eighteen breast lesions were examined using our modified procedure for direct (1)H NMR at 400 MHz. Resonances of choline metabolites and potential interferences were fit using the computer program NUTS. RESULTS: Quantitation of PC, GPC, and Cho relative to each other and to (phospho)creatine was obtained for eleven confirmed cases of infiltrating ductal carcinoma. Reliable results could not be obtained for the remaining cases primarily due to interference from lidocaine anesthetic. CONCLUSION: Some simple modifications of a previously developed (1)H NMR analysis of FNAs yielded sufficient spectral resolution of Cho, PC, and GPC to permit usable relative quantitation at 400 MHz. In 9 of the 11 quantified cases the sum of GPC and Cho exceeded 42 % of the total choline-metabolite peak area.


Assuntos
Biomarcadores Tumorais/análise , Biomarcadores Tumorais/metabolismo , Biópsia por Agulha Fina/métodos , Neoplasias da Mama/metabolismo , Colina/análise , Colina/metabolismo , Diagnóstico por Computador/métodos , Neoplasias da Mama/patologia , Feminino , Humanos , Prótons , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Sensibilidade e Especificidade
19.
Mol Ecol ; 21(14): 3562-75, 2012 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22582867

RESUMO

Genetic studies of waterfowl (Anatidae) have observed the full spectrum of mitochondrial (mt) DNA population divergence, from apparent panmixia to deep, reciprocally monophyletic lineages. Yet, these studies often found weak or no nuclear (nu) DNA structure, which was often attributed to male-biased gene flow, a common behaviour within this family. An alternative explanation for this 'conflict' is that the smaller effective population size and faster sorting rate of mtDNA relative to nuDNA lead to different signals of population structure. We tested these alternatives by sequencing 12 nuDNA introns for a Holarctic pair of waterfowl subspecies, the European goosander (Mergus merganser merganser) and the North American common merganser (M. m. americanus), which exhibit strong population structure in mtDNA. We inferred effective population sizes, gene flow and divergence times from published mtDNA sequences and simulated expected differentiation for nuDNA based on those histories. Between Europe and North America, nuDNA Ф(ST) was 3.4-fold lower than mtDNA Ф(ST) , a result consistent with differences in sorting rates. However, despite geographically structured and monophyletic mtDNA lineages within continents, nuDNA Ф(ST) values were generally zero and significantly lower than predicted. This between- and within-continent contrast held when comparing mtDNA and nuDNA among published studies of ducks. Thus, male-mediated gene flow is a better explanation than slower sorting rates for limited nuDNA differentiation within continents, which is also supported by nonmolecular data. This study illustrates the value of quantitatively testing discrepancies between mtDNA and nuDNA to reject the null hypothesis that conflict simply reflects different sorting rates.


Assuntos
Núcleo Celular/genética , DNA Mitocondrial/genética , Patos/genética , Fluxo Gênico , Animais , Europa (Continente) , Variação Genética , Genética Populacional , Íntrons , Masculino , Dados de Sequência Molecular , América do Norte , Densidade Demográfica , Análise de Sequência de DNA
20.
Learn Behav ; 40(3): 241-54, 2012 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22926999

RESUMO

A formal account of the relationship between attention and associative learning is presented within the framework of a configural theory of discrimination learning. The account is based on a connectionist network in which the entire pattern of stimulation presented on a trial activates a configural unit that then enters into an association with the trial outcome. Attention is assumed to have two roles within this network. First, the salience of the stimuli at the input to the network can be increased if they are relevant to the occurrence of reinforcement and decreased if they are irrelevant. Second, the associability of configural units can increase on trials when the outcome is surprising and decrease when the outcome is not surprising.


Assuntos
Aprendizagem por Associação , Atenção , Teoria Psicológica , Animais , Condicionamento Clássico , Discriminação Psicológica , Humanos , Modelos Psicológicos , Redes Neurais de Computação
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA