Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 5 de 5
Filtrar
Más filtros










Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Cortex ; 175: 1-11, 2024 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38691922

RESUMEN

Studies have reported substantial variability in emotion recognition ability (ERA) - an important social skill - but possible neural underpinnings for such individual differences are not well understood. This functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study investigated neural responses during emotion recognition in young adults (N = 49) who were selected for inclusion based on their performance (high or low) during previous testing of ERA. Participants were asked to judge brief video recordings in a forced-choice emotion recognition task, wherein stimuli were presented in visual, auditory and multimodal (audiovisual) blocks. Emotion recognition rates during brain scanning confirmed that individuals with high (vs low) ERA received higher accuracy for all presentation blocks. fMRI-analyses focused on key regions of interest (ROIs) involved in the processing of multimodal emotion expressions, based on previous meta-analyses. In neural response to emotional stimuli contrasted with neutral stimuli, individuals with high (vs low) ERA showed higher activation in the following ROIs during the multimodal condition: right middle superior temporal gyrus (mSTG), right posterior superior temporal sulcus (PSTS), and right inferior frontal cortex (IFC). Overall, results suggest that individual variability in ERA may be reflected across several stages of decisional processing, including extraction (mSTG), integration (PSTS) and evaluation (IFC) of emotional information.


Asunto(s)
Mapeo Encefálico , Emociones , Individualidad , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Reconocimiento en Psicología , Humanos , Masculino , Femenino , Emociones/fisiología , Adulto Joven , Adulto , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Encéfalo/fisiología , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Expresión Facial , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Reconocimiento Facial/fisiología
2.
Transl Psychiatry ; 14(1): 203, 2024 May 14.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38744808

RESUMEN

Perinatal affective disorders are common, but standard screening measures reliant on subjective self-reports might not be sufficient to identify pregnant women at-risk for developing postpartum depression and anxiety. Lower heart rate variability (HRV) has been shown to be associated with affective disorders. The current exploratory study aimed to evaluate the predictive utility of late pregnancy HRV measurements of postpartum affective symptoms. A subset of participants from the BASIC study (Uppsala, Sweden) took part in a sub-study at pregnancy week 38 where HRV was measured before and after a mild stressor (n = 122). Outcome measures were 6-week postpartum depression and anxiety symptoms as quantified by the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS) and the Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI). In total, 112 women were included in a depression outcome analysis and 106 women were included in an anxiety outcome analysis. Group comparisons indicated that lower pregnancy HRV was associated with depressive or anxious symptomatology at 6 weeks postpartum. Elastic net logistic regression analyses indicated that HRV indices alone were not predictive of postpartum depression or anxiety outcomes, but HRV indices were selected as predictors in a combined model with background and pregnancy variables. ROC curves for the combined models gave an area under the curve (AUC) of 0.93 for the depression outcome and an AUC of 0.83 for the anxiety outcome. HRV indices predictive of postpartum depression generally differed from those predictive of postpartum anxiety. HRV indices did not significantly improve prediction models comprised of psychological measures only in women with pregnancy depression or anxiety.


Asunto(s)
Ansiedad , Depresión Posparto , Frecuencia Cardíaca , Humanos , Femenino , Depresión Posparto/fisiopatología , Depresión Posparto/diagnóstico , Embarazo , Frecuencia Cardíaca/fisiología , Adulto , Ansiedad/fisiopatología , Escalas de Valoración Psiquiátrica , Suecia , Trastornos de Ansiedad/fisiopatología , Trastornos de Ansiedad/diagnóstico , Adulto Joven
3.
Dev Sci ; 27(5): e13526, 2024 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38712829

RESUMEN

Previous research and theory indicate an importance of the quality of the early caregiving environment in the development of self-regulation. However, it is unclear how attachment security and maternal sensitivity, two related but distinct aspects of the early caregiving environment, may differentially predict self-regulation at school start and whether a distinction between hot and cool executive function is informative in characterizing such predictions through mediation. In a 5-year longitudinal study (n = 108), we examined these associations using measures of maternal sensitivity and attachment security at 10-12 months, executive function at 4 years, and self-regulation at 6 years. Surprisingly, and despite methodological rigor, we found few significant bivariate associations between the study variables. We found no credible evidence of a longitudinal association between maternal sensitivity or attachment security in infancy and self-regulation at 6 years, or between executive function at 4 years and self-regulation at 6 years. The lack of bivariate longitudinal associations precluded us from building mediation models as intended. We discuss our null findings in terms of their potential theoretical implications, as well as how measurement type, reliability, and validity, may play a key role in determining longitudinal associations between early caregiving factors and later self-regulation and related abilities. RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS: The early caregiving environment has been implicated in the development of later self-regulation, which includes more basic skills, such as hot and cool executive functions (EF). In a 5-year longitudinal study, with a sample of 108 children, we rigorously measured aspects of early caregiving, EF, and self-regulation. We found no significant longitudinal associations between early caregiving and self-regulation at 6 years, nor between EF at 4 years and self-regulation at 6 years. These null results highlight the complexity of modeling self-regulation development and raise critical questions about general methodological conventions within self-regulation development research.


Asunto(s)
Función Ejecutiva , Relaciones Madre-Hijo , Autocontrol , Humanos , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Estudios Longitudinales , Preescolar , Femenino , Masculino , Niño , Lactante , Desarrollo Infantil/fisiología , Apego a Objetos , Cuidadores/psicología
4.
Dev Psychobiol ; 66(4): e22492, 2024 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38643360

RESUMEN

During adolescence, emotion regulation and reactivity are still developing and are in many ways qualitatively different from adulthood. However, the neurobiological processes underpinning these differences remain poorly understood, including the role of maturing neurotransmitter systems. We combined magnetic resonance spectroscopy in the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) and self-reported emotion regulation and reactivity in a sample of typically developed adolescents (n = 37; 13-16 years) and adults (n = 39; 30-40 years), and found that adolescents had higher levels of glutamate to total creatine (tCr) ratio in the dACC than adults. A glutamate Í age group interaction indicated a differential relation between dACC glutamate levels and emotion regulation in adolescents and adults, and within-group follow-up analyses showed that higher levels of glutamate/tCr were related to worse emotion regulation skills in adolescents. We found no age-group differences in gamma-aminobutyric acid+macromolecules (GABA+) levels; however, emotion reactivity was positively related to GABA+/tCr in the adult group, but not in the adolescent group. The results demonstrate that there are developmental changes in the concentration of glutamate, but not GABA+, within the dACC from adolescence to adulthood, in accordance with previous findings indicating earlier maturation of the GABA-ergic than the glutamatergic system. Functionally, glutamate and GABA+ are positively related to emotion regulation and reactivity, respectively, in the mature brain. In the adolescent brain, however, glutamate is negatively related to emotion regulation, and GABA+ is not related to emotion reactivity. The findings are consistent with synaptic pruning of glutamatergic synapses from adolescence to adulthood and highlight the importance of brain maturational processes underlying age-related differences in emotion processing.


Asunto(s)
Regulación Emocional , Ácido Glutámico , Adulto , Humanos , Adolescente , Giro del Cíngulo/química , Giro del Cíngulo/fisiología , Ácido gamma-Aminobutírico/análisis , Receptores de Antígenos de Linfocitos T/análisis
5.
Int. j. clin. health psychol. (Internet) ; 23(4)oct.-dic. 2023. ilus, tab, graf
Artículo en Inglés | IBECS | ID: ibc-226360

RESUMEN

Background: The antidepressant effect of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) is partly placebo, making blinding integrity important. Blinding of high-frequency rTMS and intermittent theta burst stimulation (iTBS) has been reported as successful at study end. However, blinding integrity at study start is rarely reported. The aim of this study was to investigate blinding integrity during a treatment course of iTBS over the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (DMPFC) in depression. Methods: Forty-nine patients with depression from a double-blind-designed randomized controlled trial (NCT02905604) were included. Patients received either active or sham iTBS over the DMPFC with a placebo coil. The sham group received iTBS-synchronized transcutaneous electrical nerve stimulation. Results: After one session, 74% of participants were able to correctly guess their treatment allocation. This was above chance level (p = 0.001). The percentage dropped to 64% and 56% after the fifth and last sessions. Belonging to the active group influenced the choice to guess “active” (odds ratio: 11.7, 95% CI 2.5–53.7). A higher treatment intensity of the sham treatment increased the probability to guess “active”, but pain did not influence the choice. Conclusions: Blinding integrity in iTBS trials must be investigated at study start to avoid uncontrolled confounding. Better sham methods are needed. (AU)


Asunto(s)
Humanos , Estimulación Magnética Transcraneal/efectos adversos , Trastorno Depresivo Mayor/tratamiento farmacológico , Corteza Prefrontal , Ceguera , Estimulación Magnética Transcraneal/métodos , Entrevistas como Asunto
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA