Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 5 de 5
Filtrar
Más filtros










Base de datos
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Front Psychol ; 12: 785283, 2021.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35002876

RESUMEN

Black Americans who are perceived as more racially phenotypical-that is, who possess more physical traits that are closely associated with their race-are more often associated with racial stereotypes. These stereotypes, including assumptions about criminality, can influence how Black Americans are treated by the legal system. However, it is unclear whether other forms of racial stereotypicality, such as a person's way of speaking, also activate stereotypes about Black Americans. We investigated the links between speech stereotypicality and racial stereotypes (Experiment 1) and racial phenotype bias (Experiment 2). In Experiment 1, participants listened to audio recordings of Black speakers and rated how stereotypical they found the speaker, the likely race and nationality of the speaker, and indicated which adjectives the average person would likely associate with this speaker. In Experiment 2, participants listened to recordings of weakly or strongly stereotypical Black American speakers and indicated which of two faces (either weakly or strongly phenotypical) was more likely to be the speaker's. We found that speakers whose voices were rated as more highly stereotypical for Black Americans were more likely to be associated with stereotypes about Black Americans (Experiment 1) and with more stereotypically Black faces (Experiment 2). These findings indicate that speech stereotypicality activates racial stereotypes as well as expectations about the stereotypicality of an individual's appearance. As a result, the activation of stereotypes based on speech may lead to bias in suspect descriptions or eyewitness identifications.

2.
Int J Clin Exp Hypn ; 63(3): 274-83, 2015.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25978081

RESUMEN

Hypnotizability is a multifaceted construct that may relate to multiple aspects of personality and beliefs. This study sought to address 4 known correlates of hypnotizability to aid in its understanding. Eighty undergraduates completed the Magical Ideation Scale (MIS), the Creative Experiences Questionnaire (CEQ), the Australian Sheep-Goat Scale (ASGS), and the Dissociative Experiences Scale (DES) and then were administered the Creative Imagination Scale (CIS). All 5 scales were significantly correlated. Participants higher in hypnotizability scored higher on the CEQ and the MIS. The findings demonstrate the influence of fantasy proneness and magical thinking on hypnotizability and support the theory that hypnotizability is a complex interaction of multiple traits.


Asunto(s)
Cultura , Trastornos Disociativos/psicología , Fantasía , Hipnosis/métodos , Magia , Parapsicología , Adolescente , Creatividad , Femenino , Humanos , Imaginación , Individualidad , Masculino , Psicometría/estadística & datos numéricos , Estadística como Asunto , Estudiantes/psicología , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Adulto Joven
3.
Int J Clin Exp Hypn ; 63(1): 110-28, 2015.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25365130

RESUMEN

Hypnotizability influences the development of false memories. In Experiment 1, participants heard a positive or negative suggestion regarding hypnosis and then listened to 8 Deese-Roediger-McDermott (DRM) false memory paradigm lists in a hypnotic state. Neither hypnosis nor prehypnotic suggestion affected memory. Highly hypnotizable participants were more accurate in recall and recognition. In Experiment 2, suggestions were delivered in the form of feedback. Participants heard a positive or negative suggestion about their performance prior to either the encoding or retrieval of 8 DRM lists. Neither accurate nor false memories were affected by the suggestion. Highly hypnotizable individuals recognized fewer critical lures if they received a negative suggestion about their performance. These results highlight the unusual role of hypnotizability in the creation of false memories.


Asunto(s)
Hipnosis , Represión Psicológica , Sugestión , Adolescente , Humanos , Recuerdo Mental , Reconocimiento en Psicología , Adulto Joven
4.
Am J Psychol ; 117(4): 517-30, 2004.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15605956

RESUMEN

After September 11, 2001, we distributed flashbulb memory questionnaires at 5 different dates: within 48 hr (T1) and at 1 week (T2), 1 month (T3), 3 months (T4), and 1 year (T5). We scored responses for self-reported memory (veracity unverified), memory accuracy (recollection-matched T1 response), and memory consistency (recollection-matched prior responses other than T1). Self-reported memory and subjective confidence remained near ceiling, although the accuracy declined. However, memories given a week or more after September 11 were consistent throughout. We hypothesize that flashbulb memories follow a consolidation-like process: Some details learned later are incorporated into the initial memory, and many others are discarded. After this process, memories stabilize. Therefore, the best predictor of flashbulb memories at long intervals is not the memory as initially reported but memories reported a week or more after the event.


Asunto(s)
Acontecimientos que Cambian la Vida , Memoria , Ataques Terroristas del 11 de Septiembre , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
5.
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn ; 29(6): 1058-65, 2003 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14622046

RESUMEN

The transfer-appropriate monitoring (TAM) hypothesis of metamemory predicts that judgment of learning (JOL) accuracy should improve when conditions during JOLs closely match conditions of the memory test. The authors devised 5 types of delayed JOLs for paired associates and varied them along with the type of memory test (cued recall or recognition). If the TAM hypothesis is correct, JOL and test type should interact to influence metamemory. Contrary to TAM, metamemory accuracy did not improve when JOL and test conditions matched but instead tended to vary according to whether the answer was apparent at time of JOL. Memory test scores and JOL magnitude were both greater when the correct target was evident during JOLs. Overall, the results are largely consistent with a monitoring retrieval view of delayed JOLs and do not support TAM as a viable account of JOL accuracy.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Juicio , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Recuerdo Mental , Aprendizaje por Asociación de Pares , Transferencia de Experiencia en Psicología , Señales (Psicología) , Humanos , Lectura , Retención en Psicología
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA
...