Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 5 de 5
Filtrar
Más filtros










Base de datos
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
J Pers ; 80(2): 503-36, 2012 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21299562

RESUMEN

The authors examined how conciliatory gestures exhibited in response to interpersonal transgressions influence forgiveness and feelings of friendship with the transgressor. In Study 1, 163 undergraduates who had recently been harmed were examined longitudinally. Conciliatory gestures exhibited by transgressors predicted higher rates of forgiveness over 21 days, and this relationship was mediated by victims' perceptions of their transgressors' Agreeableness. Study 2 was an experiment including 145 undergraduates who experienced a breach in trust from an anonymous partner during an iterated prisoner's dilemma. When transgressors apologized and offered financial compensation, participants reported higher levels of forgiveness and feelings of friendship when compared to a control condition and an aggravating condition. The effects of apology/compensation on forgiveness and perceived friendship were mediated by victims' perceptions of their transgressors' Agreeableness. Results suggest that conciliatory gestures promote forgiveness in part by depicting transgressors as more sympathetic, considerate, fair, and just (i.e., agreeable).


Asunto(s)
Conflicto Psicológico , Perdón , Amigos , Relaciones Interpersonales , Negociación , Confianza , Adulto , Emociones , Femenino , Humanos , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino , Motivación , Satisfacción Personal , Personalidad , Estudiantes/psicología , Adulto Joven
2.
Psychol Assess ; 23(2): 311-24, 2011 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21443367

RESUMEN

Before the developmental trajectory, outcomes, and related interventions of gratitude can be accurately and confidently studied among the youth, researchers must ensure that they have psychometrically sound measures of gratitude that are suitable for this population. Thus, considering that no known scales were specifically designed to measure gratitude in youth, this study aimed to answer an important question: Are the existing gratitude scales used with adults valid for use with youth? The present study is an empirical investigation, based on a large youth sample (N = 1,405) with ages ranging from 10 to 19 years old, of the psychometric properties of scores of the Gratitude Questionnaire-6 (GQ-6; M. E. McCullough, R. A. Emmons, & J.-A. Tsang, 2002), the Gratitude Adjective Checklist (GAC; M. E. McCullough, R. A. Emmons, & J.-A. Tsang, 2002), and the Gratitude Resentment and Appreciation Test (GRAT)-short form (M. Thomas & P. Watkins, 2003). Single-group and multiple-group confirmatory factor analyses indicated that the factor structures of these gratitude scales resemble those found with adults and were invariant across age groups. Scores of all three gratitude scales revealed acceptable internal consistency estimates (i.e., >.70) across age groups. Results showed that whereas scores of all three gratitude scales were positively correlated with each other for 14- to 19-year-olds, GRAT-short form scores tended to display relatively low correlations with scores of the other two measures for younger children (10-13 years old). Furthermore, the nomological network analysis showed that scores of all three gratitude scales were positively correlated with positive affect and life satisfaction scores across the age groups. The relationships with negative affect and depression scores, however, seemed dependent on the child's age. Pending results from subsequent research recommendations for researchers interested in studying gratitude in youth are offered.


Asunto(s)
Emociones , Pruebas Psicológicas , Adolescente , Afecto , Factores de Edad , Niño , Análisis Factorial , Humanos , Masculino , Satisfacción Personal , Pruebas Psicológicas/normas , Pruebas Psicológicas/estadística & datos numéricos , Psicología del Adolescente , Psicometría , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Adulto Joven
3.
Emotion ; 10(3): 358-76, 2010 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20515225

RESUMEN

In two studies, the authors sought to identify the mathematical function underlying the temporal course of forgiveness. A logarithmic model outperformed linear, exponential, power, hyperbolic, and exponential-power models. The logarithmic function implies a psychological process yielding diminishing returns, corresponds to the Weber-Fechner law, and is functionally similar to the power law underlying the psychophysical function (Stevens, 1971) and the forgetting function (Wixted & Ebbesen, 1997). By 3 months after their transgressions, the typical participant's forgiveness had increased by two log-odds units. Individual differences in rates of change were correlated with robust predictors of forgiveness. Consistent with evolutionary theorizing (McCullough, 2008), Study 2 revealed that forgiveness was uniquely associated with participants' perceptions that their relationships with their offenders retained value.


Asunto(s)
Relaciones Interpersonales , Modelos Psicológicos , Emociones , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Motivación , Dinámicas no Lineales , Personalidad , Pruebas Psicológicas , Factores de Tiempo , Adulto Joven
4.
Pers Soc Psychol Bull ; 34(2): 182-95, 2008 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18063835

RESUMEN

In two studies, the authors investigated the associations between interpersonal forgiveness and psychological well-being. Cross-sectional and prospective multilevel analyses demonstrated that increases in forgiveness (measured as fluctuations in individuals' avoidance, revenge, and benevolence motivations toward their transgressors) were related to within-persons increases in psychological well-being (measured as more satisfaction with life, more positive mood, less negative mood, and fewer physical symptoms). Moreover, forgiveness was more strongly linked to well-being for people who reported being closer and more committed to their partners before the transgression and for people who reported that their partners apologized and made amends for the transgression. Evidence for the reverse causal model, that increases in well-being were related to increases in forgiveness, was also found. However, changes in feelings of closeness toward the partner appeared to account for the associations of forgiveness with well-being, but not vice versa.


Asunto(s)
Beneficencia , Relaciones Interpersonales , Satisfacción Personal , Adolescente , Adulto , Estudios Transversales , Femenino , Florida , Humanos , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino , Estudios Prospectivos , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
5.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 92(3): 490-505, 2007 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17352605

RESUMEN

In 3 studies, the authors investigated whether within-persons increases in rumination about an interpersonal transgression were associated with within-persons reductions in forgiveness. Results supported this hypothesis. The association of transient increases in rumination with transient reductions in forgiveness appeared to be mediated by anger, but not fear, toward the transgressor. The association of rumination and forgiveness was not confounded by daily fluctuations in positive affect and negative affect, and it was not moderated by trait levels of positive affectivity, negative affectivity, or perceived hurtfulness of the transgression. Cross-lagged associations of rumination and forgiveness in Study 3 more consistently supported the proposition that increased rumination precedes reductions in forgiveness than the proposition that increased forgiveness precedes reductions in rumination.


Asunto(s)
Afecto , Actitud , Relaciones Interpersonales , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino , Modelos Psicológicos , Motivación , Factores de Tiempo
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA