RESUMO
The hippocampus and parahippocampal region are essential for representing episodic memories involving various spatial locations and objects, and for using those memories for future adaptive behavior. The "dual-stream model" was initially formulated based on anatomical characteristics of the medial temporal lobe, dividing the parahippocampal region into two streams that separately process and relay spatial and nonspatial information to the hippocampus. Despite its significance, the dual-stream model in its original form cannot explain recent experimental results, and many researchers have recognized the need for a modification of the model. Here, we argue that dividing the parahippocampal region into spatial and nonspatial streams a priori may be too simplistic, particularly in light of ambiguous situations in which a sensory cue alone (e.g., visual scene) may not allow such a definitive categorization. Upon reviewing evidence, including our own, that reveals the importance of goal-directed behavioral responses in determining the relative involvement of the parahippocampal processing streams, we propose the Goal-directed Interaction of Stimulus and Task-demand (GIST) model. In the GIST model, input stimuli such as visual scenes and objects are first processed by both the postrhinal and perirhinal cortices-the postrhinal cortex more heavily involved with visual scenes and perirhinal cortex with objects-with relatively little dependence on behavioral task demand. However, once perceptual ambiguities are resolved and the scenes and objects are identified and recognized, the information is then processed through the medial or lateral entorhinal cortex, depending on whether it is used to fulfill navigational or non-navigational goals, respectively. As complex sensory stimuli are utilized for both navigational and non-navigational purposes in an intermixed fashion in naturalistic settings, the hippocampus may be required to then put together these experiences into a coherent map to allow flexible cognitive operations for adaptive behavior to occur.
Assuntos
Objetivos , Córtex Perirrinal , Córtex Entorrinal/fisiologia , Hipocampo/fisiologia , Vias Neurais/fisiologia , Giro Para-Hipocampal/fisiologia , Córtex Perirrinal/fisiologia , Lobo Temporal/fisiologiaAssuntos
Espasmo Brônquico/terapia , Oxigenação por Membrana Extracorpórea/métodos , Stents , Estenose Traqueal/terapia , Idoso , Espasmo Brônquico/complicações , Feminino , Humanos , Imageamento Tridimensional , Recidiva , Tomografia Computadorizada por Raios X , Estenose Traqueal/complicações , TraqueostomiaRESUMO
The fasciola cinereum (FC) is a subregion of the hippocampus that has received relatively little attention compared with other hippocampal subregions with respect to anatomical characteristics and functional significance. Here, we show that the FC exhibits clear anatomical borders with the distalmost region of the CA1. Principal neurons in the FC resemble the granule cells in the dentate gyrus (DG). However, adult neurogenesis was not found unlike in the DG. The FC receives inputs mostly from the lateral entorhinal cortex and perirhinal cortex while projecting exclusively to the crest of the DG within the hippocampus. Neurotoxic lesions in the FC using colchicine impaired the acquisition, but not retrieval, of visual contextual memory in rats. FC lesions also impaired place recognition and object-in-place memory. As the rat performed the contextual memory task on the T-maze, place cells in the FC exhibited robust place fields and were indiscriminable from those in CA1 with respect to the basic firing properties. However, place cells in the FC fired only transiently in their place fields on the maze compared with those in CA1. Our findings suggest that the episodic firing patterns of the place cells in the FC may play critical roles in learning a novel contextual environment by facilitating temoporally structured contextual pattern separation in the DG of the hippocampus.
Assuntos
Fasciola , Animais , Hipocampo/fisiologia , Humanos , Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Memória/fisiologia , Neurônios/fisiologia , RatosAssuntos
Reanimação Cardiopulmonar/métodos , Oxigenação por Membrana Extracorpórea , Hipotermia Induzida , Síndrome do QT Longo/complicações , Parada Cardíaca Extra-Hospitalar/terapia , Adolescente , Terapia Combinada , Feminino , Humanos , Síndrome do QT Longo/congênito , Parada Cardíaca Extra-Hospitalar/etiologiaRESUMO
Objective: Humans interpret sensory inputs based on actual stimuli and expectations of the stimuli. We investigated whether manipulating information related to the physiological response could change the somatosensory experience of acupuncture. Methods: Twenty-four participants received tactile stimulations with a von Frey filament on the left arm. Participants were informed that they would receive acupuncture stimulations at different angles while they were presented with changes in their peripheral blood flow (PBF) measured with Laser Doppler perfusion imaging. However, in reality, they were observing premade pseudo-biosignal images (six sessions: one circular, two rectangular elongated, two diagonally elongated, and one cross-fixation [control] shape). After each session, the participants reported the intensity and location of the de qi sensations perceived on their arm using a bodily sensation mapping tool. The spatial patterns of the somatic sensations were visualized using statistical parametric mapping. The F1 score was calculated to measure the similarity between the presented pseudo-biosignals and reported de qi response images. Results: The spatial configurations of the presented pseudo-biosignal images and de qi response images were similar. The rectangular elongated pseudo-biosignal shape had a significantly higher F1 score compared to the control. All tactile stimulations produced similar levels of enhanced PBF regardless of the pseudo-biosignal shape. Conclusion: The spatial configurations of somatic sensations changed according to the presented pseudo-biosignal shape, suggesting that expectations of the physiological response to acupuncture stimulation can influence the perceived somatic sensation.
RESUMO
How visual scene memory is processed differentially by the upstream structures of the hippocampus is largely unknown. We sought to dissociate functionally the lateral and medial subdivisions of the entorhinal cortex (LEC and MEC, respectively) in visual scene-dependent tasks by temporarily inactivating the LEC and MEC in the same rat. When the rat made spatial choices in a T-maze using visual scenes displayed on LCD screens, the inactivation of the MEC but not the LEC produced severe deficits in performance. However, when the task required the animal to push a jar or to dig in the sand in the jar using the same scene stimuli, the LEC but not the MEC became important. Our findings suggest that the entorhinal cortex is critical for scene-dependent mnemonic behavior, and the response modality may interact with a sensory modality to determine the involvement of the LEC and MEC in scene-based memory tasks.
Assuntos
Comportamento de Escolha , Córtex Entorrinal/fisiologia , Comportamento Espacial , Animais , Vias Neurais/fisiologia , RatosRESUMO
OBJECTIVES: To evaluate the incidence of preauricular sinus, the association between preauricular sinus of a parent and their children, and the odds of hearing impairments in patients with preauricular sinus. METHODS: A cross-sectional study was performed using data from the Korea National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. We evaluated data from 23,533 subjects who were interviewed between 2010 and 2012. RESULTS: The incidence of unilateral preauricular sinus was 1.3% and that of bilateral preauricular sinus was 0.3%. Female gender was statistically associated with preauricular sinus (adjusted odd ratio, AOR=1.401). There was no statistical difference between right- and left-sided preauricular sinus. There was an association between parents and their children for bilateral preauricular sinus (AOR of father's bilateral preauricular sinus=35.711; AOR of mother's bilateral preauricular sinus=7.683), but there was no association found for unilateral preauricular sinus. There was no link found between hearing impairment and preauricular sinus. CONCLUSION: This large population-based study provides reliable information about the incidence of preauricular sinus, the association between preauricular sinus in parents and their children, and the lack of an association with hearing impairment; these results can help clinicians in the management of their patients.
Assuntos
Anormalidades Craniofaciais/epidemiologia , Anormalidades Craniofaciais/genética , Predisposição Genética para Doença , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Anormalidades Craniofaciais/complicações , Estudos Transversais , Feminino , Perda Auditiva/epidemiologia , Perda Auditiva/etiologia , Humanos , Incidência , Lactente , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Pais , Linhagem , República da Coreia/epidemiologia , Fatores Sexuais , Adulto JovemRESUMO
OBJECTIVES: To correlate Frey's syndrome with subjective symptoms, Minor's starch iodine test results, and infrared thermography measurements, and to discuss the utility of thermography as a quantitative diagnostic method. METHODS: This study included 59 patients who underwent unilateral parotidectomy. A subjective clinical questionnaire and an objective Minor's starch iodine test were performed to evaluate the incidence of Frey's syndrome. Infrared thermography was performed, and the subjects were divided into seven groups according to the temperature differences between operated and unoperated sites. The thermal differences were correlated with the results from Minor's starch iodine test and the subjective symptoms questionnaire. RESULTS: Of the 59 patients, 20 patients (33.9%) reported subjective symptoms after eating; 30 patients (50.8%) tested positive for Minor's starch iodine test, 19 patients (63.3%) of which reported subjective symptoms. Of the 29 patients who were negative for the iodine test, 2 patients (6.9%) reported subjective symptoms. Thus, subjective symptoms were well correlated with Minor's starch iodine test (r=0.589, P<0.001). As the thermal differences with infrared thermography increased, the number of patients with subjective symptoms increased (χ(2)=22.5, P<0.001). Using infrared thermography, the mean temperature difference in the positive group for the iodine test was 0.82â±0.26â, and that in the negative group was 0.10â±0.47â. With increased thermal differences, more patients showed positivity in the iodine test (χ(2)=29.9, P<0.001). CONCLUSION: Subjective symptoms, Minor's starch iodine test, and infrared thermography are well correlated with one another. Quantitative thermography provides clues for the wide variation in the incidence of Frey's syndrome, and could be a useful method for diagnosing and studying Frey's syndrome.