Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 15 de 15
Filtrar
1.
Aggress Behav ; 33(6): 574-82, 2007.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17654672

RESUMEN

Research aimed at identifying and studying subtypes of aggression have historically dichotomized aggressive subtypes, although specific nomenclature has varied; one approach has been to classify aggressive behavior as predominantly impulsive or predominantly premeditated. There are a number of behavioral and cognitive differences between those exhibiting these different forms of aggression. This study was designed to extend understanding of the impulsive/premeditated aggression dichotomy by comparing time estimation among adolescents exhibiting predominantly impulsive or predominantly premeditated forms of physical aggression who have a psychiatric diagnosis of conduct disorder (CD). Time estimation has previously been shown to be disrupted in impulsive and some aggressive individuals. Time estimation was compared between healthy Controls (n = 37) and two groups of adolescents with CD, those with histories of either predominantly impulsive (CD-Impulsive, n = 26) or predominantly premeditated (CD-Premeditated, n = 38) aggressive behaviors. Participants completed five computerized trials during which they estimated when 1 min had passed. Among aggressive adolescents with CD, the misperception of time was specific to those with histories of impulsive aggression, although time estimates improved with repeated testing and performance feedback. This study confirms the importance of considering the role and type of physical aggression when studying heterogeneous diagnostic groups like CD and supports the relevance of time estimation to certain subgroups of adolescents with CD.


Asunto(s)
Agresión/psicología , Trastornos del Conocimiento/epidemiología , Trastorno de la Conducta/epidemiología , Trastorno de la Conducta/psicología , Trastornos Disruptivos, del Control de Impulso y de la Conducta/epidemiología , Trastornos Disruptivos, del Control de Impulso y de la Conducta/psicología , Percepción del Tiempo , Adolescente , Trastornos del Conocimiento/diagnóstico , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Psicometría , Escalas de Wechsler
2.
Drug Alcohol Depend ; 90(2-3): 288-91, 2007 Oct 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17475417

RESUMEN

Because of the widespread use of drugs by adolescents, there is demand for scientific rigor in sampling and accuracy in methods for ascertaining drug use patterns. The present study: (1) characterized adolescents who responded to advertisements for marijuana users; (2) compared rates of drug use reported on the telephone versus an on-site interview; and (3) examined drug use patterns as a function of parental awareness of drug use. Adolescents, identifying themselves as marijuana users during telephone interviews, reported more use of other drugs than those denying marijuana use. There was a high degree of correspondence between telephone and on-site interviews for all drugs except alcohol, which was reported at a higher rate on-site. Of those reporting marijuana use in the past week, 69% tested positive for marijuana in their urine-drug screens. Finally, marijuana and alcohol use patterns were higher among adolescents whose parents were aware of drug use than those whose parents indicated that their adolescent did not use marijuana. These results indicate that adolescents are willing to self-identify as marijuana users and report drug and alcohol use during telephone interviews. Additionally, parents appear to become more aware of their adolescent's drug use with increased frequency of use.


Asunto(s)
Decepción , Trastornos Relacionados con Sustancias/epidemiología , Teléfono , Revelación de la Verdad , Adolescente , Niño , Demografía , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
3.
J Pers ; 75(3): 629-62, 2007 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17489894

RESUMEN

We examine two issues linking personality pathology and judgment of traits within the Five-Factor Model of personality. We hypothesize that pathology moderates self-other agreement--"target" participants with pathology should be less judgable than participants without pathology. In addition, we hypothesize that pathology could partially produce agreement across a variety of traits, particularly those traits fundamental to the pathology. In an adolescent sample including a group with Conduct Disorder (CD) and a Control group, we examine agreement between adolescents' self-reports and their mothers' informant reports. Using trait-centered and person-centered perspectives, we find support for both hypotheses. Results have implications for understanding the processes affecting personality judgment, for increasing integration of traditional personality research and personality pathology, and for personality assessment.


Asunto(s)
Conducta del Adolescente/psicología , Trastorno de la Conducta/psicología , Autoimagen , Autoevaluación (Psicología) , Deseabilidad Social , Percepción Social , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Modelos Psicológicos , Relaciones Padres-Hijo , Grupo Paritario , Psicometría
4.
Psychiatry Res ; 151(3): 231-42, 2007 Jun 30.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17383014

RESUMEN

This study extends the use of the Impulsive/Premeditated Aggression Scale for subtyping aggressive behavior among adolescents with Conduct Disorder. Of the Conduct Disorder symptoms, aggression has the strongest prognostic and treatment implications. While aggression is a complex construct, convergent evidence supports a dichotomy of impulsive and premeditated aggressive subtypes that are qualitatively different from one another in terms of phenomenology and neurobiology. Previous attempts at measuring subtypes of aggression in children and adults are not clearly generalizable to adolescents. Sixty-six adolescents completed a questionnaire for characterizing aggression (Impulsive/Premeditated Aggression Scale), along with standard measures of personality and general functioning. Principal components analysis demonstrated two stable factors of aggression with good internal consistency and construct validity. Compared to the premeditated aggression factor, the impulsive aggression factor was associated with a broader range of personality, thought, emotional, and social problems. As in the adult and child literature, characterization of aggressive behavior into two subtypes appears to be relevant to understanding individual differences among adolescents with Conduct Disorder.


Asunto(s)
Agresión/psicología , Trastorno de Personalidad Antisocial/psicología , Trastorno de la Conducta/psicología , Toma de Decisiones , Conducta Impulsiva/psicología , Inventario de Personalidad/estadística & datos numéricos , Población Urbana , Adolescente , Trastorno de Personalidad Antisocial/diagnóstico , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/diagnóstico , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/psicología , Comorbilidad , Trastorno de la Conducta/diagnóstico , Femenino , Humanos , Conducta Impulsiva/diagnóstico , Masculino , Análisis de Componente Principal , Psicometría/estadística & datos numéricos , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Texas
5.
Psychopharmacology (Berl) ; 193(1): 137-50, 2007 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17377773

RESUMEN

RATIONALE: Indirect evidence supports a link between serotonergic activity and individual differences in the behavioral response to alcohol, but few studies have experimentally demonstrated that an individual's biological state can influence the sensitivity to alcohol-induced behaviors. OBJECTIVE: Our purpose was to temporarily modify serotonin synthesis in healthy individuals to determine how altered biological states may interact with alcohol administration to affect impulsive behavior. MATERIALS AND METHODS: In a repeated-measures design, 18 normal controls consumed a 50-g L: -tryptophan (Trp) depleting (ATD) or loading (ATL) amino-acid beverage that temporarily decreased or increased (respectively) serotonin synthesis before receiving either a moderate dose of alcohol (0.65 g/kg) or placebo. All participants completed three impulsivity testing sessions on each of the five experimental days. Session one was a baseline session. Session two included testing after ATD-only or ATL-only. Session three included: (1) placebo after ATL (ATL+PBO); (2) placebo after ATD (ATD+PBO); (3) alcohol after ATL (ATL+ALC); (4) alcohol after ATD (ATD+ALC); and (5) Alcohol-only conditions. Impulsivity was assessed using the Immediate Memory Task (Dougherty et al., Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput 34:391-398, 2002), a continuous performance test yielding commission errors that have been previously validated as a component of impulsive behavior. RESULTS: Primary findings were that ATD-only increased impulsive responding compared to ATL-only, and ATD+ALC increased commission errors to levels higher than either the ATL+ALC or Alcohol-only conditions. CONCLUSIONS: These findings demonstrate that reduced serotonin synthesis can produce increased impulsivity even among non-impulsive normal controls, and that the behavioral effects of alcohol are, in part, dependent on this biological state.


Asunto(s)
Etanol/efectos adversos , Conducta Impulsiva , Serotonina/biosíntesis , Triptófano , Adulto , Pruebas Respiratorias , Método Doble Ciego , Femenino , Humanos , Conducta Impulsiva/inducido químicamente , Conducta Impulsiva/metabolismo , Conducta Impulsiva/psicología , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Triptófano/administración & dosificación , Triptófano/deficiencia , Triptófano/farmacología
6.
Behav Res Methods ; 37(1): 82-90, 2005 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16097347

RESUMEN

Previous research and theory have conceptualized impulsivity as a multifaceted construct that requires multiple modes of measurement for accurate assessment. This article describes a software package that includes four paradigms for measuring multiple and unique aspects of impulsivity. Specifically, four tasks are described: (1) the two choice impulsivity paradigm, (2) the single key impulsivity paradigm, (3) the GoStop impulsivity paradigm, and (4) the time paradigm. These tasks measure processes related to the capacity to tolerate delay for reward, to inhibit an already initiated response, and to estimate the passage of time. These processes have been found to be important to the understanding of impulsive behaviors. The programs are flexible and allow the experimenter to manipulate a number of parameters related to delay-reward contingencies, timing, performance feedback/payment, and data output variables. Manipulation of these parameters makes the paradigms scalable to a wide range of ability levels and appropriate for samples ranging from children to adults. The four paradigms in this software package are available at no cost and can be obtained by contacting the corresponding author.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Impulsiva/epidemiología , Programas Informáticos , Animales , Atención , Behaviorismo , Conducta de Elección , Recolección de Datos/estadística & datos numéricos , Retroalimentación , Humanos , Conducta Impulsiva/psicología , Cómputos Matemáticos , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Motivación , Esquema de Refuerzo , Percepción del Tiempo
7.
Suicide Life Threat Behav ; 34(4): 374-85, 2004.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15585459

RESUMEN

The purpose of this study was to examine the relationship between laboratory behavioral measured impulsivity (using the Immediate and Delayed Memory Tasks) and suicidal attempt histories. Three groups of adults were recruited, those with either: no previous suicide attempts (Control, n = 20), only a single suicide attempt (Single, n = 20), or multiple suicidal attempts (Multiple, n = 10). As hypothesized, impulsive responses increased with the number of suicide attempts (Control < Single < Multiple). This study helps to demonstrate how laboratory behavioral measures of impulsivity can be used to discriminate groups based on suicidal histories among samples not currently exhibiting significant suicidal behaviors.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos Disruptivos, del Control de Impulso y de la Conducta/psicología , Intento de Suicidio/psicología , Adulto , Ansiedad/diagnóstico , Manual Diagnóstico y Estadístico de los Trastornos Mentales , Trastornos Disruptivos, del Control de Impulso y de la Conducta/diagnóstico , Estudios de Seguimiento , Humanos , Proyectos de Investigación , Autoevaluación (Psicología) , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
8.
Drug Alcohol Depend ; 76 Suppl: S93-S105, 2004 Dec 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15555820

RESUMEN

Impulsivity appears to play an important role in suicidal behaviors and drug abuse, which are two psychiatric problems that may interact with one another. Interpretation of the literature on impulsivity in these behaviors may be complicated by the variety of measurement techniques for the assessment of impulsivity. There are three general types of impulsivity assessment: self-report, biological, and laboratory behavioral. Because laboratory behavioral measures both meet an operational definition of impulsivity and are sensitive to state-dependent changes in impulsivity, this paper presents data that focuses on laboratory behavioral performance among samples emitting suicidal behaviors or substance abuse. To better account for influence of impulsivity in these psychiatric disturbances, no single source of measurement should be used without the consideration of other types of instruments.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos Disruptivos, del Control de Impulso y de la Conducta/diagnóstico , Trastornos Disruptivos, del Control de Impulso y de la Conducta/epidemiología , Trastornos Relacionados con Sustancias/diagnóstico , Trastornos Relacionados con Sustancias/epidemiología , Intento de Suicidio/estadística & datos numéricos , Toma de Decisiones , Humanos , Trastornos de la Memoria/epidemiología
9.
Alcohol Clin Exp Res ; 28(3): 408-14, 2004 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15084898

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: This study examined the relationship between laboratory-measured impulsivity and age at first drink. METHODS: Using a laboratory behavioral measure of impulsivity [Immediate (IMT) and Delayed Memory Tasks (DMT)], we compared two groups of women differing in their self-reported age at first drink (early-onset drinking, age <18 years, n = 40; late-onset drinking, age > or =21 years, n = 23). It was expected that those who first consumed alcohol before the legal drinking age (i.e., early onset) would perform in a more impulsive manner on the laboratory behavioral measure than the late-onset drinkers. RESULTS: The main finding was that the early-onset group (IMT: mean, 28.7%; DMT: mean, 30.4%) had increased commission error rates compared with the late-onset group (IMT: mean, 21.2%; DMT: mean, 15.5%) during both the IMT [ANOVA:F (1,61) = 4.30; p = 0.042; f = 0.27] and DMT [F (1,61) = 10.76; p = 0.002; f = 0.42]. Age at first drink was significantly correlated with DMT commission errors (r = -0.23; p = 0.037), although this was only at the trend level for IMT commission errors (r = -0.20; p = 0.062); these correlations are likely to be underestimates because of range restriction of the age variable. CONCLUSIONS: These results demonstrate that differences in impulsive behavioral responding are distinguishable even between groups of alcohol drinkers who are not experiencing clinically significant problems with alcohol.


Asunto(s)
Consumo de Bebidas Alcohólicas/psicología , Conducta Impulsiva/psicología , Memoria/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Adulto , Factores de Edad , Análisis de Varianza , Distribución de Chi-Cuadrado , Femenino , Humanos , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología
10.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry ; 44(8): 1145-57, 2003 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14626456

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Behavioral impulsivity paradigms vary widely and studies using these measures have typically relied on a single measure used in isolation. As a result, comparisons between measures are difficult, with little consensus regarding which method may be most sensitive to individual impulsivity differences of different populations. METHOD: A single testing session of each of four different impulsivity tasks was completed by two groups of adolescents aged 13-17: hospitalized inpatients with disruptive behavior disorders (DBD; n = 22) and controls (n = 22). Tasks included two rapid-decision (IMT/DMT and GoStop) and two reward-directed (TC and SKIP) impulsivity paradigms. Behavioral testing took place within 3 days of hospitalization for the adolescents with DBD. RESULTS: Compared to controls, the DBD group exhibited higher commission error rates, lower inhibited response rates after a stop-signal, and twice as many reward-directed responses even after IQ differences between the groups were taken into account. When the four paradigms were compared, effect-size calculations indicated that the two rapid-decision paradigms were more sensitive to group differences than the reward-directed tasks. CONCLUSIONS: Despite the initiation of pharmacotherapy within the first 3 days of hospitalization, in contrast to the control group, the adolescents with DBD performed consistently with what has been operationally defined as impulsivity. Based on these results, these tasks appear to measure similar, but unique components of the impulsivity construct. With further study, laboratory behavioral paradigms may prove to be useful additions to current clinical diagnostic and treatment procedures in a variety of psychiatric populations.


Asunto(s)
Adolescente Hospitalizado/psicología , Déficit de la Atención y Trastornos de Conducta Disruptiva/psicología , Conducta Impulsiva/diagnóstico , Conducta Impulsiva/psicología , Adolescente , Adolescente Hospitalizado/estadística & datos numéricos , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Toma de Decisiones/fisiología , Femenino , Hospitales Psiquiátricos , Humanos , Pruebas de Inteligencia , Masculino , Memoria/fisiología , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Psicometría , Valores de Referencia , Recompensa
11.
Assessment ; 10(1): 3-12, 2003 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12675379

RESUMEN

This study is one in a series investigating the relationship between impulsive behavior on a Continuous Performance Test (i.e., the Immediate and Delayed Memory Task) and other cognitive deficits measured by clinical instruments. Forty-two adolescents were selected for two groups, controls and hospitalized patients with disruptive behavior disorders. Each adolescent completed the Immediate and Delayed Memory Task and the Benton Visual Retention Test. Our main findings were that, even when controlling for IQ, the Immediate and Delayed Memory Task commission errors were associated with adverse Benton performance, but only in the patient group. These results may be explained by a shared association between processes of impulsivity and other deficits of executive control that may interfere with successful performance of the Benton.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Impulsiva/psicología , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Pruebas Psicológicas , Adolescente , Déficit de la Atención y Trastornos de Conducta Disruptiva/psicología , Humanos , Memoria , Retención en Psicología
12.
J Gen Psychol ; 130(1): 5-21, 2003 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12635853

RESUMEN

Continuous Performance Tests (CPTs) provide information on attentional processing and impulsive behavior. The results of previous research that used self-report measures have provided evidence for familial transmission (through genetic and/or environmental influences) of impulsive characteristics. The authors of the present study examined whether the impulsive behavioral parameters that are measured by the CPT also share familial relationships. The researchers asked 26 healthy parent-adolescent pairs to complete the Immediate and Delayed Memory Tasks (IMT/DMT; D. M. Dougherty, 1999; D. M. Dougherty, D. M. Marsh, & C. W. Mathias, 2002), a modified CPT (B. A. Cornblatt, N. J. Risch, G. Faris, D. Friedman, & L. Erlenmeyer-Kimling, 1988; H. E. Rosvold, A. Mirsky, I. Sarason, E. D. Breansome, Jr., & L. H. Beck, 1956), and the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS; J. H. Patton, M. S. Stanford, & E. S. Barratt, 1995), a self-report measure. The main findings can be summarized as follows: (a) commission errors (but not correct detections) on the IMT and DMT were correlated between parents and their adolescent children, (b) adolescents emitted a higher proportion of commission errors than did their parents, and (c) self-reported impulsivity (i.e., BIS) was correlated with commission errors for parents, but not for adolescents. The findings of this study support the use of an objective behavioral measure of impulsivity to assess familial relationships of impulsivity.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Conducta Impulsiva/genética , Pruebas Psicológicas , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Conducta Impulsiva/diagnóstico , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad
13.
Percept Mot Skills ; 95(2): 559-69, 2002 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12434851

RESUMEN

The Immediate and Delayed Memory Tasks are variations of the Continuous Performance Test. While previous research with the tasks has focused on group performance across multiple blocks and sessions, reliability estimates have not been established. We estimated reliability (1) between blocks of a single testing session (N=106), (2) across four sessions within a single testing day (N=20), and (3) across sessions on four consecutive testing days (N=20). Analyses indicated that the primary variable of interest, i.e., commission of errors, showed acceptable reliability within and across testing periods.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas/estadística & datos numéricos , Retención en Psicología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Psicometría , Valores de Referencia , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados
14.
Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput ; 34(3): 391-8, 2002 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12395555

RESUMEN

The Immediate and Delayed Memory Task (IMT/DMT), a variant of the Continuous Performance Test (CPT), is a new software package designed to be a flexible research tool for the study of attention, memory, and impulsivity. This package allows researchers to determine the design to be used during a testing session and to manipulate many of the parameters. It features two components: the IMT and the DMT, both of which present sequential 2- to 7-digit stimuli with variable presentation rates and intertrial intervals. Subjects respond to identically matched stimuli presented consecutively, spanning a brief period of time (IMT), or to stimuli spanning a greater period of time (during which intervening stimuli to be ignored appear; DMT). Task complexity can be adjusted to suit applications for both children and adults. Preliminary studies have demonstrated that these laboratory tasks are sensitive to group differences, produce stable baselines of performance, and are sensitive to drug-induced performance decrements.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Conducta Impulsiva , Memoria , Humanos , Programas Informáticos , Factores de Tiempo
15.
Neuropsychopharmacology ; 26(5): 660-71, 2002 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11927191

RESUMEN

Plasma L-tryptophan (Trp) reductions have been related to aggression increases in men. Impairment of serotonin synthesis and neurotransmission is one explanation. Using repeated-measures, this Trp manipulation study measured laboratory-induced aggression in 12 women after Trp augmentation (T+), depletion (T-), and food-restricted (fasting control) conditions. Participants were provoked with periodic subtraction of money from their task earnings by a (fictitious) partner. Aggression was defined as the number of point subtractions participants made from their fictitious partner. Participants completed five testing sessions under each condition. T+ decreased aggressive responses and T- increased aggressive responses. Post-hoc analyses showed changes in aggressive behavior were specific to women with higher fasting control plasma Trp, which is consistent with research demonstrating that men with higher levels of baseline Trp are more aggressive. These findings indicate that both T+ and T- can influence aggressive behavior and that certain subgroups of women may be more susceptible to serotonin manipulation.


Asunto(s)
Agresión/efectos de los fármacos , Triptófano/metabolismo , Triptófano/farmacología , Adolescente , Adulto , Agresión/fisiología , Agresión/psicología , Análisis de Varianza , Distribución de Chi-Cuadrado , Técnicas de Laboratorio Clínico/estadística & datos numéricos , Femenino , Humanos , Triptófano/deficiencia
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA
...