Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 21
Filtrar
Más filtros










Base de datos
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Curr Opin Insect Sci ; 56: 101018, 2023 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36842606

RESUMEN

The focus of bee neuroscience has for a long time been on only a handful of social honeybee and bumblebee species, out of thousands of bees species that have been described. On the other hand, information about the chemical ecology of bees is much more abundant. Here we attempted to compile the scarce information about olfactory systems of bees across species. We also review the major categories of intra- and inter-specific olfactory behaviors of bees, with specific focus on recent literature. We finish by discussing the most promising avenues for bee olfactory research in the near future.


Asunto(s)
Abejas , Olfato , Animales , Abejas/fisiología
2.
Methods Mol Biol ; 2540: 35-78, 2022.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35980572

RESUMEN

Binary expression systems are useful genetic tools for experimentally labeling or manipulating the function of defined cells. The Q-system is a repressible binary expression system that consists of a transcription factor QF (and the recently improved QF2/QF2w), the inhibitor QS, a QUAS-geneX effector, and a drug that inhibits QS (quinic acid). The Q-system can be used alone or in combination with other binary expression systems, such as GAL4/UAS and LexA/LexAop. In this review chapter, we discuss the past, present, and future of the Q-system for applications in Drosophila and other organisms. We discuss the in vivo application of the Q-system for transgenic labeling, the modular nature of QF that allows chimeric or split transcriptional activators to be developed, its temporal control by quinic acid, new methods to generate QF2 reagents, intersectional expression labeling, and its recent adoption into many emerging experimental species.


Asunto(s)
Proteínas de Drosophila , Ácido Quínico , Animales , Animales Modificados Genéticamente , Drosophila/genética , Proteínas de Drosophila/genética , Proteínas de Drosophila/metabolismo , Ácido Quínico/farmacología , Factores de Transcripción/genética , Factores de Transcripción/metabolismo , Transgenes
3.
Cold Spring Harb Protoc ; 2022(11): Pdb.top107691, 2022 11 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35960628

RESUMEN

Mosquitoes are fascinating and diverse study organisms that are rapidly becoming genetically tractable. Here, we review the latest and most commonly used approaches to silence or edit genes and express transgenes in mosquitoes. We also focus on gene drives-a genetic technology that may prove essential for combating mosquito-borne diseases.


Asunto(s)
Culicidae , Animales , Culicidae/genética , Animales Modificados Genéticamente , Transgenes
4.
Front Physiol ; 12: 697886, 2021.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34955870

RESUMEN

Gaze direction is closely coupled with body movement in insects and other animals. If movement patterns interfere with the acquisition of visual information, insects can actively adjust them to seek relevant cues. Alternatively, where multiple visual cues are available, an insect's movements may influence how it perceives a scene. We show that the way a foraging bumblebee approaches a floral pattern could determine what it learns about the pattern. When trained to vertical bicoloured patterns, bumblebees consistently approached from below centre in order to land in the centre of the target where the reward was located. In subsequent tests, the bees preferred the colour of the lower half of the pattern that they predominantly faced during the approach and landing sequence. A predicted change of learning outcomes occurred when the contrast line was moved up or down off-centre: learned preferences again reflected relative frontal exposure to each colour during the approach, independent of the overall ratio of colours. This mechanism may underpin learning strategies in both simple and complex visual discriminations, highlighting that morphology and action patterns determines how animals solve sensory learning tasks. The deterministic effect of movement on visual learning may have substantially influenced the evolution of floral signals, particularly where plants depend on fine-scaled movements of pollinators on flowers.

5.
Cell Tissue Res ; 383(1): 75-90, 2021 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33475852

RESUMEN

There are 3559 species of mosquitoes in the world (Harbach 2018) but, so far, only a handful of them have been a focus of olfactory neuroscience and neurobiology research. Here we discuss mosquito olfactory anatomy and function and connect these to mosquito ecology. We highlight the least well-known and thus most interesting aspects of mosquito olfactory systems and discuss promising future directions. We hope this review will encourage the insect neuroscience community to work more broadly across mosquito species instead of focusing narrowly on the main disease vectors.


Asunto(s)
Neuronas Receptoras Olfatorias/fisiología , Animales , Mosquitos Vectores
6.
Nature ; 580(7802): 263-268, 2020 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32269334

RESUMEN

In cells, organs and whole organisms, nutrient sensing is key to maintaining homeostasis and adapting to a fluctuating environment1. In many animals, nutrient sensors are found within the enteroendocrine cells of the digestive system; however, less is known about nutrient sensing in their cellular siblings, the absorptive enterocytes1. Here we use a genetic screen in Drosophila melanogaster to identify Hodor, an ionotropic receptor in enterocytes that sustains larval development, particularly in nutrient-scarce conditions. Experiments in Xenopus oocytes and flies indicate that Hodor is a pH-sensitive, zinc-gated chloride channel that mediates a previously unrecognized dietary preference for zinc. Hodor controls systemic growth from a subset of enterocytes-interstitial cells-by promoting food intake and insulin/IGF signalling. Although Hodor sustains gut luminal acidity and restrains microbial loads, its effect on systemic growth results from the modulation of Tor signalling and lysosomal homeostasis within interstitial cells. Hodor-like genes are insect-specific, and may represent targets for the control of disease vectors. Indeed, CRISPR-Cas9 genome editing revealed that the single hodor orthologue in Anopheles gambiae is an essential gene. Our findings highlight the need to consider the instructive contributions of metals-and, more generally, micronutrients-to energy homeostasis.


Asunto(s)
Canales de Cloruro/metabolismo , Proteínas de Drosophila/metabolismo , Drosophila melanogaster/crecimiento & desarrollo , Drosophila melanogaster/metabolismo , Ingestión de Alimentos/fisiología , Intestinos/fisiología , Zinc/metabolismo , Animales , Drosophila melanogaster/genética , Enterocitos/metabolismo , Femenino , Preferencias Alimentarias , Homeostasis , Insectos Vectores , Insulina/metabolismo , Activación del Canal Iónico , Larva/genética , Larva/crecimiento & desarrollo , Larva/metabolismo , Lisosomas/metabolismo , Masculino , Oocitos/metabolismo , Proteínas Tirosina Quinasas Receptoras/metabolismo , Transducción de Señal , Xenopus
7.
Curr Biol ; 29(21): 3669-3680.e5, 2019 11 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31630950

RESUMEN

The mode of action for most mosquito repellents is unknown. This is primarily due to the difficulty in monitoring how the mosquito olfactory system responds to repellent odors. Here, we used the Q-system of binary expression to enable activity-dependent Ca2+ imaging in olfactory neurons of the African malaria mosquito Anopheles coluzzii. This system allows neuronal responses to common insect repellents to be directly visualized in living mosquitoes from all olfactory organs, including the antenna. The synthetic repellents N,N-diethyl-meta-toluamide (DEET) and IR3535 did not activate Anopheles odorant receptor co-receptor (Orco)-expressing olfactory receptor neurons (ORNs) at any concentration, and picaridin weakly activated ORNs only at high concentrations. In contrast, natural repellents (i.e. lemongrass oil and eugenol) strongly activated small numbers of ORNs in the Anopheles mosquito antennae at low concentrations. We determined that DEET, IR3535, and picaridin decrease the response of Orco-expressing ORNs when these repellents are physically mixed with activating human-derived odorants. We present evidence that synthetic repellents may primarily exert their olfactory mode of action by decreasing the amount of volatile odorants reaching ORNs. These results suggest that synthetic repellents disruptively change the chemical profile of host scent signatures on the skin surface, rendering humans invisible to Anopheles mosquitoes.


Asunto(s)
Anopheles/efectos de los fármacos , Anopheles/fisiología , Repelentes de Insectos/farmacología , Odorantes/análisis , Percepción Olfatoria/efectos de los fármacos , Olfato/efectos de los fármacos , Animales , DEET/química , DEET/farmacología , Femenino , Humanos , Repelentes de Insectos/química , Piperidinas/química , Piperidinas/farmacología , Propionatos/química , Propionatos/farmacología , Receptores Odorantes/metabolismo
8.
Curr Opin Insect Sci ; 36: 45-56, 2019 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31494407

RESUMEN

In the span of a decade we have seen a rapid progress in the application of genetic tools and genome editing approaches in 'non-model' insects. It is now possible to target sensory receptor genes and neurons, explore their functional roles and manipulate behavioral responses in these insects. In this review, we focus on the latest examples from Diptera, Lepidoptera and Hymenoptera of how applications of genetic tools advanced our understanding of diverse behavioral phenomena. We further discuss genetic methods that could be applied to study insect behavior in the future.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Animal , Insectos/genética , Insectos/fisiología , Animales , Conducta Apetitiva , Sistemas CRISPR-Cas , Mutagénesis Sitio-Dirigida , Oviposición , Interferencia de ARN , Conducta Social
9.
Genetics ; 212(1): 53-63, 2019 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30862621

RESUMEN

The Q-system is a binary expression system that works well across species. Here, we report the development and demonstrate the applications of a split-QF system that drives strong expression in Drosophila, is repressible by QS, and is inducible by a small nontoxic molecule (quinic acid). The split-QF system is fully compatible with existing split-GAL4 and split-LexA lines, thus greatly expanding the range of possible advanced intersectional experiments and anatomical, physiological, and behavioral assays in Drosophila, and in other organisms.


Asunto(s)
Drosophila/genética , Expresión Génica , Transgenes , Animales , Animales Modificados Genéticamente , Proteínas Bacterianas/genética , Proteínas de Drosophila/genética , Femenino , Técnicas Genéticas , Masculino , Ácido Quínico , Serina Endopeptidasas/genética , Factores de Transcripción/genética
10.
Nat Commun ; 7: 13010, 2016 10 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27694947

RESUMEN

Mosquitoes are vectors for multiple infectious human diseases and use a variety of sensory cues (olfactory, temperature, humidity and visual) to locate a human host. A comprehensive understanding of the circuitry underlying sensory signalling in the mosquito brain is lacking. Here we used the Q-system of binary gene expression to develop transgenic lines of Anopheles gambiae in which olfactory receptor neurons expressing the odorant receptor co-receptor (Orco) gene are labelled with GFP. These neurons project from the antennae and maxillary palps to the antennal lobe (AL) and from the labella on the proboscis to the suboesophageal zone (SEZ), suggesting integration of olfactory and gustatory signals occurs in this brain region. We present detailed anatomical maps of olfactory innervations in the AL and the SEZ, identifying glomeruli that may respond to human body odours or carbon dioxide. Our results pave the way for anatomical and functional neurogenetic studies of sensory processing in mosquitoes.


Asunto(s)
Anopheles/genética , Anopheles/metabolismo , Encéfalo/metabolismo , Olfato , Animales , Animales Modificados Genéticamente , Femenino , Perfilación de la Expresión Génica , Regulación de la Expresión Génica , Proteínas Fluorescentes Verdes/metabolismo , Malaria/transmisión , Masculino , Mosquitos Vectores , Neuronas/metabolismo , Vías Olfatorias/fisiología , Neuronas Receptoras Olfatorias/metabolismo , Receptores Odorantes/metabolismo , Olfato/fisiología , Temperatura
11.
Methods Mol Biol ; 1478: 53-78, 2016.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27730575

RESUMEN

Binary expression systems are flexible and versatile genetic tools in Drosophila. The Q-system is a recently developed repressible binary expression system that offers new possibilities for transgene expression and genetic manipulations. In this review chapter, we focus on current state-of-the-art Q-system tools and reagents. We also discuss in vivo applications of the Q-system, together with GAL4/UAS and LexA/LexAop systems, for simultaneous expression of multiple effectors, intersectional labeling, and clonal analysis.


Asunto(s)
Proteínas de Drosophila/genética , Drosophila melanogaster/genética , Regulación de la Expresión Génica , Vectores Genéticos/metabolismo , Neurospora crassa/genética , Factores de Transcripción/genética , Transgenes , Animales , Proteínas Bacterianas/genética , Proteínas Bacterianas/metabolismo , Proteínas de Drosophila/antagonistas & inhibidores , Proteínas de Drosophila/metabolismo , Drosophila melanogaster/metabolismo , Perfilación de la Expresión Génica , Genes Reporteros , Técnicas Genéticas , Vectores Genéticos/química , Neurospora crassa/metabolismo , ARN Interferente Pequeño/genética , ARN Interferente Pequeño/metabolismo , Proteínas Represoras/genética , Proteínas Represoras/metabolismo , Proteínas de Saccharomyces cerevisiae/genética , Proteínas de Saccharomyces cerevisiae/metabolismo , Serina Endopeptidasas/genética , Serina Endopeptidasas/metabolismo , Transducción de Señal , Factores de Transcripción/antagonistas & inhibidores , Factores de Transcripción/metabolismo , Transcripción Genética
12.
J Vis Exp ; (114)2016 08 20.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27585032

RESUMEN

A key challenge in neurobiology is to understand how neural circuits function to guide appropriate animal behaviors. Drosophila melanogaster is an excellent model system for such investigations due to its complex behaviors, powerful genetic techniques, and compact nervous system. Laboratory behavioral assays have long been used with Drosophila to simulate properties of the natural environment and study the neural mechanisms underlying the corresponding behaviors (e.g. phototaxis, chemotaxis, sensory learning and memory)(1-3). With the recent availability of large collections of transgenic Drosophila lines that label specific neural subsets, behavioral assays have taken on a prominent role to link neurons with behaviors(4-11). Versatile and reproducible paradigms, together with the underlying computational routines for data analysis, are indispensable for rapid tests of candidate fly lines with various genotypes. Particularly useful are setups that are flexible in the number of animals tested, duration of experiments and nature of presented stimuli. The assay of choice should also generate reproducible data that is easy to acquire and analyze. Here, we present a detailed description of a system and protocol for assaying behavioral responses of Drosophila flies in a large four-field arena. The setup is used here to assay responses of flies to a single olfactory stimulus; however, the same setup may be modified to test multiple olfactory, visual or optogenetic stimuli, or a combination of these. The olfactometer setup records the activity of fly populations responding to odors, and computational analytical methods are applied to quantify fly behaviors. The collected data are analyzed to get a quick read-out of an experimental run, which is essential for efficient data collection and the optimization of experimental conditions.


Asunto(s)
Quimiotaxis/fisiología , Drosophila melanogaster/genética , Drosophila melanogaster/fisiología , Animales , Conducta Animal , Drosophila , Aprendizaje , Memoria , Percepción Olfatoria
13.
Nat Neurosci ; 18(6): 917-25, 2015 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25961791

RESUMEN

Intracellular Ca(2+) is a widely used neuronal activity indicator. Here we describe a transcriptional reporter of intracellular Ca(2+) (TRIC) in Drosophila that uses a binary expression system to report Ca(2+)-dependent interactions between calmodulin and its target peptide. We found that in vitro assays predicted in vivo properties of TRIC and that TRIC signals in sensory systems depend on neuronal activity. TRIC was able to quantitatively monitor neuronal responses that changed slowly, such as those of neuropeptide F-expressing neurons to sexual deprivation and neuroendocrine pars intercerebralis cells to food and arousal. Furthermore, TRIC-induced expression of a neuronal silencer in nutrient-activated cells enhanced stress resistance, providing a proof of principle that TRIC can be used for circuit manipulation. Thus, TRIC facilitates the monitoring and manipulation of neuronal activity, especially those reflecting slow changes in physiological states that are poorly captured by existing methods. TRIC's modular design should enable optimization and adaptation to other organisms.


Asunto(s)
Señalización del Calcio/genética , Señalización del Calcio/fisiología , Calcio/fisiología , Animales , Células Cultivadas , Drosophila , Datos de Secuencia Molecular , Mutagénesis , Vías Nerviosas/metabolismo , Vías Nerviosas/fisiología , Vías Olfatorias/metabolismo , Vías Olfatorias/fisiología , Lóbulo Óptico de Animales no Mamíferos/citología , Lóbulo Óptico de Animales no Mamíferos/fisiología , Estrés Psicológico/genética , Estrés Psicológico/fisiopatología , Transmisión Sináptica/genética , Transmisión Sináptica/fisiología , Vías Visuales/metabolismo , Vías Visuales/fisiología
14.
Nat Methods ; 12(3): 219-22, 5 p following 222, 2015 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25581800

RESUMEN

The Q system is a repressible binary expression system for transgenic manipulations in living organisms. Through protein engineering and in vivo functional tests, we report here variants of the Q-system transcriptional activator, including QF2, for driving strong and ubiquitous expression in all Drosophila tissues. Our QF2, Gal4QF and LexAQF chimeric transcriptional activators substantially enrich the toolkit available for transgenic regulation in Drosophila melanogaster.


Asunto(s)
Drosophila melanogaster/genética , Ingeniería Genética/métodos , Transactivadores/genética , Transgenes , Animales , Animales Modificados Genéticamente , Conducta Animal , Proteínas de Drosophila/genética , Drosophila melanogaster/embriología , Embrión no Mamífero , Femenino , Regulación de la Expresión Génica , Proteínas Fluorescentes Verdes/genética , Larva , Masculino , Regiones Promotoras Genéticas , Sueño/genética , Factores de Transcripción/genética
15.
J Exp Biol ; 217(Pt 15): 2633-42, 2014 Aug 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25079890

RESUMEN

Insects inform themselves about the 3D structure of their surroundings through motion parallax. During flight, they often simplify this task by minimising rotational image movement. Coordinated head and body movements generate rapid shifts of gaze separated by periods of almost zero rotational movement, during which the distance of objects from the insect can be estimated through pure translational optic flow. This saccadic strategy is less appropriate for assessing the distance between objects. Bees and wasps face this problem when learning the position of their nest-hole relative to objects close to it. They acquire the necessary information during specialised flights performed on leaving the nest. Here, we show that the bumblebee's saccadic strategy differs from other reported cases. In the fixations between saccades, a bumblebee's head continues to turn slowly, generating rotational flow. At specific points in learning flights these imperfect fixations generate a form of 'pivoting parallax', which is centred on the nest and enhances the visibility of features near the nest. Bumblebees may thus utilize an alternative form of motion parallax to that delivered by the standard 'saccade and fixate' strategy in which residual rotational flow plays a role in assessing the distances of objects from a focal point of interest.


Asunto(s)
Abejas/fisiología , Vuelo Animal/fisiología , Movimientos de la Cabeza , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Flujo Optico/fisiología , Animales , Movimiento , Orientación/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Percepción Espacial/fisiología
16.
J Exp Biol ; 216(Pt 6): 1093-104, 2013 Mar 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23447668

RESUMEN

Many wasps and bees learn the position of their nest relative to nearby visual features during elaborate 'learning' flights that they perform on leaving the nest. Return flights to the nest are thought to be patterned so that insects can reach their nest by matching their current view to views of their surroundings stored during learning flights. To understand how ground-nesting bumblebees might implement such a matching process, we have video-recorded the bees' learning and return flights and analysed the similarities and differences between the principal motifs of their flights. Loops that take bees away from and bring them back towards the nest are common during learning flights and less so in return flights. Zigzags are more prominent on return flights. Both motifs tend to be nest based. Bees often both fly towards and face the nest in the middle of loops and at the turns of zigzags. Before and after flight direction and body orientation are aligned, the two diverge from each other so that the nest is held within the bees' fronto-lateral visual field while flight direction relative to the nest can fluctuate more widely. These and other parallels between loops and zigzags suggest that they are stable variations of an underlying pattern, which enable bees to store and reacquire similar nest-focused views during learning and return flights.


Asunto(s)
Abejas/fisiología , Vuelo Animal/fisiología , Fenómenos de Retorno al Lugar Habitual/fisiología , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Orientación/fisiología , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Animales , Estadísticas no Paramétricas , Grabación en Video
17.
J Exp Biol ; 216(Pt 6): 1105-13, 2013 Mar 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23447669

RESUMEN

Bumblebees tend to face their nest over a limited range of compass directions when learning the nest's location on departure and finding it on their approach after foraging. They thus obtain similar views of the nest and its surroundings on their learning and return flights. How do bees coordinate their flights relative to nest-based and compass-based reference frames to get such similar views? We show, first, that learning and return flights contain straight segments that are directed along particular compass bearings, which are independent of the orientation of a bee's body. Bees are thus free within limits to adjust their viewing direction relative to the nest, without disturbing flight direction. Second, we examine the coordination of nest-based and compass-based control during likely information gathering segments of these flights: loops during learning flights and zigzags on return flights. We find that bees tend to start a loop or zigzag when flying within a restricted range of compass directions and to fly towards the nest and face it after a fixed change in compass direction, without continuous interactions between their nest-based and compass-based directions of flight. A preferred trajectory of compass-based flight over the course of a motif, combined with the tendency of the bees to keep their body oriented towards the nest automatically narrows the range of compass directions over which bees view the nest. Additionally, the absence of interactions between the two reference frames allows loops and zigzags to have a stereotyped form that can generate informative visual feedback.


Asunto(s)
Abejas/fisiología , Vuelo Animal/fisiología , Fenómenos de Retorno al Lugar Habitual/fisiología , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Orientación/fisiología , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Animales , Grabación en Video
18.
J Exp Biol ; 214(Pt 16): 2739-48, 2011 Aug 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21795571

RESUMEN

The visually guided foraging routes of some formicine ants are individually stereotyped, suggesting the importance of visual learning in maintaining these routes. We ask here whether the wood ant Formica rufa learns a sequence of visual features encountered at different stages along a route, as reported for honeybees. We trained ants in several simple mazes to follow two alternative routes. Along each two-stage route, the ants first encountered one of two priming stimuli. The identity of the priming stimulus determined which of two choice stimuli was rewarded in the second stage of the route. As stimuli we used ultraviolet and yellow/green light panels, and two black-and-white patterns. Did ants learn to pair each colour with the appropriate black-and-white pattern? Ants learnt readily to discriminate between the two coloured stimuli or between the two black-and-white patterns. They could also pair coloured and black-and-white patterns, provided that the two were presented simultaneously. The ants' behaviour with sequential stimuli varied according to whether the priming stimulus was a coloured stimulus or a black-and-white pattern. When the priming stimulus was coloured, ants seemed to learn the two sequences, but tests showed that their success was probably caused by the after-effects of colour adaptation. With a black-and-white priming stimulus and a coloured second stage stimulus, robust sequential learning could not be demonstrated, although under certain experimental conditions a tiny proportion of ants did acquire the sequence. Thus, ants perform conditional discriminations reliably when priming and choice stimuli are simultaneous, but they usually fail when the stimuli are sequential.


Asunto(s)
Hormigas/fisiología , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa , Madera , Postimagen/fisiología , Animales , Color , Discriminación en Psicología/fisiología , Orientación/fisiología
19.
Curr Biol ; 21(8): 658-64, 2011 Apr 26.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21458268

RESUMEN

The courtship behavior of Drosophilid flies has served as a long-standing model for studying the bases of animal communication. During courtship, male flies flap their wings to send a complex pattern of airborne vibrations to the antennal ears of the females. These "courtship songs" differ in their spectrotemporal composition across species and are considered a crucial component of the flies' premating barrier. However, whether the species-specific differences in song structure are also reflected in the receivers of this communication system, i.e., the flies' antennal ears, has remained unexplored. Here we show for seven members of the melanogaster species group that (1) their ears are mechanically tuned to different best frequencies, (2) the ears' best frequencies correlate with high-frequency pulses of the conspecific courtship songs, and (3) the species-specific tuning relies on amplificatory mechanical feedback from the flies' auditory neurons. As a result of its level-dependent nature, the active mechanical feedback amplification is particularly useful for the detection of small stimuli, such as conspecific song pulses, and becomes negligible for sensing larger stimuli, such as the flies' own wingbeat during flight.


Asunto(s)
Drosophila melanogaster/fisiología , Drosophila/fisiología , Comunicación Animal , Animales , Antenas de Artrópodos/fisiología , Vías Auditivas/fisiología , Percepción Auditiva , Cortejo , Femenino , Masculino , Conducta Sexual Animal , Especificidad de la Especie , Vibración , Alas de Animales/fisiología
20.
J Exp Biol ; 212(Pt 20): 3193-204, 2009 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19801423

RESUMEN

Many bees and wasps learn about the immediate surroundings of their nest during learning flights, in which they look back towards the nest and acquire visual information that guides their subsequent returns. Visual guidance to the nest is simplified by the insects' tendency to adopt similar viewing directions during learning and return flights. To understand better the factors determining the particular viewing directions that insects choose, we have recorded the learning and return flights of a ground-nesting bumblebee in two visual environments--an enclosed garden with a partly open view between north and west, and a flat roof with a more open panorama. In both places, bees left and returned to an inconspicuous nest hole in the centre of a tabletop, with the hole marked by one or more nearby cylinders. In all experiments, bees adopted similar preferred orientations on their learning and return flights. Bees faced predominantly either north or south, suggesting the existence of two attractors. The bees' selection between attractors seems to be influenced both by the distribution of light, as determined by the shape of the skyline, and by the direction of wind. In the partly enclosed garden with little or no wind, bees tended to face north throughout the day, i.e. towards the pole in the brighter half of their surroundings. When white curtains, which distributed skylight more evenly, were placed around the table, bees faced both north and south. The bees on the roof tended to face south or north when the wind came from a wide arc of directions from the south or north, respectively. We suggest that bees switch facing orientation between north and south as a compromise between maintaining a single viewing direction for efficient view-based navigation and responding to the distribution of light for the easier detection of landmarks seen against the ground or to the direction of the wind for exploiting olfactory cues.


Asunto(s)
Abejas , Vuelo Animal , Fenómenos de Retorno al Lugar Habitual/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Conducta Espacial/fisiología , Animales , Percepción de Color , Señales (Psicología) , Aprendizaje , Odorantes , Orientación , Avispas , Viento
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA
...