Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 96
Filtrar
1.
Psychophysiology ; 38(2): 343-58, 2001 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11347879

RESUMEN

We used a novel application of principal components analysis (spatiotemporal PCA) to decompose the event-related brain potentials (ERPs) obtained with a dense electrode array, with the purpose of elucidating the late ERP components elicited by deviant stimuli under "attend" and "ignore" conditions. First, a "spatial" PCA was performed to identify a set of scalp distributions (spatial factors or "virtual electrodes") that accounted for the spatial variance in the data set. The data were expressed as spatial factor scores or "virtual ERPs" measured at each of the virtual electrodes. These virtual ERPs were submitted to a "temporal" PCA, yielding a set of temporal factors or "virtual epochs." Statistical analyses of the temporal factor scores found that (1) attended deviant stimuli elicited the P300 and Novelty P3 components, the latter being largest for highly salient nontargets: (2) "ignored" deviants elicited a small Novelty P3, and depending on the primary task, a small P300: and (3) the classical Slow Wave consisted of separate frontal-negative and posterior-positive components.


Asunto(s)
Electroencefalografía , Potenciales Relacionados con Evento P300/fisiología , Adulto , Atención/fisiología , Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Electrodos , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
2.
Brain Res Cogn Brain Res ; 11(2): 305-23, 2001 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11275491

RESUMEN

The present study investigates the processes involved in the recovery from temporarily ambiguous garden-path sentences. Event-related brain potentials (ERP) were recorded while subjects read German subject-object ambiguous relative and complement clauses. As both clause types are initially analyzed as subject-first structures, object-first structures require a revision which is more difficult for complement than for relative clauses. The hypothesis is tested that the revision process consists of two sub-processes, namely diagnosis and actual reanalysis. Applying a spatio-temporal principal component analysis to the ERP data, distinct positive sub-components presumably reflecting different sub-processes could be identified in the time range of the P300 and P600. It will be argued that the P600 is not a monolithic component, and that different sub-processes may be involved at varying time points depending on the type of garden-path sentence.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados , Lenguaje , Procesos Mentales/fisiología , Adulto , Electroencefalografía , Potenciales Relacionados con Evento P300 , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Tiempo de Reacción , Estadística como Asunto
3.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 12(2): 120-30, 2001 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11169876

RESUMEN

The P300 and Novelty P3 are positive components of the event related brain potential (ERP) with a latency of at least 300 ms, which are manifestations of brain activity evoked by deviant events. Spencer et al. [1999, 2001] demonstrated that these are two distinct components, both of which may be elicited, with different amplitudes, by both rare and novel events. However, the locations of the intracranial sources of the components remain unknown. We describe the application of cortical potential imaging (CPI) analysis to the data described by Spencer et al. [1999]. The ERPs were recorded from 15 healthy subjects presented with auditory oddball sequences. Cortical potential maps (CPMs) were reconstructed from the scalp potential maps (SPMs) corresponding to the P300 and Novelty P3 components by deblurring the smoothing effect of the head volume conductor. The reconstructed CPMs, derived from the SPMs by means of the CPI, showed localized areas of activity distributed in both the frontal and parietal lobes; the parietal region was active throughout the period of the late positivities. The reconstructed CPMs associated with novel events showed prominent activity at the frontal lobe (Novelty P3) followed by progressively pronounced parietal lobe activity (P300), and these two components can be well separated by the CPMs. These analyses show how the CPI can be used to relate the scalp electrical recordings to the underlying brain activity.


Asunto(s)
Corteza Cerebral/fisiología , Potenciales Relacionados con Evento P300 , Potenciales Evocados , Conducta Exploratoria/fisiología , Mapeo Encefálico/métodos , Diagnóstico por Imagen , Electrodos , Humanos , Cuero Cabelludo/fisiología , Factores de Tiempo , Interfaz Usuario-Computador
4.
Psychophysiology ; 37(5): 644-61, 2000 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11037041

RESUMEN

Distinctive words elicit the P300 component of the event-related brain potential, and are also likely to be recalled. Previous studies have shown that the larger the P300 elicited by distinctive words, the more likely it is that those words will be recalled. The present study addressed whether this relationship is affected by the manner in which distinctiveness is induced. Distinctiveness was manipulated either by varying the size of the characters in which a word was displayed, or by surrounding the word with a frame at close or far distance. All distinctiveness attributes resulted in improved recall performance. The words whose size was distinctive elicited a large P300, and P300 amplitude was larger for subsequently recalled words. The frame attributes elicited a small P300, and the amplitude of these P300s was not correlated with subsequent recall performance. Instead, a frontal slow wave was correlated with subsequent recall performance in the far frame group. It is concluded that the relationship between P300 amplitude and subsequent recall depends on the type of distinctiveness attribute, and should therefore not be ascribed to a generalized effect of distinctiveness on memory encoding processes.


Asunto(s)
Potenciales Relacionados con Evento P300/fisiología , Recuerdo Mental/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Señales (Psicología) , Potenciales Relacionados con Evento P300/efectos de los fármacos , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
5.
Psychophysiology ; 37(4): 494-506, 2000 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10934908

RESUMEN

M.E. Smith (1993) obtained event-related brain potentials (ERPs) from subjects performing a recognition memory task using "remember" (R) and "know" (K) judgments, and reported observing in the ERP a "neurophysiological manifestation of recollective experience" as a difference between the positive waveforms elicited by stimuli that yielded R and K judgments. We replicated his experiment and examined the componential structure of the R > K effect in two ways. First, we found that correction for P300 latency jitter eliminated the effect reported by Smith. Second, the application of principal component analysis indicated that the positive waveform elicited by the words in the test list was a P300. These analyses do not support the hypothesis that there is a new component (the "memory-evoked shift") that is a specific manifestation of recollection.


Asunto(s)
Recuerdo Mental/fisiología , Fenómenos Fisiológicos del Sistema Nervioso , Estimulación Acústica , Adolescente , Adulto , Interpretación Estadística de Datos , Electroencefalografía , Potenciales Relacionados con Evento P300/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Lenguaje , Masculino
6.
IEEE Trans Rehabil Eng ; 8(2): 164-73, 2000 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10896178

RESUMEN

Over the past decade, many laboratories have begun to explore brain-computer interface (BCI) technology as a radically new communication option for those with neuromuscular impairments that prevent them from using conventional augmentative communication methods. BCI's provide these users with communication channels that do not depend on peripheral nerves and muscles. This article summarizes the first international meeting devoted to BCI research and development. Current BCI's use electroencephalographic (EEG) activity recorded at the scalp or single-unit activity recorded from within cortex to control cursor movement, select letters or icons, or operate a neuroprosthesis. The central element in each BCI is a translation algorithm that converts electrophysiological input from the user into output that controls external devices. BCI operation depends on effective interaction between two adaptive controllers, the user who encodes his or her commands in the electrophysiological input provided to the BCI, and the BCI which recognizes the commands contained in the input and expresses them in device control. Current BCI's have maximum information transfer rates of 5-25 b/min. Achievement of greater speed and accuracy depends on improvements in signal processing, translation algorithms, and user training. These improvements depend on increased interdisciplinary cooperation between neuroscientists, engineers, computer programmers, psychologists, and rehabilitation specialists, and on adoption and widespread application of objective methods for evaluating alternative methods. The practical use of BCI technology depends on the development of appropriate applications, identification of appropriate user groups, and careful attention to the needs and desires of individual users. BCI research and development will also benefit from greater emphasis on peer-reviewed publications, and from adoption of standard venues for presentations and discussion.


Asunto(s)
Corteza Cerebral/fisiopatología , Equipos de Comunicación para Personas con Discapacidad , Personas con Discapacidad/rehabilitación , Electroencefalografía/instrumentación , Enfermedades Neuromusculares/rehabilitación , Interfaz Usuario-Computador , Algoritmos , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Humanos , Enfermedades Neuromusculares/fisiopatología , Procesamiento de Señales Asistido por Computador/instrumentación
7.
IEEE Trans Rehabil Eng ; 8(2): 174-9, 2000 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10896179

RESUMEN

We describe a study designed to assess a brain-computer interface (BCI), originally described by Farwell and Donchin [9] in 1988. The system utilizes the fact that the rare events in the oddball paradigm elicit the P300 component of the event-related potential (ERP). The BCI presents the user with a matrix of 6 by 6 cells, each containing one letter of the alphabet. The user focuses attention on the cell containing the letter to be communicated while the rows and the columns of the matrix are intensified. Each intensification is an event in the oddball sequence, the row and the column containing the attended cell are "rare" items and, therefore, only these events elicit a P300. The computer thus detects the transmitted character by determining which row and which column elicited the P300. We report an assessment, using a boot-strapping approach, which indicates that an off line version of the system can communicate at the rate of 7.8 characters a minute and achieve 80% accuracy. The system's performance in real time was also assessed. Our data indicate that a P300-based BCI is feasible and practical. However, these conclusions are based on tests using healthy individuals.


Asunto(s)
Corteza Cerebral/fisiopatología , Equipos de Comunicación para Personas con Discapacidad , Electroencefalografía/instrumentación , Potenciales Relacionados con Evento P300/fisiología , Cuadriplejía/rehabilitación , Interfaz Usuario-Computador , Adulto , Atención/fisiología , Electrodos , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Paraplejía/fisiopatología , Paraplejía/rehabilitación , Cuadriplejía/fisiopatología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Valores de Referencia , Procesamiento de Señales Asistido por Computador/instrumentación
8.
Psychophysiology ; 37(2): 127-52, 2000 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10731765

RESUMEN

Event-related potentials (ERPs) recorded from the human scalp can provide important information about how the human brain normally processes information and about how this processing may go awry in neurological or psychiatric disorders. Scientists using or studying ERPs must strive to overcome the many technical problems that can occur in the recording and analysis of these potentials. The methods and the results of these ERP studies must be published in a way that allows other scientists to understand exactly what was done so that they can, if necessary, replicate the experiments. The data must then be analyzed and presented in a way that allows different studies to be compared readily. This paper presents guidelines for recording ERPs and criteria for publishing the results.


Asunto(s)
Cognición/fisiología , Electroencefalografía , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Humanos , Edición
9.
Psychophysiology ; 36(3): 409-14, 1999 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10352565

RESUMEN

In this study, we examined the relationship between the novelty P3 and the P300 components of the brain event-related potential (ERP). Fifteen subjects responded manually to the rare stimuli embedded either in a classical auditory oddball series or in a series in which "novel" stimuli were inserted. The electroencephalogram (EEG) was recorded with a dense array of 129 electrodes. The data were analyzed by using spatial Principal Components Analysis (PCA) to identify a set of orthogonal scalp distributions, "virtual electrodes" that account for the spatial variance. The data were then expressed as ERPs measured at each of the virtual electrodes. These ERPs were analyzed using temporal PCA, yielding a set of "virtual epochs." Most of the temporal variance of the rare events was associated with a virtual electrode with a posterior topography, that is, with a classical P300, which was active during the virtual epoch associated with the P300. The novel stimuli were found to elicit both a classical P300 and a component focused on a virtual electrode with a frontal topography. We propose that the term Novelty P3 should be restricted to this frontal component.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Corteza Cerebral/fisiología , Discriminación en Psicología/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Conducta Exploratoria/fisiología , Enmascaramiento Perceptual/fisiología , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Análisis Factorial , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Factores de Tiempo
10.
IEEE Trans Rehabil Eng ; 4(4): 425-30, 1996 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8973969

RESUMEN

Current rehabilitation engineering combines new prosthetic methods with recent developments in personal computers to provide alternative communication and control channels to individuals with motor impairments. Despite these advances, all commercially available systems still require some measure of voluntary motor control. Thus, these systems are not useful for individuals who are totally paralyzed. Electroencephalographic (EEG) activity may provide the basis for a system that would completely bypass normal motor output. EEG-based communication technology might provide assistive devices for individuals who have little or no reliable motor function. This paper reviews the prospects for and problems of EEG-based communication. It summarizes current approaches to development of this new technology, describes the major problems that must be resolved, and focuses on issues critical for its use by those with severe motor disabilities.


Asunto(s)
Estimulación Eléctrica/métodos , Electroencefalografía , Potenciales Evocados Visuales/fisiología , Enfermedad de la Neurona Motora/rehabilitación , Encéfalo/fisiopatología , Estimulación Eléctrica/instrumentación , Electroencefalografía/métodos , Electroencefalografía/tendencias , Humanos , Enfermedad de la Neurona Motora/terapia , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Percepción Visual/fisiología
11.
Psychophysiology ; 33(1): 42-53, 1996 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8570794

RESUMEN

Recent research has suggested that there is a component of the event-related brain potential, the error-related negativity (ERN), that is associated with error detection and remedial actions such as error inhibition, immediate error correction, or error compensation. The present experiment used a go/no-go task to define more precisely the functional significance of this component. In this task, an ERN was observed for incorrect responses on go trials (errors of choice) and for responses on no-go trials (errors of action). Because errors of action cannot be corrected immediately by executing another response, these results indicate that the process manifested by the ERN is not dependent on immediate error correction. Other aspects of the data converge in suggesting that the ERN process is more closely related to error detection and that the connections between detection and remedial actions may depend on the task situation.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Adulto , Electroencefalografía , Electromiografía , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología
12.
Schizophr Bull ; 22(4): 703-25, 1996.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8938923

RESUMEN

The Carmel Workshop on Cognitive Psychophysiology began in 1980, and the focus of the 1996 workshop was on schizophrenia. Research into schizophrenia is in the midst of a period of unparalleled advance, driven in large part, by improvements in neuroimaging technology that make detailed examination of in vivo brain structure and function possible. Neuroimaging studies may help provide a bridge between investigations demonstrating molecular and cellular abnormalities in schizophrenia and those demonstrating cognitive dysfunction. The workshop brought together experts in different neuroimaging modalities to present the strengths and advantages of each, as well as the insights each modality might bring into normal and schizophrenic cognitition. It began with a series of tutorials to inform participants of the state of the art in various disciplines. It then broke into four panels, each given a very specific topic assignment related to neuroimaging and/or the cognitive neuroscience of schizophrenia. After 1 1/2 days of discussion, each panel reported its conclusions to the workshop. Group I presented cellular models of the pathophysiology of schizophrenia. Group II examined experimental paradigms for studying cognitive function and schizophrenia. Group III examined technical issues in image processing and combining data across different modalities. Group IV sought to survey the current state of knowledge about the pathophysiology of schizophrenia. The conclusions of each of the groups are presented in this report.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos del Conocimiento , Neurorradiografía , Neurociencias , Esquizofrenia , California , Humanos
13.
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn ; 21(1): 224-40, 1995 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7876771

RESUMEN

The mechanisms underlying the improved recall of isolated events (von Restorff effect) were investigated. Participants studied lists of stimuli containing a physical and a semantic isolate while performing a physical task or a lexical decision task. The physical-task group showed a physical but not a semantic isolation effect (IE) in free recall, whereas the lexical-decision group displayed both types of IEs. The recall of the isolates was independent of that of the other words, and isolates were usually reported separately from other words in the list. Event-related potentials recorded at encoding predicted the recall of both types of isolates. In recognition tests, the IE was obtained only when the encoding context was reinstated. These results are consistent with a model of the IE that stresses the role of the encoding processes immediately following the presentation of distinctive events, and that postulates interactions between these processes and subsequent elaboration of the stimuli.


Asunto(s)
Aprendizaje por Asociación , Atención , Recuerdo Mental , Aprendizaje Verbal , Adulto , Nivel de Alerta/fisiología , Aprendizaje por Asociación/fisiología , Atención/fisiología , Corteza Cerebral/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados Visuales/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Recuerdo Mental/fisiología , Orientación/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Percepción del Tamaño/fisiología , Aprendizaje Verbal/fisiología
15.
J Exp Psychol Gen ; 121(4): 480-506, 1992 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1431740

RESUMEN

Recent studies indicate that subjects may respond to visual information during either an early parallel phase or a later focused phase and that the selection of the relevant phase is data driven. Using the noise-compatibility paradigm, we tested the hypothesis that this selection may also be strategic and context driven. At least part of the interference effect observed in this paradigm is due to response activation during the parallel-processing phase. We manipulated subjects' expectancies for compatible and incompatible noise in 4 experiments and effectively modulated the interference effect. The results suggest that expectancies about the relative utility of the information extracted during the parallel and focused phases determine which phase is used to activate responses.


Asunto(s)
Nivel de Alerta/fisiología , Atención/fisiología , Aprendizaje Discriminativo/fisiología , Procesos Mentales/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Adulto , Corteza Cerebral/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados Visuales/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Orientación/fisiología , Aprendizaje por Probabilidad , Psicofisiología
16.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 18(1): 217-32, 1992 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1532189

RESUMEN

Results are reported from a new paradigm that uses movement-related brain potentials to detect response preparation based on partial information. The paradigm uses a hybrid choice-reaction go/nogo procedure in which decisions about response hand and whether to respond are based on separate stimulus attributes. A lateral asymmetry in the movement-related brain potential was found on nogo trials without overt movement. The direction of this asymmetry depended primarily on the signaled response hand rather than on properties of the stimulus. When the asymmetry first appeared was influenced by the time required to select the signaled hand, and when it began to differ on go and nogo trials was influenced by the time to decide whether to respond. These findings indicate that both stimulus attributes were processed in parallel and that the asymmetry reflected preparation of the response hand that began before the go/nogo decision was completed.


Asunto(s)
Toma de Decisiones , Procesos Mentales , Electromiografía , Humanos , Modelos Psicológicos , Psicofisiología , Tiempo de Reacción , Análisis y Desempeño de Tareas , Factores de Tiempo
17.
Psychophysiology ; 28(5): 531-47, 1991 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1758929

RESUMEN

The feasibility of using Event Related Brain Potentials (ERPs) in Interrogative Polygraphy ("Lie Detection") was tested by examining the effectiveness of the Guilty Knowledge Test designed by Farwell and Donchin (1986, 1988). The subject is assigned an arbitrary task requiring discrimination between experimenter-designated targets and other, irrelevant stimuli. A group of diagnostic items ("probes"), which to the unwitting are indistinguishable from the irrelevant items, are embedded among the irrelevant. For subjects who possess "guilty knowledge" these probes are distinct from the irrelevants and are likely to elicit a P300, thus revealing their possessing the special knowledge that allows them to differentiate the probes from the irrelevants. We report two experiments in which this paradigm was tested. In Experiment 1, 20 subjects participated in one of two mock espionage scenarios and were tested for their knowledge of both scenarios. All stimuli consisted of short phrases presented for 300 ms each at an interstimulus interval of 1550 ms. A set of items were designated as "targets" and appeared on 17% of the trials. Probes related to the scenarios also appeared on 17% of the trials. The rest of the items were irrelevants. Subjects responded by pressing one switch following targets, and the other following irrelevants (and, of course, probes). ERPs were recorded from FZ, CZ, and PZ. As predicted, targets elicited large P300s in all subjects. Probes associated with a given scenario elicited a P300 in subjects who participated in that scenario. A bootstrapping method was used to assess the quality of the decision for each subject. The algorithm declared the decision indeterminate in 12.5% of the cases. In all other cases a decision was made. There were no false positives and no false negatives: whenever a determination was made it was accurate. The second experiment was virtually identical to the first, with identical results, except that this time 4 subjects were tested, each of which had a minor brush with the law. Subjects were tested to determine whether they possessed information on their own "crimes." The results were as expected; the Guilty Knowledge Test determined correctly which subject possessed which information. The implications of these data both for the practice of Interrogative Polygraphy and the interpretation of the P300 are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Detección de Mentiras , Adulto , Electroencefalografía , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
18.
Neuropsychologia ; 29(6): 557-69, 1991.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1944861

RESUMEN

The search for the intracranial sources of components of the ERP using non-human species can benefit from an analysis of the functional significance of these components. Such analysis provides criteria, based on the response of the components to experimental manipulations, for identifying those aspects of the ERP recorded in other species that are analogous to specific ERP components recorded from human subjects. This thesis is illustrated by examining the problems presented by the data recorded in monkeys by Donchin et al. (Electroenceph. clin. Neurophysiol. 31, 115-127, 1971). The different ERP components they observed in the monkeys could not be fully interpreted because a detailed analysis of the ERPs recorded during similar paradigms in humans was lacking. We review studies of the Lateralized Readiness Potential using human subjects which indicate how the activity recorded during such waiting intervals which specifically relates to motor preparation can be identified.


Asunto(s)
Nivel de Alerta/fisiología , Atención/fisiología , Mapeo Encefálico/métodos , Corteza Cerebral/fisiología , Electroencefalografía , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Animales , Variación Contingente Negativa/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Femenino , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino
19.
Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol ; 75(5): 419-32, 1990 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1692277

RESUMEN

Measures of overt response and of the event-related brain potential (ERP) were used to investigate the processing of a priming stimulus varying in its information content. Subjects were shown sequences of 2 letters that served as a priming and an imperative stimulus. In 3 randomly interspersed conditions the imperative stimulus had a 0.80, 0.50, or 0.20 probability of physically matching the priming letter. The different probability conditions were signaled by the position of a dot flanking the priming letter. Reaction time and accuracy data indicated that the subjects primed their responses as a function of the information conveyed by the priming stimulus. The amplitude and latency of the P300 to the priming stimulus were sensitive to the amount of information conveyed by the priming stimulus and the duration of the processing required. The readiness potential in the foreperiod was lateralized as a function of the priming stimulus. Furthermore, the larger the amplitude of the P300 to the priming stimulus, the larger the lateralization of the readiness potential, indicating that information extraction, indexed by the P300, was related to response priming, indexed by the readiness potential. The results indicate that ERP measures make manifest covert aspects of the priming process occurring in the foreperiod.


Asunto(s)
Procesos Mentales/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Estimulación Eléctrica , Electroencefalografía , Electromiografía , Potenciales Evocados , Lateralidad Funcional , Humanos , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Psicofisiología , Tiempo de Reacción
20.
Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol ; 75(2): 22-35, 1990 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1688770

RESUMEN

Subjects were instructed to use either rote or elaborative strategies to memorize words in a Von Restorff paradigm. When instructed to use rote strategies, subjects displayed a higher Von Restorff effect and a lower recall performance than when instructed to use elaborative strategies. Furthermore the amplitude of the P300 component of the event-related brain potential predicted subsequent recall only when subjects used rote strategies. When subjects used elaborative strategies, the relationship between P300 amplitude and subsequent recall was not observed. These results confirm and expand, in a within-subjects design, the results reported by Karis, Fabiani and Donchin (1984) who capitalized on different strategies used by different subjects. These results also lend support to a 3-phase model of the von Restorff effect.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiología , Electroencefalografía , Potenciales Evocados , Memoria/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Modelos Neurológicos
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA
...