Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 41
Filtrar
Más filtros










Base de datos
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Am J Phys Anthropol ; 174(4): 744-751, 2021 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33393687

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVES: The Giles hypothesis posits that differences in the cranial morphology of Pan troglodytes and Gorilla gorilla are largely the result of allometric scaling. However, previous support for the Giles hypothesis was based on bivariate plots of linear measurements. This investigation uses geometric morphometric methods to retest this hypothesis and its prediction that extending the ontogenetic trajectory of a chimpanzee would produce an adult gorilla-like cranial morphology. MATERIALS AND METHODS: Forty-three 3D cranial landmarks were collected from an ontogenetic series of 76 Pan troglodytes and 58 Gorilla gorilla specimens. Ontogenetic trajectories of cranial shape change were computed via multivariate regression of Procrustes aligned coordinates against LnCS (size vector) and molar eruption stage (developmental vector). These two vectors were then used in developmental simulations to extend the ontogenetic trajectories of adult chimpanzees. Allometric trajectories of chimpanzees and gorillas were also directly compared using Procrustes ANOVA. RESULTS: Pan and Gorilla significantly differ in their allometric trajectories, and none of the Pan developmental simulations resembled actual adult gorillas. Additionally, the more the Pan developmental vector was extended, the more morphologically distinct the simulations became from actual adult gorillas. DISCUSSION: Taken together, these results do not support the Giles hypothesis that allometric scaling is primarily responsible for observed morphological differences between chimpanzee and gorilla crania. This investigation demonstrates that neither "growing" a chimpanzee to the size of a gorilla, nor extending a chimpanzee's developmental shape trajectory will result in an adult gorilla-like cranial morphology as they differ in their patterns of allometry.


Asunto(s)
Cefalometría , Gorilla gorilla/anatomía & histología , Pan troglodytes/anatomía & histología , Cráneo/anatomía & histología , Animales , Antropología Física , Evolución Biológica , Femenino , Gorilla gorilla/crecimiento & desarrollo , Masculino , Pan troglodytes/crecimiento & desarrollo , Cráneo/crecimiento & desarrollo
2.
J Comp Psychol ; 134(3): 293-302, 2020 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32162933

RESUMEN

The anomalous position of gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) in the capacity for self-recognition remains puzzling. The standard measure of self-recognition is Gallup's (1970) mark test that assesses an individual's ability to recognize its altered image in a mirror following the application of paint marks to visually inaccessible areas. Here, the results of a small-scale pilot study are presented, utilizing video playback through a TV monitor, to examine behavioral differences indicative of developing self-recognition. The behaviors of 4 Western lowland gorillas at Bristol Zoo, United Kingdom, were observed while watching a TV screen during 5 conditions: blank screen, white noise interference, footage of unfamiliar gorillas, self previously recorded, and self-live. Differences were predicted in the frequency of the gorillas' observed behaviors when viewing each of the conditions: specifically, that there would be more visual inspection, contingent body and facial movements, and self-exploration in the self-recorded and self-live conditions compared with the other conditions. These predictions were partially supported. No agonistic or fear responses were observed, and self-exploration was only seen in the self-live condition. During live playback, contingency-checking movements and self-exploration of the mouth were observed, particularly in the youngest gorilla, providing important video evidence of a close parallel to the mouth exploratory behavior witnessed in self-recognizing chimpanzees. On the basis of these preliminary findings of differentiated spontaneous behaviors, a tentative framework is proposed for categorizing gorillas according to levels of developing self-recognition along a continuum. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Conducta Animal , Reconocimiento Facial , Gorilla gorilla , Grabación en Video , Animales , Animales de Zoológico , Gorilla gorilla/crecimiento & desarrollo , Gorilla gorilla/psicología , Masculino , Proyectos Piloto , Conducta Social , Televisión
3.
Am J Phys Anthropol ; 163(3): 570-590, 2017 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28464240

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVES: Great apes show considerable diversity in socioecology and life history, but knowledge of their physical growth in natural settings is scarce. We characterized linear body size growth in wild mountain gorillas from Volcanoes National Park, Rwanda, a population distinguished by its extreme folivory and accelerated life histories. METHODS: In 131 individuals (0.09-35.26 years), we used non-invasive parallel laser photogrammetry to measure body length, back width, arm length and two head dimensions. Nonparametric LOESS regression was used to characterize cross-sectional distance and velocity growth curves for males and females, and consider links with key life history milestones. RESULTS: Sex differences became evident between 8.5 and 10.0 years of age. Thereafter, female growth velocities declined, while males showed increased growth velocities until 10.0-14.5 years across dimensions. Body dimensions varied in growth; females and males reached 98% of maximum body length at 11.7 and 13.1 years, respectively. Females attained 95.3% of maximum body length by mean age at first birth. Neonates were 31% of maternal size, and doubled in size by mean weaning age. Males reached maximum body and arm length and back width before emigration, but experienced continued growth in head dimensions. CONCLUSIONS: While comparable data are scarce, our findings provide preliminary support for the prediction that mountain gorillas reach maximum body size at earlier ages compared to more frugivorous western gorillas. Data from other wild populations are needed to better understand comparative great ape development, and investigate links between trajectories of physical, behavioral, and reproductive maturation.


Asunto(s)
Tamaño Corporal/fisiología , Gorilla gorilla/crecimiento & desarrollo , Gorilla gorilla/fisiología , Animales , Antropología Física , Femenino , Masculino , Parques Recreativos , Rwanda
4.
Am J Phys Anthropol ; 163(1): 129-147, 2017 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28251607

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVES: While dental development is important to life history investigations, data from wild known-aged great apes are scarce. We report on the first radiographic examination of dental development in wild Virunga mountain gorillas, using known-age skeletal samples recovered in Rwanda. MATERIALS AND METHODS: In 43 individuals (0.0-14.94 years), we collected radiographs of mandibular molars, and where possible, cone beam CT scans. Molar crown and root calcification status was assessed using two established staging systems, and age prediction equations generated using polynomial regression. Results were compared to available data from known-age captive and wild chimpanzees. RESULTS: Mountain gorillas generally fell within reported captive chimpanzee distributions or exceeded them, exhibiting older ages at equivalent radiographic stages of development. Differences reflect delayed initiation and/or an extended duration of second molar crown development, and extended first and second molar root development, in mountain gorillas compared to captive chimpanzees. However, differences in the duration of molar root development were less evident compared to wild chimpanzees. DISCUSSION: Despite sample limitations, our findings extend the known range of variation in radiographic estimates of molar formation timing in great apes, and provide a new age prediction technique based on wild specimens. However, mountain gorillas do not appear accelerated in radiographic assessment of molar formation compared to chimpanzees, as they are for other life history traits. Future studies should aim to resolve the influence of species differences, wild versus captive environments, and/or sampling phenomena on patterns observed here, and more generally, how they relate to variation in tooth size, eruption timing, and developmental life history.


Asunto(s)
Gorilla gorilla/crecimiento & desarrollo , Diente Molar/diagnóstico por imagen , Diente Molar/crecimiento & desarrollo , Animales , Antropología Física , Femenino , Masculino , Radiografía Dental , Rwanda
5.
Am J Primatol ; 77(6): 666-78, 2015 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25809396

RESUMEN

Uncovering sources of variation in gorilla infant mortality informs conservation and life history research efforts. The international studbook for the western lowland gorilla provides information on a sample of captive gorillas large enough for which to analyze genetic, maternal, and various other effects on early life mortality in this critically endangered species. We assess the importance of variables such as sex, maternal parity, paternal age, and hand rearing with regard to infant survival. We also quantify the proportions of variation in mortality influenced by heritable variation and maternal effects from these pedigree and survival data using variance component estimation. Markov chain Monte Carlo simulations of generalized linear mixed models produce variance component distributions in an animal model framework that employs all pedigree information. Two models, one with a maternal identity component and one with both additive genetic and maternal identity components, estimate variance components for different age classes during the first 2 years of life. This is informative of the extent to which mortality risk factors change over time during gorilla infancy. Our results indicate that gorilla mortality is moderately heritable with the strongest genetic influence just after birth. Maternal effects are most important during the first 6 months of life. Interestingly, hand-reared infants have lower mortality for the first 6 months of life. Aside from hand rearing, we found other predictors commonly used in studies of primate infant mortality to have little influence in these gorilla data.


Asunto(s)
Gorilla gorilla/crecimiento & desarrollo , Gorilla gorilla/genética , Mortalidad , Crianza de Animales Domésticos , Animales , Animales de Zoológico , Femenino , Humanos , Modelos Lineales , Masculino , Paridad , Edad Paterna , Embarazo
6.
J Hum Evol ; 81: 41-7, 2015 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25771994

RESUMEN

The Mojokerto calvaria has been central to assessment of brain growth in Homo erectus, but different analytical approaches and uncertainty in the specimen's age at death have hindered consensus on the nature of H. erectus brain growth. We simulate average annual rates (AR) of absolute endocranial volume (ECV) growth and proportional size change (PSC) in H. erectus, utilizing estimates of H. erectus neonatal ECV and a range of ages for Mojokerto. These values are compared with resampled ARs and PSCs from ontogenetic series of humans, chimpanzees, and gorillas from birth to six years. Results are consistent with other studies of ECV growth in extant taxa. There is extensive overlap in PSC between all living species through the first postnatal year, with continued but lesser overlap between humans and chimpanzees to age six. Human ARs are elevated above those of apes, although there is modest overlap up to 0.50 years. Ape ARs overlap throughout the sequence, with gorillas slightly elevated over chimpanzees up to 0.50 years. Simulated H. erectus PSCs can be found in all living species by 0.50 years, and the median falls below the human and chimpanzee ranges after 2.5 years. H. erectus ARs are elevated above those of all extant taxa prior to 0.50 years, and after two years they fall out of the human range but are still above ape ranges. A review of evidence for the age at death of Mojokerto supports an estimate of around one year, indicating absolute brain growth rates in the lower half of the human range. These results point to secondary altriciality in H. erectus, implying that key human adaptations for increasing the energy budget of females may have been established by at least 1 Ma.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/crecimiento & desarrollo , Fósiles/anatomía & histología , Hominidae/crecimiento & desarrollo , Cráneo/crecimiento & desarrollo , Animales , Evolución Biológica , Encéfalo/anatomía & histología , Gorilla gorilla/anatomía & histología , Gorilla gorilla/crecimiento & desarrollo , Hominidae/anatomía & histología , Humanos , Pan troglodytes/anatomía & histología , Pan troglodytes/crecimiento & desarrollo , Cráneo/anatomía & histología
7.
J Hum Evol ; 65(6): 693-703, 2013 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24129040

RESUMEN

Behavioral studies indicate that adult mountain gorillas (Gorilla beringei) are the most terrestrial of all nonhuman hominoids, but that infant mountain gorillas are much more arboreal. Here we examine ontogenetic changes in diaphyseal strength and length of the femur, tibia, humerus, radius, and ulna in 30 Virunga mountain gorillas, including 18 immature specimens and 12 adults. Comparisons are also made with 14 adult western lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla), which are known to be more arboreal than adult mountain gorillas. Infant mountain gorillas have significantly stronger forelimbs relative to hind limbs than older juveniles and adults, but are nonsignificantly different from western lowland gorilla adults. The change in inter-limb strength proportions is abrupt at about two years of age, corresponding to the documented transition to committed terrestrial quadrupedalism in mountain gorillas. The one exception is the ulna, which shows a gradual increase in strength relative to the radius and other long bones during development, possibly corresponding to the gradual adoption of stereotypical fully pronated knuckle-walking in older juvenile gorillas. Inter-limb bone length proportions show a contrasting developmental pattern, with hind limb/forelimb length declining rapidly from birth to five months of age, and then showing no consistent change through adulthood. The very early change in length proportions, prior to significant independent locomotion, may be related to the need for relatively long forelimbs for climbing in a large-bodied hominoid. Virunga mountain gorilla older juveniles and adults have equal or longer forelimb relative to hind limb bones than western lowland adults. These findings indicate that both ontogenetically and among closely related species of Gorilla, long bone strength proportions better reflect actual locomotor behavior than bone length proportions.


Asunto(s)
Huesos del Brazo/fisiología , Diáfisis/fisiología , Gorilla gorilla/fisiología , Huesos de la Pierna/fisiología , Actividad Motora , Animales , Huesos del Brazo/anatomía & histología , Huesos del Brazo/crecimiento & desarrollo , Evolución Biológica , Diáfisis/anatomía & histología , Diáfisis/crecimiento & desarrollo , Femenino , Gorilla gorilla/anatomía & histología , Gorilla gorilla/crecimiento & desarrollo , Huesos de la Pierna/anatomía & histología , Huesos de la Pierna/crecimiento & desarrollo , Masculino , Rwanda
8.
Folia Primatol (Basel) ; 84(3-5): 220-38, 2013.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23942111

RESUMEN

Few studies have directly compared human and African ape upper limb skeletal asymmetries despite the potential such comparisons have for understanding the origins of functional lateralization in humans and non-human primates. Here, we report the magnitude and direction of asymmetries in humeral torsion and humeral length in paired humeri of 40 Gorilla gorilla, 40 Pan troglodytes and 40 Homo sapiens. We test whether absolute and directional asymmetries differ between measurements, species and sexes. Our results show that humans are unique in being lateralized to the right for both measurements, consistent with human population-level handedness patterns, while apes show no significant directionality at the species level in either measurement. However, absolute torsion asymmetries in apes occur in the same magnitude as in humans, suggesting the existence of functional lateralization at the individual level.


Asunto(s)
Gorilla gorilla/anatomía & histología , Húmero/anatomía & histología , Pan troglodytes/anatomía & histología , Animales , Evolución Biológica , Femenino , Gorilla gorilla/crecimiento & desarrollo , Humanos , Húmero/crecimiento & desarrollo , Masculino , Pan troglodytes/crecimiento & desarrollo
9.
Am J Phys Anthropol ; 152(2): 165-72, 2013 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23907657

RESUMEN

Life history is influenced by factors both intrinsic (e.g., body and relative brain size) and extrinsic (e.g., diet, environmental instability) to organisms. In this study, we examine the prediction that energetic risk influences the life history of gorillas. Recent comparisons suggest that the more frugivorous western lowland gorilla shows increased infant dependence, and thus a slower life history, than the primarily folivorous mountain gorilla to buffer against the risk of starvation during periods of food unpredictability. We further tested this hypothesis by incorporating additional life history data from wild western lowland gorillas and captive western lowland gorillas with the assumption that the latter live under ecological conditions of energetic risk that more closely resemble those of mountain gorillas and thus should show faster life histories than wild members of the species. Overall, we found captive western lowland and wild mountain gorillas to have faster developmental life histories than wild western lowland gorillas, weaning their infants approximately a year earlier and thus reducing interbirth intervals by a year. These results provide support that energetic risk plays an important role in determining gorilla life history. Unlike previous assertions, gorillas do not have substantially faster life histories, at least at the genus level, than other great apes. This calls for a re-evaluation of theories concerning comparative ape life history and evolution and highlights the need for data from additional populations that vary in energetic risk.


Asunto(s)
Gorilla gorilla/crecimiento & desarrollo , Gorilla gorilla/fisiología , Animales , Antropología Física , Ecología , Femenino , Herbivoria , Masculino , Embarazo , Reproducción/fisiología , Destete
10.
Am J Primatol ; 75(5): 450-63, 2013 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23208801

RESUMEN

Understanding the life history correlates of ontogenetic differences in hominoid brain growth requires information from multiple species. At present, however, data on how brain size changes over the course of development are only available from chimpanzees and modern humans. In this study, we examined brain growth in wild Virunga mountain gorillas using data derived from necropsy reports (N = 34) and endocranial volume (EV) measurements (N = 86). The youngest individual in our sample was a 10-day-old neonatal male with a brain mass of 208 g, representing 42% of the adult male average. Our results demonstrate that Virunga mountain gorillas reach maximum adult-like brain mass by 3-4 years of age; adult-sized EV is reached by the time the first permanent molars emerge. This is in contrast to the pattern observed in chimpanzees, which despite their smaller absolute brain size, reportedly attain adult brain mass approximately 1 year later than Virunga mountain gorillas. Our findings demonstrate that brain growth is completed early in Virunga mountain gorillas compared to other great apes studied thus far, in a manner that appears to be linked with other life history characteristics of this population.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/fisiología , Animales Salvajes , Encéfalo/crecimiento & desarrollo , Gorilla gorilla/crecimiento & desarrollo , Animales , República Democrática del Congo , Femenino , Masculino , Rwanda , Uganda
11.
Anat Rec (Hoboken) ; 295(3): 438-53, 2012 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22190256

RESUMEN

To speculate how the extrinsic cardiac nerve plexus (ECNP) evolves phyletically and ontogenetically within the primate lineage, we conducted a comparative anatomical study of the ECNP, including an imaging examination in the great apes using 20 sides from 11 bodies from three species and a range of postnatal stages from newborns to mature adults. Although the position of the middle cervical ganglion (MG) in the great apes tended to be relatively lower than that in humans, the morphology of the ECNP in adult great apes was almost consistent with that in adult humans but essentially different from that in the lesser apes or gibbons. Therefore, the well-argued anatomical question of when did the MG acquire communicating branches with the spinal cervical nerves and appear constantly in all sympathetic cardiac nerves during primate evolution is clearly considered to be after the great apes and gibbons split. Moreover, a horizontal four-chambered heart and a lifted cardiac apex with a relatively large volume in newborn great apes rapidly changed its position downward, as seen in humans during postnatal growth and was associated with a reduction in the hepatic volume by imaging diagnosis and gross anatomy. In addition, our observation using a range of postnatal stages exhibits that two sympathetic ganglia, the middle cervical and cervicothoracic ganglia, differed between the early and later postnatal stages.


Asunto(s)
Vías Autónomas/anatomía & histología , Vías Autónomas/crecimiento & desarrollo , Corazón/crecimiento & desarrollo , Corazón/inervación , Hominidae/anatomía & histología , Animales , Femenino , Gorilla gorilla/anatomía & histología , Gorilla gorilla/crecimiento & desarrollo , Hominidae/crecimiento & desarrollo , Humanos , Masculino , Pan troglodytes/anatomía & histología , Pan troglodytes/crecimiento & desarrollo , Sistema Nervioso Parasimpático/anatomía & histología , Sistema Nervioso Parasimpático/crecimiento & desarrollo , Pongo/anatomía & histología , Pongo/crecimiento & desarrollo , Especificidad de la Especie , Sistema Nervioso Simpático/anatomía & histología , Sistema Nervioso Simpático/crecimiento & desarrollo
12.
BMC Evol Biol ; 11: 85, 2011 Apr 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21457536

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Today many large mammals live in small, fragmented populations, but it is often unclear whether this subdivision is the result of long-term or recent events. Demographic modeling using genetic data can estimate changes in long-term population sizes while temporal sampling provides a way to compare genetic variation present today with that sampled in the past. In order to better understand the dynamics associated with the divergences of great ape populations, these analytical approaches were applied to western gorillas (Gorilla gorilla) and in particular to the isolated and Critically Endangered Cross River gorilla subspecies (G. g. diehli). RESULTS: We used microsatellite genotypes from museum specimens and contemporary samples of Cross River gorillas to infer both the long-term and recent population history. We find that Cross River gorillas diverged from the ancestral western gorilla population ~17,800 years ago (95% HDI: 760, 63,245 years). However, gene flow ceased only ~420 years ago (95% HDI: 200, 16,256 years), followed by a bottleneck beginning ~320 years ago (95% HDI: 200, 2,825 years) that caused a 60-fold decrease in the effective population size of Cross River gorillas. Direct comparison of heterozygosity estimates from museum and contemporary samples suggests a loss of genetic variation over the last 100 years. CONCLUSIONS: The composite history of western gorillas could plausibly be explained by climatic oscillations inducing environmental changes in western equatorial Africa that would have allowed gorilla populations to expand over time but ultimately isolate the Cross River gorillas, which thereafter exhibited a dramatic population size reduction. The recent decrease in the Cross River population is accordingly most likely attributable to increasing anthropogenic pressure over the last several hundred years. Isolation of diverging populations with prolonged concomitant gene flow, but not secondary admixture, appears to be a typical characteristic of the population histories of African great apes, including gorillas, chimpanzees and bonobos.


Asunto(s)
Evolución Molecular , Gorilla gorilla/genética , África Occidental , Animales , Ecosistema , Flujo Génico , Variación Genética , Gorilla gorilla/clasificación , Gorilla gorilla/crecimiento & desarrollo , Repeticiones de Microsatélite , Densidad de Población , Dinámica Poblacional
13.
J Anat ; 218(5): 471-9, 2011 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21385182

RESUMEN

Research has shown that Pan and Homo have similar ectocranial suture synostosis patterns and a similar suture ontogeny (relative timing of suture fusion during the species ontogeny). This ontogeny includes patency during and after neurocranial expansion with a delayed bony response associated with adaptation to biomechanical forces generated by mastication. Here we investigate these relationships for Gorilla by examining the association among ectocranial suture morphology, cranial volume (as a proxy for neurocranial expansion) and dental development (as a proxy for the length of time that it has been masticating hard foods and exerting such strains on the cranial vault) in a large sample of Gorilla gorilla skulls. Two-hundred and fifty-five Gorilla gorilla skulls were examined for ectocranial suture closure status, cranial volume and dental eruption. Regression models were calculated for cranial volumes by suture activity, and Kendall's tau (a non-parametric measure of association) was calculated for dental eruption status by suture activity. Results suggest that, as reported for Pan and Homo, neurocranial expansion precedes suture synostosis activity. Here, Gorilla was shown to have a strong relationship between dental development and suture activity (synostosis). These data are suggestive of suture fusion extending further into ontogeny than brain expansion, similar to Homo and Pan. This finding allows for the possibility that masticatory forces influence ectocranial suture morphology.


Asunto(s)
Suturas Craneales/anatomía & histología , Gorilla gorilla/anatomía & histología , Cráneo/anatomía & histología , Erupción Dental , Animales , Suturas Craneales/crecimiento & desarrollo , Gorilla gorilla/crecimiento & desarrollo , Cráneo/crecimiento & desarrollo
14.
Am J Phys Anthropol ; 144(1): 162-5, 2011 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20979206

RESUMEN

A fused/closed basilar suture is usually treated as an indication of old age in great apes. A sample, drawn from a variety of sources, of known-aged captive great ape skeletons was analyzed to test the usefulness of using the basilar suture to categorize adult skeletons as either "adult" or "old adult". The state of closure of the basilar suture was examined in 30 chimpanzees, 19 gorillas, and 15 orangutans, all of known age. The results show that the basilar suture demonstrates a high level of uniformity in rate of closure and is closed at an early age in virtually all known-aged individuals. Thus, an old adult category most likely includes individuals who are, in fact, relatively young. This indicates that using the basilar suture as a means to classify individual skeletons as adult or old adult is very imprecise. The homogenous nature of basilar suture closure appears to prevent meaningful application of suture status for categorizing adult ape skeletons by age groups.


Asunto(s)
Determinación de la Edad por el Esqueleto/métodos , Gorilla gorilla/crecimiento & desarrollo , Pan troglodytes/crecimiento & desarrollo , Pongo/crecimiento & desarrollo , Animales , Desarrollo Óseo , Femenino , Gorilla gorilla/anatomía & histología , Masculino , Hueso Occipital/crecimiento & desarrollo , Pan troglodytes/anatomía & histología , Pongo/anatomía & histología , Hueso Esfenoides/crecimiento & desarrollo
15.
Am J Primatol ; 72(6): 492-501, 2010 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20082439

RESUMEN

Ontogenic development is divided into infant, juvenile, adolescent and adult life-stages. Although the developmental trajectory of an individual is a flexible entity, which differs within species, environment and sex, life-stage classifications are generally structured, age-based systems. This invariably leads to rigidity within a dynamic system and consequently hampers our understanding of primate life history strategies. We propose that life-stage classifications should be quantitative, flexible entities, which use a reliable measurement of development. Here, we provide a methodological example where placement into a life-stage is based upon behavioral variance between other similar-aged individuals. Behavioral data were collected from 12 male (3-11 years old) and 9 female (3-8 years old) captive immature western gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) housed in five family groups, using continuous focal sampling; 900 hr of data were collected over 131 days. Data were applied to four published life-stage classifications for mountain gorillas (Gorilla beringei beringei), which showed variable ability to determine life-stage in western gorillas. A new life-stage classification (Hutchinson & Fletcher) was proposed specifically for western gorillas, whereby multiple co-varying behavior provided a robust measure of linear development across immaturity. Each life-stage was found to be a distinct ontogenic phase and the classification discriminated life-stage with a high level of accuracy. Using the Hutchinson & Fletcher classification we provide evidence for disparity in developmental trajectories between the sexes from the juvenile period onwards. To expand the understanding of primate life histories, we propose that flexible classifications should be used to enable comparison of allometric life history traits within and between species, from birth onwards.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/psicología , Conducta Animal , Gorilla gorilla/crecimiento & desarrollo , Estadios del Ciclo de Vida , Distribución por Edad , Animales , Clasificación , Femenino , Gorilla gorilla/psicología , Masculino
16.
Am J Primatol ; 71(2): 106-19, 2009 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19003901

RESUMEN

Physical maturation and life-history parameters are seen as evolutionary adaptations to different ecological and social conditions. Comparison of life-history patterns of closely related species living in diverse environments helps to evaluate the validity of these assumptions but empirical data are lacking. The two gorilla species exhibit substantial differences in their environment, which allows investigation into the role of increased frugivory in shaping western gorilla life histories. We present behavioral and morphological data on western gorilla physical maturation and life-history parameters from a 12.5-year study at Mbeli Bai, a forest clearing in the Nouabalé-Ndoki National Park in northern Congo. We assign photographs of known individuals to different life-history classes and propose new age boundaries for life-history classes in western gorillas, which can be used and tested at other western gorilla research sites. Our results show that western gorillas are weaned at a later age compared with mountain gorillas and indicate slower physical maturation of immatures. These findings support the risk-aversion hypothesis for more frugivorous species. However, our methods need to be applied and tested with other gorilla populations. The slow life histories of western gorillas could have major consequences for social structure, mortality patterns and population growth rates that will affect recovery from population crashes of this critically endangered species. We emphasize that long-term studies can provide crucial demographic and life-history data that improve our understanding of life-history evolution and adaptation and help to refine conservation strategies.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Animal/fisiología , Dieta , Ambiente , Frutas , Gorilla gorilla/crecimiento & desarrollo , Factores de Edad , Animales , Congo , Gorilla gorilla/anatomía & histología , Estudios Longitudinales , Observación , Especificidad de la Especie
17.
Evolution ; 62(4): 943-58, 2008 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18194472

RESUMEN

Patterns of morphological integration and modularity among shape features emerge from genetic and developmental factors with varying pleiotropic effects. Factors or processes affecting morphology only locally may respond to selection more easily than common factors that may lead to deleterious side effects and hence are expected to be more conserved. We briefly review evidence for such global factors in primate cranial development as well as for local factors constrained to either the face or the neurocranium. In a sample comprising 157 crania of Homo sapiens, Pan troglodytes, and Gorilla gorilla, we statistically estimated common and local factors of shape variation from Procrustes coordinates of 347 landmarks and semilandmarks. Common factors with pleiotropic effects on both the face and the neurocranium account for a large amount of shape variation, but mainly by extension or truncation of otherwise conserved developmental pathways. Local factors (modular shape characteristics) have more degrees of freedom for evolutionary change than mere ontogenetic scaling. Cranial shape is similarly integrated during development in all three species, but human evolution involves dissociation among several characteristics. The dissociation has probably been achieved by evolutionary alterations and by the novel emergence of local factors affecting characteristics that are controlled at the same time by the common factors.


Asunto(s)
Evolución Biológica , Gorilla gorilla/anatomía & histología , Pan troglodytes/anatomía & histología , Cráneo/anatomía & histología , Animales , Femenino , Gorilla gorilla/crecimiento & desarrollo , Humanos , Masculino , Pan troglodytes/crecimiento & desarrollo , Cráneo/crecimiento & desarrollo
18.
Am J Primatol ; 70(5): 439-51, 2008 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18176937

RESUMEN

The conservation status of western lowland gorillas and central chimpanzees in western equatorial Africa remains largely speculative because many remote areas have never been surveyed and the impact of emergent diseases in the region has not been well documented. In this study, we compared ape densities and habitats in the Lokoué study area in Odzala National Park and the Goualougo Triangle in Nouabalé-Ndoki National Park in northern Republic of Congo. Both of these sites have long been considered strongholds for the conservation of chimpanzees and gorillas, but supposedly differ in vegetative composition and relative ape abundance. We compared habitats between these sites using conventional ground surveys and classified Landsat-7 ETM+ satellite images. We present density estimates via both standing-crop and marked-nest methods for the first time for sympatric apes of the Congo Basin. The marked-nest method was effective in depicting chimpanzee densities, but underestimated gorilla densities at both sites. Marked-nest surveys also revealed a dramatic decline in the ape population of Lokoué which coincided with a local Ebola epidemic. Normal baseline fluctuations in ape nest encounter rates during the repeated passages of marked-nest surveys were clearly distinguishable from a 80% decline in ape nest encounter rates at Lokoué. Our results showed that ape densities, habitat composition, and population dynamics differed between these populations in northern Congo. We emphasize the importance of intensifying monitoring efforts and further refinement of ape survey methods, as our results indicated that even the largest remaining ape populations in intact and protected forests are susceptible to sudden and dramatic declines.


Asunto(s)
Conservación de los Recursos Naturales , Ecosistema , Gorilla gorilla/crecimiento & desarrollo , Pan troglodytes/crecimiento & desarrollo , Animales , Congo , Densidad de Población
19.
Oecologia ; 155(1): 111-22, 2008 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17999090

RESUMEN

We tested the effects of age, sex, and season on the nutritional strategies of a group of mountain gorillas (Gorilla beringei) in the Bwindi Impenetrable National Park, Uganda. Through observations of food intake of individual gorillas and nutritional analyses of dietary components over different seasons and environments, we estimated nutrient intake and evaluated diet adequacy. Our results suggest that the nutritional costs of reproduction and growth affect nutrient intake; growing juveniles and adult females ate more food and more protein per kilogram of metabolic body mass than did silverbacks. The diets of silverback males, adult females, and juveniles contained similar concentrations of protein, fiber, and sugar, indicating that adult females and juveniles did not select higher protein foods than silverbacks but rather consumed more dry matter to ingest more protein. Juveniles consumed more minerals (Ca, P, Mg, K, Fe, Zn, Mn, Mo) per kilogram of body mass than adult females and silverback males, and juveniles consumed diets with higher concentrations of phosphorous, iron, and zinc, indicating that the foods they ate contained higher concentrations of these minerals. Seasonally, the amount of food consumed on a dry weight basis did not vary, but with increased frugivory, dietary concentrations of protein and fiber decreased and those of water-soluble carbohydrates increased. Energy intake did not change over the year. With the exception of sodium, gorillas ate diets that exceeded human nutrient requirements. A better understanding of the relative importance of food quantity and quality for different age-sex classes provides insights into the ways in which gorillas may be limited by food resources when faced with environmental heterogeneity.


Asunto(s)
Dieta , Ecosistema , Gorilla gorilla/fisiología , Estado Nutricional , Alimentación Animal , Animales , Ingestión de Energía , Femenino , Gorilla gorilla/crecimiento & desarrollo , Masculino , Plantas , Estaciones del Año , Uganda
20.
Primates ; 47(4): 294-9, 2006 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16596463

RESUMEN

The transfer of food items between individuals has been described in primates as serving an informative purpose in addition to supplementing the diet of immature individuals. This behaviour has yet to be described in western lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla), and results are presented here of observations of food transfers in immature gorillas at Mbeli Bai, Republic of Congo. The frequency of food transfers decreased with increasing immature age, while the frequency of independent feeding and processing of food increased. Transfers between mothers and infants were the most frequent, with infants attempting to take items from the mother. These attempts were not always successful and the item was relinquished on less than 50% of attempts. Mothers also took items from their offspring. The results point to the functional significance of food transfers in western lowland gorillas being informational. In a bai environment, where one species forms the majority of a visiting gorilla's diet despite other species being available, the initiation of food transfers by immatures is proposed to serve the purpose of familiarising them with which species, and which parts of those species, may be eaten.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Alimentaria , Gorilla gorilla/psicología , Factores de Edad , Animales , Femenino , Alimentos , Gorilla gorilla/crecimiento & desarrollo , Aprendizaje , Masculino
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA
...