Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 1.058
Filtrar
1.
Optom Vis Sci ; 101(6): 393-398, 2024 Jun 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38990237

RESUMEN

SIGNIFICANCE: It is important to know whether early-onset vision loss and late-onset vision loss are associated with differences in the estimation of distances of sound sources within the environment. People with vision loss rely heavily on auditory cues for path planning, safe navigation, avoiding collisions, and activities of daily living. PURPOSE: Loss of vision can lead to substantial changes in auditory abilities. It is unclear whether differences in sound distance estimation exist in people with early-onset partial vision loss, late-onset partial vision loss, and normal vision. We investigated distance estimates for a range of sound sources and auditory environments in groups of participants with early- or late-onset partial visual loss and sighted controls. METHODS: Fifty-two participants heard static sounds with virtual distances ranging from 1.2 to 13.8 m within a simulated room. The room simulated either anechoic (no echoes) or reverberant environments. Stimuli were speech, music, or noise. Single sounds were presented, and participants reported the estimated distance of the sound source. Each participant took part in 480 trials. RESULTS: Analysis of variance showed significant main effects of visual status (p<0.05) environment (reverberant vs. anechoic, p<0.05) and also of the stimulus (p<0.05). Significant differences (p<0.05) were shown in the estimation of distances of sound sources between early-onset visually impaired participants and sighted controls for closer distances for all conditions except the anechoic speech condition and at middle distances for all conditions except the reverberant speech and music conditions. Late-onset visually impaired participants and sighted controls showed similar performance (p>0.05). CONCLUSIONS: The findings suggest that early-onset partial vision loss results in significant changes in judged auditory distance in different environments, especially for close and middle distances. Late-onset partial visual loss has less of an impact on the ability to estimate the distance of sound sources. The findings are consistent with a theoretical framework, the perceptual restructuring hypothesis, which was recently proposed to account for the effects of vision loss on audition.


Asunto(s)
Localización de Sonidos , Humanos , Masculino , Femenino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Anciano , Adulto , Localización de Sonidos/fisiología , Juicio , Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Percepción de Distancia/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica/métodos , Adulto Joven , Agudeza Visual/fisiología , Edad de Inicio , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Señales (Psicología)
2.
Exp Brain Res ; 242(8): 2023-2031, 2024 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38953973

RESUMEN

The influence of travel time on perceived traveled distance has often been studied, but the results are inconsistent regarding the relationship between the two magnitudes. We argue that this is due to differences in the lengths of investigated travel distances and hypothesize that the influence of travel time differs for rather short compared to rather long traveled distances. We tested this hypothesis in a virtual environment presented on a desktop as well as through a head-mounted display. Our results show that, for longer distances, more travel time leads to longer perceived distance, while we do not find an influence of travel time on shorter distances. The presentation through an HMD vs. desktop only influenced distance judgments in the short distance condition. These results are in line with the idea that the influence of travel time varies by the length of the traveled distance, and provide insights on the question of how distance perception in path integration studies is affected by travel time, thereby resolving inconsistencies reported in previous studies.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Distancia , Humanos , Percepción de Distancia/fisiología , Femenino , Masculino , Adulto Joven , Adulto , Factores de Tiempo , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Realidad Virtual , Juicio/fisiología
3.
Elife ; 122024 Jul 18.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39023517

RESUMEN

We reliably judge locations of static objects when we walk despite the retinal images of these objects moving with every step we take. Here, we showed our brains solve this optical illusion by adopting an allocentric spatial reference frame. We measured perceived target location after the observer walked a short distance from the home base. Supporting the allocentric coding scheme, we found the intrinsic bias , which acts as a spatial reference frame for perceiving location of a dimly lit target in the dark, remained grounded at the home base rather than traveled along with the observer. The path-integration mechanism responsible for this can utilize both active and passive (vestibular) translational motion signals, but only along the horizontal direction. This asymmetric path-integration finding in human visual space perception is reminiscent of the asymmetric spatial memory finding in desert ants, pointing to nature's wondrous and logically simple design for terrestrial creatures.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Distancia , Humanos , Percepción de Distancia/fisiología , Masculino , Femenino , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Adulto , Adulto Joven , Ilusiones Ópticas/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología
4.
eNeuro ; 11(6)2024 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38844346

RESUMEN

In measurement, a reference frame is needed to compare the measured object to something already known. This raises the neuroscientific question of which reference frame is used by humans when exploring the environment. Previous studies suggested that, in touch, the body employed as measuring tool also serves as reference frame. Indeed, an artificial modification of the perceived dimensions of the body changes the tactile perception of external object dimensions. However, it is unknown if such a change in tactile perception would occur when the body schema is modified through the illusion of owning a limb altered in size. Therefore, employing a virtual hand illusion paradigm with an elongated forearm of different lengths, we systematically tested the subjective perception of distance between two points [tactile distance perception (TDP) task] on the corresponding real forearm following the illusion. Thus, the TDP task is used as a proxy to gauge changes in the body schema. Embodiment of the virtual arm was found significantly greater after the synchronous visuotactile stimulation condition compared with the asynchronous one, and the forearm elongation significantly increased the TDP. However, we did not find any link between the visuotactile-induced ownership over the elongated arm and TDP variation, suggesting that vision plays the main role in the modification of the body schema. Additionally, significant effect of elongation found on TDP but not on proprioception suggests that these are affected differently by body schema modifications. These findings confirm the body schema malleability and its role as a reference frame in touch.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Distancia , Ilusiones , Percepción del Tacto , Realidad Virtual , Humanos , Femenino , Masculino , Percepción del Tacto/fisiología , Adulto Joven , Adulto , Ilusiones/fisiología , Percepción de Distancia/fisiología , Propiocepción/fisiología , Imagen Corporal , Antebrazo/fisiología
5.
J AAPOS ; 28(3): 103904, 2024 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38552945

RESUMEN

Distance stereoacuity measurement enables the evaluation and management of binocular vision disorders. Here, we compare the results obtained using standard tests for distance stereoacuity measurement with the novel STab test. We tested 87 children (4-17 years of age) using different tests for the quantification of stereopsis at distance: Distance Randot Stereotest (DRS), M&S random dots (M&S), and STab. A strong correlation was demonstrated between M&S-DRS (0.8), M&S-STab (0.81), DRS-STab (0.85) (all P < 0.0001). The limit of agreement between M&S and DRS was 0.45; between M&S and STab, 0.47; and between DRS and STab, 0.38. Our results suggest that all three methods can be used interchangeably.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Profundidad , Pruebas de Visión , Visión Binocular , Agudeza Visual , Humanos , Niño , Preescolar , Percepción de Profundidad/fisiología , Adolescente , Agudeza Visual/fisiología , Femenino , Masculino , Pruebas de Visión/métodos , Visión Binocular/fisiología , Percepción de Distancia/fisiología
6.
Neuropsychologia ; 193: 108746, 2024 Jan 29.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38081353

RESUMEN

A stable representation of object size, in spite of continuous variations in retinal input due to changes in viewing distance, is critical for perceiving and acting in a real 3D world. In fact, our perceptual and visuo-motor systems exhibit size and grip constancies in order to compensate for the natural shrinkage of the retinal image with increased distance. The neural basis of this size-distance scaling remains largely unknown, although multiple lines of evidence suggest that size-constancy operations might take place remarkably early, already at the level of the primary visual cortex. In this study, we examined for the first time the temporal dynamics of size constancy during perception and action by using a combined measurement of event-related potentials (ERPs) and kinematics. Participants were asked to maintain their gaze steadily on a fixation point and perform either a manual estimation or a grasping task towards disks of different sizes placed at different distances. Importantly, the physical size of the target was scaled with distance to yield a constant retinal angle. Meanwhile, we recorded EEG data from 64 scalp electrodes and hand movements with a motion capture system. We focused on the first positive-going visual evoked component peaking at approximately 90 ms after stimulus onset. We found earlier latencies and greater amplitudes in response to bigger than smaller disks of matched retinal size, regardless of the task. In line with the ERP results, manual estimates and peak grip apertures were larger for the bigger targets. We also found task-related differences at later stages of processing from a cluster of central electrodes, whereby the mean amplitude of the P2 component was greater for manual estimation than grasping. Taken together, these findings provide novel evidence that size constancy for real objects at real distances occurs at the earliest cortical stages and that early visual processing does not change as a function of task demands.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Distancia , Percepción Visual , Humanos , Percepción de Distancia/fisiología , Fenómenos Biomecánicos , Movimiento , Electroencefalografía , Percepción del Tamaño/fisiología
7.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 378(1869): 20210454, 2023 01 30.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36511412

RESUMEN

The dominant inferential approach to human 3D perception assumes a model of spatial encoding based on a physical description of objects and space. Prevailing models based on this physicalist approach assume that the visual system infers an objective, unitary and mostly veridical representation of the external world. However, careful consideration of the phenomenology of 3D perception challenges these assumptions. I review important aspects of phenomenology, psychophysics and neurophysiology which suggest that human visual perception of 3D  objects and space is underwritten by distinct and dissociated spatial encodings that are optimized for specific regions of space. Specifically, I argue that 3D perception is underwritten by at least three distinct encodings for (1) egocentric distance perception at the ambulatory scale, (2) exocentric distance (scaled depth) perception optimized for near space, and (3) perception of object shape and layout (unscaled depth). This tripartite division can more satisfactorily account for the phenomenology, psychophysics and adaptive logic of human 3D perception. This article is part of a discussion meeting issue 'New approaches to 3D vision'.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Profundidad , Percepción de Distancia , Humanos , Percepción de Profundidad/fisiología , Psicofísica , Percepción de Distancia/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Percepción Espacial
8.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 48(5): 467-480, 2022 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35389709

RESUMEN

This study aimed to test the hypothesis that associates blindness with a reduced ability to judge the absolute distance from sound sources. Our working hypotheses were the following: (a) Within reach, a blind subject will be able to make up for the lack of vision using proprioceptive information to calibrate the acoustic distance perception cues. (b) As the source becomes unreachable, blind people will show greater biases since, out of reach, the proposed mechanism for calibration could not be used. To approach these topics, we carried out a series of auditory distance experiments in which we asked sighted and blind participants to report their distance estimates verbally or by reaching the sound source. Within-reach results showed that blind participants performed better than (reaching) or similar to (verbal report) the sighted. The verbal report results showed similar biases between both groups. However, blind participants had more compressive responses than the sighted. Furthermore, blind participants showed more biased responses in the far field than in the peripersonal space, while sighted participants showed similar biases regardless of distance. Our results strongly suggest that the blind can calibrate their distance estimations through the use of proprioceptive spatial information. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Percepción Auditiva , Percepción de Distancia , Estimulación Acústica , Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Ceguera , Señales (Psicología) , Percepción de Distancia/fisiología , Humanos , Percepción Espacial/fisiología
9.
Vision Res ; 195: 108024, 2022 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35286904

RESUMEN

The perceived size of an object depends on its spatial context, in addition to its projected image on the retina and perceived distance. However, how these factors interact with each other to affect perceived object size is still not clear. In this study, we manipulated the binocular disparity of images to assess the effect of perceived distance on perceived object size, as well as background element size to assess the effect of context. The perceived target size under different combinations of perceived distance and context was measured with a two-interval forced-choice paradigm, in which one interval contained a standard disk with a textured background while the other contained a comparison disk on a blank background in each trial. The observers were instructed to indicate which interval contained a larger disk. A staircase procedure was used to measure the point of subjective equality for the perceived target size. Our results showed that the perceived target size increased with the perceived distance while decreased with background element size. In addition, context modulated the relationship between the perceived target size and perceived distance. The data can be explained by a computational model that incorporates several size selective channels whose size sensitivity to a stimulus can be modulated by its disparity. The target response of each channel is subjected to the divisive inhibition signal from the size information in the context. The perceived size is determined by the weighted average of the responses of these size channels. This model can explain more than 91% of variability in the averaged data. Thus, while both perceived distance and context can affect the perceived size of an object, they exert the effect through different mechanisms.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Distancia , Disparidad Visual , Percepción de Distancia/fisiología , Humanos , Percepción del Tamaño/fisiología
10.
Behav Brain Res ; 418: 113622, 2022 02 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34648795

RESUMEN

Headbobs are up-down movements of the cranium associated with the use of motion parallax for depth perception. Mongolian gerbils (aka jirds; Meriones unguiculatus) often execute a series of headbobs prior to jumping between surfaces. Gerbils were tested in a jumping stand task and headbobs videotaped under three light levels approximating low daylight, dawn/dusk, and moonlight across a range of distances to target. Headbobs per trial increased linearly with increasing distance to the target platform, whereas headbob frequency (rate of headbobbing pre-jump on the start platform) increased with gap distance up to an intermediate level and then decreased. Overall, gerbils made the most headbobs per trial under the darkest conditions, whereas their headbobbing rate was highest for medium illumination, especially for medium-long gap distances. There was a positive correlation between headbob frequency and volume of the superior colliculus (SC), but no relationship between headbobs and relative size of the temporo-posterior (TP) visual cortex. The results suggest that gerbils employ a specific visuomotor strategy for depth perception differentially under different conditions. We suggest that the deployment of headbobs under specific conditions may be part of an SC-driven vigilant state, of which more rapid sampling of the visual environment using headbobs for depth estimation is one component. Moreover, the findings highlight the importance of considering ecological factors in designing studies of visual behavior and its underpinnings in rodents.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Animal/fisiología , Percepción de Distancia/fisiología , Gerbillinae/fisiología , Cabeza , Iluminación , Movimiento , Animales , Potenciales Evocados , Masculino , Colículos Superiores/ultraestructura , Corteza Visual
11.
Sci Rep ; 11(1): 23312, 2021 12 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34857779

RESUMEN

To avoid collisions, pedestrians depend on their ability to perceive and interpret the visual motion of other road users. Eye movements influence motion perception, yet pedestrians' gaze behavior has been little investigated. In the present study, we ask whether observers sample visual information differently when making two types of judgements based on the same virtual road-crossing scenario and to which extent spontaneous gaze behavior affects those judgements. Participants performed in succession a speed and a time-to-arrival two-interval discrimination task on the same simple traffic scenario-a car approaching at a constant speed (varying from 10 to 90 km/h) on a single-lane road. On average, observers were able to discriminate vehicle speeds of around 18 km/h and times-to-arrival of 0.7 s. In both tasks, observers placed their gaze closely towards the center of the vehicle's front plane while pursuing the vehicle. Other areas of the visual scene were sampled infrequently. No differences were found in the average gaze behavior between the two tasks and a pattern classifier (Support Vector Machine), trained on trial-level gaze patterns, failed to reliably classify the task from the spontaneous eye movements it elicited. Saccadic gaze behavior could predict time-to-arrival discrimination performance, demonstrating the relevance of gaze behavior for perceptual sensitivity in road-crossing.


Asunto(s)
Accidentes de Tránsito/prevención & control , Toma de Decisiones/fisiología , Percepción de Distancia/fisiología , Fijación Ocular/fisiología , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Vehículos a Motor , Peatones/psicología , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Percepción del Tiempo/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Juicio/fisiología , Masculino , Adulto Joven
12.
Cell Rep ; 35(1): 108934, 2021 04 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33826896

RESUMEN

Cortical activity related to erroneous behavior in discrimination or decision-making tasks is rarely analyzed, yet it can help clarify which computations are essential during a specific task. Here, we use a hidden Markov model (HMM) to perform a trial-by-trial analysis of the ensemble activity of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (PFdl) neurons of rhesus monkeys performing a distance discrimination task. By segmenting the neural activity into sequences of metastable states, HMM allows us to uncover modulations of the neural dynamics related to internal computations. We find that metastable dynamics slow down during error trials, while state transitions at a pivotal point during the trial take longer in difficult correct trials. Both these phenomena occur during the decision interval, with errors occurring in both easy and difficult trials. Our results provide further support for the emerging role of metastable cortical dynamics in mediating complex cognitive functions and behavior.


Asunto(s)
Discriminación en Psicología , Percepción de Distancia/fisiología , Corteza Prefrontal/fisiología , Análisis y Desempeño de Tareas , Animales , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Cadenas de Markov , Neuronas/fisiología
13.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 28(3): 845-852, 2021 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33501593

RESUMEN

Spatial distance of response keys has been shown to have an effect on nonspatial tasks in that performance improved if the spatial distance increased. Comparably, spatial distance of stimulus features has been shown to have a performance-improving effect in a (partly) spatial task. Here, we combined these two findings in the same task to test for the commonality of the effect of stimulus distance and the effect of response distance. Thus, we varied spatial distance in exactly the same fashion either between stimuli or between responses in a standard Eriksen flanker task. The results show that spatial distance only affected the processing of stimulus features, while it had no effect on the processing of response features. Regarding the idea of common coding of action and perception (Prinz, 1990), stimulus and response processing should be influenced by spatial distance in the same way so that our data might suggest a boundary for the idea of common coding.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Distancia/fisiología , Actividad Motora/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
14.
Neuroimage ; 229: 117730, 2021 04 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33454399

RESUMEN

Psychophysical experiments have demonstrated large and highly systematic perceptual distortions of tactile space. Such a space can be referred to our experience of the spatial organisation of objects, at representational level, through touch, in analogy with the familiar concept of visual space. We investigated the neural basis of tactile space by analysing activity patterns induced by tactile stimulation of nine points on a 3 × 3 square grid on the hand dorsum using functional magnetic resonance imaging. We used a searchlight approach within pre-defined regions of interests to compute the pairwise Euclidean distances between the activity patterns elicited by tactile stimulation. Then, we used multidimensional scaling to reconstruct tactile space at the neural level and compare it with skin space at the perceptual level. Our reconstructions of the shape of skin space in contralateral primary somatosensory and motor cortices reveal that it is distorted in a way that matches the perceptual shape of skin space. This suggests that early sensorimotor areas critically contribute to the distorted internal representation of tactile space on the hand dorsum.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Distancia/fisiología , Mano/fisiología , Corteza Motora/fisiología , Fenómenos Fisiológicos de la Piel , Corteza Somatosensorial/fisiología , Percepción del Tacto/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Mano/inervación , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética/métodos , Masculino , Corteza Motora/diagnóstico por imagen , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Estimulación Física/métodos , Corteza Somatosensorial/diagnóstico por imagen , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Adulto Joven
15.
Sci Rep ; 11(1): 494, 2021 01 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33436801

RESUMEN

There is growing interest in how human observers perceive social scenes containing multiple people. Interpersonal distance is a critical feature when appraising these scenes; proxemic cues are used by observers to infer whether two people are interacting, the nature of their relationship, and the valence of their current interaction. Presently, however, remarkably little is known about how interpersonal distance is encoded within the human visual system. Here we show that the perception of interpersonal distance is distorted by the Müller-Lyer illusion. Participants perceived the distance between two target points to be compressed or expanded depending on whether face pairs were positioned inside or outside the to-be-judged interval. This illusory bias was found to be unaffected by manipulations of face direction. These findings aid our understanding of how human observers perceive interpersonal distance and may inform theoretical accounts of the Müller-Lyer illusion.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Distancia/fisiología , Ilusiones Ópticas/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Percepción del Tamaño/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Adulto Joven
16.
Optom Vis Sci ; 98(1): 3-12, 2021 01 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33394925

RESUMEN

SIGNIFICANCE: This study presents the relationship between distance visual acuity and a range of uncorrected refractive errors, a complex association that is fundamental to clinical eye care and the identification of children needing refractive correction. PURPOSE: This study aimed to analyze data from the Collaborative Longitudinal Evaluation of Ethnicity and Refractive Error Study to describe the relationship between distance uncorrected refractive error and visual acuity in children. METHODS: Subjects were 2212 children (51.2% female) 6 to 14 years of age (mean ± standard deviation, 10.2 ± 2.1 years) participating in the Collaborative Longitudinal Evaluation of Ethnicity and Refractive Error Study between 2000 and 2010. Uncorrected distance visual acuity was measured using a high-contrast projected logMAR chart. Cycloplegic refractive error was measured using the Grand Seiko WR-5100K autorefractor. The ability of logMAR acuity to detect various categories of refractive error was examined using receiver operating characteristic curves. RESULTS: Isoacuity curves show that increasing myopic spherical refractive errors, increasing astigmatic refractive errors, or a combination of both reduces distance visual acuity. Visual acuity was reduced by approximately 0.5 minutes of MAR per 0.30 to 0.40 D of spherical refractive error and by approximately 0.5 minutes of MAR per 0.60 to 0.90 D of astigmatism. Higher uncorrected hyperopic refractive error had little effect on distance visual acuity. Receiver operating characteristic curve analysis suggests that a logMAR distance acuity of 0.20 to 0.32 provides the best balance between sensitivity and specificity for detecting refractive errors other than hyperopia. Distance acuity alone was ineffective for detecting hyperopic refractive errors. CONCLUSIONS: Higher myopic and/or astigmatic refractive errors were associated with predictable reductions in uncorrected distance visual acuity. The reduction in acuity per diopter of cylindrical error was about half that for spherical myopic error. Although distance acuity may be a useful adjunct to the detection of myopic spherocylindrical refractive errors, accommodation presumably prevents acuity from assisting in the detection of hyperopia. Alternate procedures need to be used to detect hyperopia.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Distancia/fisiología , Errores de Refracción/fisiopatología , Agudeza Visual/fisiología , Acomodación Ocular , Adolescente , Astigmatismo/fisiopatología , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Hiperopía/fisiopatología , Masculino , Miopía/fisiopatología , Curva ROC , Sensibilidad y Especificidad , Pruebas de Visión
17.
Curr Biol ; 31(5): 1029-1036.e2, 2021 03 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33290742

RESUMEN

How do we estimate the position of an object in the world around us? Naturally, we would direct our gaze to that object. Accordingly, neural motor coordinates entail the distance of external objects and thus might be used to derive perceptual estimates. Several general frameworks in the history of perceptual science have offered such a view.1-4 However, a mechanism showing how motor and visual processes communicate remains elusive. Here, we report that every post-saccadic error biases visual localization in a serially dependent manner. In order to simulate a realignment of visual space through motor coordinates, we induced an artificial de-alignment between visual and motor space. We found that when performing saccades under this distortion, post-saccadic error information clearly realigned visual and motor space, again in a serially dependent manner. These results demonstrate that the consequences of every saccade directly influence where we see objects in the world. On a neural basis, this requires that motor signals, which generate close to the saccade production machinery, are reported to cortical areas and arrange visual space. This view is consistent with recent electrophysiological findings of post-saccadic error processing in posterior parietal cortex.5.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Distancia/fisiología , Modelos Neurológicos , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Lóbulo Parietal/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa
18.
Psychol Res ; 85(3): 1234-1247, 2021 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32036443

RESUMEN

Responses to object stimuli are often faster when jutting handles are aligned with responding hands, than when they are not: handle-to-hand correspondence effects. According to a location coding account, locations of visually salient jutting parts determine the spatial coding of objects. This asymmetry then facilitates same-sided responses compared to responses on the opposite side. Alternatively, this effect has been attributed to grasping actions of the left or the right hand afforded by the handle orientation and independent of its salience (affordance activation account). Our experiments were designed to disentangle the effects of pure salience from those of affordance activations. We selected pictures of tools with one salient and non-graspable side, and one graspable and non-salient side (non-jutting handle). Two experiments were run. Each experiment had two groups of participants: one group discriminated the location of the salient side of the object stimuli; the other group discriminated the location of the graspable side of them. In Experiment 1, responses were left and right button presses; in Experiment 2, they were left and right button presses plus reach-and-grasp actions. When visual salience was removed from graspable sides, no correspondence effect was observed between their orientation and the responding hands in both the experiments. Conversely, when salience depended on non-graspable portions, a correspondence effect was produced between their orientation and the responding hand. Overt attention to graspable sides did not potentiate any grasping affordance even when participants executed grasping responses in addition to button presses. Results support the location coding account: performance was influenced by the spatial coding of visually salient properties of objects.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Distancia/fisiología , Fuerza de la Mano/fisiología , Orientación Espacial/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Tiempo de Reacción/inmunología , Estudiantes , Universidades , Adulto Joven
19.
J Neurosci Res ; 99(2): 455-466, 2021 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33070400

RESUMEN

Time-to-contact (TTC) perception refers to the ability of an observer to estimate the remaining time before an object reaches a point in the environment, and is of crucial importance in daily life. Noninvasive correlational approaches have identified several brain areas sensitive to TTC information. Here we report the results of two studies, including one during an awake brain surgery, that aimed to identify the specific areas causally engaged in the TTC estimation process. In Study 1, we tested 40 patients with brain tumor in a TTC estimation task. The results showed that four of the six patients with impaired performance had tumors in right upper parietal cortex, although this tumoral location represented only six over 40 patients. In Study 2, 15 patients underwent awake brain surgery electrostimulation mapping to examine the implication of various brain areas in the TTC estimation process. We acquired and normalized to MNI space the coordinates of the functional areas that influenced task performance. Our results seem to demonstrate that the early stage of the TTC estimation process involved specific cortical territories in the ventral region of the right intraparietal sulcus. Downstream processing of TTC could also involve the frontal eye field (middle frontal gyrus) related to ocular search. We also found that deactivating language areas in the left hemisphere interfered with the TTC estimation process. These findings demonstrate a fine grained, cortical representation of TTC processing close to the ventral right intraparietal sulcus and complement those described in other human studies.


Asunto(s)
Mapeo Encefálico , Neoplasias Encefálicas/fisiopatología , Percepción de Distancia/fisiología , Lóbulo Frontal/fisiopatología , Glioma/fisiopatología , Lóbulo Parietal/fisiopatología , Percepción del Tiempo/fisiología , Adulto , Anciano , Neoplasias Encefálicas/psicología , Neoplasias Encefálicas/cirugía , Causalidad , Dominancia Cerebral , Estimulación Eléctrica , Femenino , Glioma/psicología , Glioma/cirugía , Humanos , Periodo Intraoperatorio , Lenguaje , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología
20.
Evol Psychol ; 18(4): 1474704920961953, 2020.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33161781

RESUMEN

The vertical-horizontal illusion is the overestimation of a vertical line compared to a horizontal line of the same length. Jackson and Cormack (2007) proposed that the vertical-horizontal illusion might be a byproduct of the mechanisms that generate the environmental vertical illusion, which is the tendency to overestimate vertical distances (i.e., heights) relative to horizontal distances the same length. In our study, 326 undergraduate participants stood atop an 18.6-meter parking structure and estimated both the height of the structure and the horizontal distance of a target placed 18.6 meters away, using a moveable horizontal target across the length of the structure. Participants also completed a vertical-horizontal illusion task by drawing a horizontal line below a 9.1 cm vertical line. We correlated vertical distance estimates with vertical line estimates to test Jackson and Cormack's byproduct hypothesis. This hypothesis was very weakly-if at all-supported by the data: Participants' overestimations in the vertical-horizontal illusion task explained 1% of the variance associated with their overestimations in the environmental vertical illusion task. Additionally, to test whether the environmental vertical illusion is impervious to explicit awareness, a random half of our participants were advised to be mindful that people tend to overestimate heights. The results supported our second hypothesis: Even when participants were made aware of the environmental vertical illusion, they still reliably overestimated heights. Discussion addressed implications for the robustness of the environmental vertical illusion (e.g., treatment of those with acrophobia).


Asunto(s)
Ilusiones/fisiología , Juicio/fisiología , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Concienciación/fisiología , Percepción de Distancia/fisiología , Ambiente , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Adulto Joven
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA