Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
: 20 | 50 | 100
1 - 20 de 142
1.
Medicine (Baltimore) ; 100(30): e25442, 2021 Jul 30.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34397680

ABSTRACT: This study aimed to explore the efficacy of Baduanjin exercise on promoting memory function, executive function and general self-efficacy, decreasing the level of subjective memory complaints of older adults.In this randomized controlled trial, 80 patients were randomly allocated in a 1:1 ratio to 12-week Baduanjin exercise group or 12-week control group. Subjective memory complaint questionnaire, Auditory verbal learning test, Trail Making Test and General Self-Efficacy Scale was used to assess the subjective memory complaint level, memory function, executive function and general self-efficacy level at three times (baseline, after intervention and follow up at 3 months). One-way repeated measures analysis of variance was used to compare the outcome variables of the two groups.There were no significant differences between the Baduanjin exercise and the control group at baseline in demographic, SMCQ, MoCA, and GDS-15. Compared to participants in the control group, participants in the Baduanjin group had a significantly improvement in memory function (F = 46.93, P < .00), executive function (F = 317.83, P < .00) and general self-efficacy (F = 38.72, P < .00) at the end of 12-week intervention period and after 3months follow-up period (P < .01). At the same time, participants in the Baduanjin group had a significantly greater decrease in subjective memory complaint scores at the end of 12-week intervention period and after 3months follow-up period (F = 24.53, P < 0.00).Baduanjin exercise appears to be a feasible and acceptable intervention to improve subjective memory complaint among older adults.


Diagnostic Self Evaluation , Exercise Therapy/standards , Memory Disorders/psychology , Aged , Exercise Therapy/psychology , Exercise Therapy/statistics & numerical data , Female , Geriatrics/methods , Humans , Male , Memory Disorders/classification , Middle Aged , Quality of Life/psychology , Surveys and Questionnaires , Treatment Outcome
2.
Alzheimers Dement ; 17(4): 641-652, 2021 04.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33325121

INTRODUCTION: The clinical relevance of brain atrophy subtypes categorization in non-demented persons without a priori knowledge regarding their amyloid status or clinical presentation is unknown. METHODS: A total of 2083 outpatients with either subjective cognitive complaint or mild cognitive impairment at study entry were followed during 4 years (MEMENTO cohort). Atrophy subtypes were defined using baseline magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and previously described algorithms. RESULTS: Typical/diffuse atrophy was associated with faster cognitive decline and the highest risk of developing dementia and Alzheimer's disease (AD) over time, both in the whole analytic sample and in amyloid-positive participants. Hippocampal-sparing and limbic-predominant atrophy were also associated with incident dementia, with faster cognitive decline in the limbic predominant atrophy group. Lewy body dementia was more frequent in the hippocampal-sparing and minimal/no atrophy groups. DISCUSSION: Atrophy subtypes categorization predicted different subsequent patterns of cognitive decline and rates of conversion to distinct etiologies of dementia in persons attending memory clinics.


Alzheimer Disease , Ambulatory Care Facilities , Atrophy/pathology , Brain/pathology , Memory Disorders , Aged , Alzheimer Disease/classification , Alzheimer Disease/pathology , Cohort Studies , Female , Hippocampus/pathology , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Memory Disorders/classification
3.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol ; 41(8): 775-785, 2019 10.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31156042

Prognostic modeling in moderate to severe traumatic brain injury (TBI) has historically focused primarily on the projection of crude outcomes such as the risk of mortality and disability. Initial work in this area has perpetuated the notion that prognosis after moderate to severe TBI can be measured as a single, static, and dichotomous outcome. However, more recent conceptualizations describe moderate to severe TBI as the initiation of a chronic disease state with high levels of inter-individual variability in terms of symptom manifestation and disease progression. Unfortunately, existing prognostic models provide limited insight into the extent of chronic cognitive and neurodegenerative changes experienced by moderate to severe TBI survivors. Though prior research has identified a variety of acute factors that appear to influence post-injury cognitive and neuropathological outcomes, an empirically supported framework for prognostic modeling of these injury-distal outcomes does not exist. The current review considers the literature on an expanded array of empirically supported predictors (both premorbid and injury-related) in association with long-term sequelae of moderate to severe TBI. We also provide a theoretical framework and statistical approach for prognostic modeling in moderate to severe TBI in order to unify efforts across research groups and facilitate important progress in this research area.


Brain Injuries, Traumatic/diagnosis , Brain Injuries, Traumatic/pathology , Brain Injury, Chronic/diagnosis , Brain Injury, Chronic/pathology , Cognition Disorders/diagnosis , Cognition Disorders/pathology , Brain/pathology , Brain Injuries, Traumatic/classification , Brain Injury, Chronic/classification , Cognition Disorders/classification , Disability Evaluation , Educational Status , Executive Function , Female , Glasgow Outcome Scale , Humans , Learning Disabilities/classification , Learning Disabilities/diagnosis , Learning Disabilities/pathology , Male , Memory Disorders/classification , Memory Disorders/diagnosis , Memory Disorders/pathology , Neurodegenerative Diseases/classification , Neurodegenerative Diseases/diagnosis , Neurodegenerative Diseases/pathology , Neuropsychological Tests , Organ Size/physiology , Prognosis , Risk Factors
4.
Mil Med ; 184(Suppl 1): 206-217, 2019 03 01.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30901472

Subconcussive head injuries are connected to both short-term cognitive changes and long-term neurodegeneration. Further study is required to understand what types of subconcussive impacts might prove detrimental to cognition. We studied cadets at the US Air Force Academy engaged in boxing and physical development, measuring head impact motions during exercise with accelerometers. These head impact measures were compared with post-exercise memory performance. Investigators explored multiple techniques for characterizing the magnitude of head impacts. Boxers received more head impacts and achieved lower performance in post-exercise memory than non-boxers. For several measures of impact motion, impact intensity appeared to set an upper bound on post-exercise memory performance - stronger impacts led to lower expected memory performance. This trend was most significant when impact intensity was measured through a novel technique, applying principal component analysis to boxer motion. Principal component analysis measures also captured more distinct impact information than seven traditional impact measures also tested.


Boxing/injuries , Brain Concussion/complications , Memory Disorders/etiology , Accelerometry/methods , Adolescent , Brain Concussion/physiopathology , Educational Measurement/methods , Educational Measurement/statistics & numerical data , Humans , Male , Memory Disorders/classification , Memory and Learning Tests , Neuropsychological Tests/statistics & numerical data , Young Adult
5.
Appl Neuropsychol Adult ; 26(1): 17-27, 2019.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28850256

HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders (HAND) commonly feature verbal episodic memory impairment historically characterized by a retrieval deficit, consistent with a classic "subcortical" presentation; however, there are hints of a subtle shift toward a more "cortical" memory profile characterized by a primary encoding deficit. The current study evaluated this possibility by comparing the pattern of HAND-associated verbal episodic memory deficits to those of traditional "subcortical" (i.e., Huntington's disease; HD) versus "cortical" (i.e., left temporal lobe epilepsy with mesial temporal sclerosis; L-MTLE) profiles. Seventy-seven individuals with HAND, 47 individuals with HD, 21 individuals with L-MTLE, and 45 healthy participants were administered the California Verbal Learning Test - 2nd Edition (CVLT-II). CVLT-II profiles were classified as reflecting a primary encoding deficit, retrieval deficit, or a normal profile. Among participants with a deficit profile, the HAND group showed the highest rates of retrieval versus encoding profiles (71% vs. 29%), followed by HD (59% vs. 41%), L-MTLE (46% vs. 54%), and healthy (50% vs. 50%) groups. While significant profile heterogeneity was observed across clinical groups, findings suggest that HIV-associated verbal episodic memory impairments are most consistent with a traditional "subcortical," retrieval deficit profile, consistent with the primary frontostriatal neuropathogenesis of HIV disease.


Epilepsy, Temporal Lobe/complications , HIV Infections/complications , Huntington Disease/complications , Memory Disorders , Memory, Episodic , Mental Recall/physiology , Neurocognitive Disorders , Verbal Learning/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Memory Disorders/classification , Memory Disorders/etiology , Memory Disorders/physiopathology , Memory and Learning Tests/statistics & numerical data , Middle Aged , Neurocognitive Disorders/classification , Neurocognitive Disorders/etiology , Neurocognitive Disorders/physiopathology
6.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 72(4): 798-817, 2019 Apr.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29554833

Current theory has divided memory into multiple systems, resulting in a fractionated account of human behaviour. By an alternative perspective, memory is a single system. However, debate over the details of different single-system theories has overshadowed the converging agreement among them, slowing the reunification of memory. Evidence in favour of dividing memory often takes the form of dissociations observed in amnesia, where amnesic patients are impaired on some memory tasks but not others. The dissociations are taken as evidence for separate explicit and implicit memory systems. We argue against this perspective. We simulate two key dissociations between classification and recognition in a computational model of memory, A Theory of Nonanalytic Association. We assume that amnesia reflects a quantitative difference in the quality of encoding. We also present empirical evidence that replicates the dissociations in healthy participants, simulating amnesic behaviour by reducing study time. In both analyses, we successfully reproduce the dissociations. We integrate our computational and empirical successes with the success of alternative models and manipulations and argue that our demonstrations, taken in concert with similar demonstrations with similar models, provide converging evidence for a more general set of single-system analyses that support the conclusion that a wide variety of memory phenomena can be explained by a unified and coherent set of principles.


Computer Simulation , Memory Disorders/physiopathology , Models, Psychological , Recognition, Psychology , Female , Humans , Male , Memory Disorders/classification , Neuropsychological Tests
7.
Exp Brain Res ; 236(7): 2037-2046, 2018 Jul.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29744565

Confabulating patients produce statements and actions that are unintentionally incongruous to their history, background, present and future situation. Here we present the very unusual case of a patient with right hemisphere damage and signs of left visual neglect, who, when presented with visual stimuli, confabulated both for consciously undetected and for consciously detected left-sided details. Advanced anatomical investigation suggested a disconnection between the parietal and the temporal lobes in the right hemisphere. A disconnection between the ventral cortical visual stream and the dorsal fronto-parietal networks in the right hemisphere may contribute to confabulatory behaviour by restricting processing of left-sided stimuli to pre-conscious stages in the ventral visual stream.


Functional Laterality , Memory Disorders/etiology , Neural Pathways/pathology , Perceptual Disorders , Space Perception/physiology , Aged , Attention , Brain Mapping , Case-Control Studies , Diffusion Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Humans , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted , Male , Memory Disorders/classification , Memory Disorders/diagnostic imaging , Neural Pathways/diagnostic imaging , Neuropsychological Tests , Perceptual Disorders/complications , Perceptual Disorders/diagnostic imaging , Perceptual Disorders/etiology , Photic Stimulation , Stroke/complications
8.
Praxis (Bern 1994) ; 107(8): 435-451, 2018 04.
Article De | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29642795

The early diagnosis of subjectively perceived or externally anamnestically observed cognitive impairments is essential for proving neurodegenerative diseases or excluding treatable causes such as internal, neurological or psychiatric disorders. Only in this way is early treatment made possible. As part of the project 3.1 of the National Dementia Strategy 2014­2019 («Development and expansion of regional and networked centres of competence for diagnostics¼), the association Swiss Memory Clinics (SMC) set itself the goal of developing quality standards for dementia clarification and improving the community-based care in this field. In these recommendations, general guidelines of diagnostics and individual examination possibilities are presented, and standards for the related processes are suggested. Individual areas such as anamnesis, clinical examination, laboratory examination, neuropsychological testing and neuroradiological procedures are discussed in detail as part of standard diagnostics, and supplementary examination methods for differential diagnosis considerations are portrayed. The most important goals of the SMC recommendations for the diagnosis of dementia are to give all those affected access to high-quality diagnostics, if possible, to improve early diagnosis of dementia and to offer the basic service providers and the employees of Memory Clinics a useful instrument for the clarification.


Cognitive Dysfunction/diagnosis , Dementia/diagnosis , Hospitals, Special , Memory Disorders/diagnosis , Neurodegenerative Diseases/diagnosis , Aged , Algorithms , Cognitive Dysfunction/classification , Cognitive Dysfunction/psychology , Cognitive Dysfunction/therapy , Community Networks/standards , Dementia/classification , Dementia/psychology , Dementia/therapy , Diagnosis, Differential , Early Diagnosis , General Practice , Hospitals, Special/standards , Humans , Interdisciplinary Communication , Intersectoral Collaboration , Memory Disorders/classification , Memory Disorders/psychology , Memory Disorders/therapy , Middle Aged , Neurodegenerative Diseases/classification , Neurodegenerative Diseases/psychology , Neurodegenerative Diseases/therapy , Quality Assurance, Health Care/standards , Switzerland
9.
Alzheimers Dement ; 14(6): 734-742, 2018 06.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29352855

INTRODUCTION: We compared risk of progression from subjective cognitive decline (SCD) to mild cognitive impairment (MCI) in an academic memory clinic versus a population-based study. METHODS: Older adults presenting at a memory clinic were classified as SCD (n = 113) or as noncomplainers (n = 82). Participants from a population study were classified as SCD (n = 592) and noncomplainers (n = 589) based on a memory complaint score. Annual follow-up performed for a mean of 3 years. RESULTS: The adjusted hazard ratio for SCD was 15.97 (95% confidence interval: 6.08-42.02, P < .001) in the memory clinic versus 1.18 (95% confidence interval: 1.00-1.40, P = .047) in the population study, where reported "worry" about memory further increased SCD-associated risk for MCI. DISCUSSION: SCD is more likely to progress to MCI in a memory clinic than the general population; participants' characteristics vary across settings. Study setting should be considered when evaluating SCD as a risk state for MCI and dementia.


Cognitive Dysfunction/classification , Disease Progression , Memory Disorders/classification , Aged , Ambulatory Care Facilities , Cognitive Dysfunction/epidemiology , Female , Humans , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Risk Factors , United States/epidemiology
10.
Brain Pathol ; 28(2): 143-154, 2018 03.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28380661

Neuropathological subtypes of hippocampal sclerosis (HS) in temporal lobe epilepsy (The 2013 International League Against Epilepsy classification) are based on the qualitative assessment of patterns of neuronal loss with NeuN. In practice, some cases appear indeterminate between type 1 (CA1 and CA4 loss) and type 2 HS (CA1 loss) and we predicted that MAP2 would enable a more stringent classification. HS subtypes, as well as the accompanying alteration of axonal networks, regenerative capacity and neurodegeneration have been previously correlated with outcome and memory deficits and may provide prognostic clinical information. We selected 92 cases: 52 type 1 HS, 15 type 2 HS, 18 indeterminate-HS and 7 no-HS. Quantitative analysis was carried out on NeuN and MAP2 stained sections and a labeling index (LI) calculated for six hippocampal subfields. We also evaluated hippocampal regenerative activity (MCM2, nestin, olig2, calbindin), degeneration (AT8/phosphorylated tau) and mossy-fiber pathway re-organization (ZnT3). Pathology measures were correlated with clinical epilepsy history, memory and naming test scores and postoperative outcomes, at 1 year following surgery. MAP2 LI in indeterminate-HS was statistically similar to type 2 HS but this clustering was not shown with NeuN. Moderate verbal and visual memory deficits were noted in all HS types, including 54% and 69% of type 2 HS. Memory deficits correlated with several pathology factors including lower NeuN or MAP2 LI in CA4, CA1, dentate gyrus (DG) and subiculum and poor preservation of the mossy fiber pathway. Decline in memory at 1 year associated with AT8 labeling in the subiculum and DG but not HS type. We conclude that MAP2 is a helpful addition in the classification of HS in some cases. Classification of HS subtype, however, did not significantly correlate with outcome or pre- or postoperative memory dysfunction, which was associated with multiple pathology factors including hippocampal axonal pathways, regenerative capacity and degenerative changes.


Epilepsy, Temporal Lobe/pathology , Hippocampus/pathology , Memory Disorders/pathology , Sclerosis/classification , Sclerosis/pathology , Adolescent , Adult , Antigens, Nuclear/metabolism , Disease Progression , Epilepsy, Temporal Lobe/classification , Epilepsy, Temporal Lobe/physiopathology , Epilepsy, Temporal Lobe/psychology , Female , Hippocampus/physiopathology , Hippocampus/surgery , Humans , Immunohistochemistry , Male , Memory Disorders/classification , Memory Disorders/etiology , Memory Disorders/physiopathology , Microtubule-Associated Proteins/metabolism , Middle Aged , Nerve Tissue Proteins/metabolism , Neuronal Plasticity , Neurons/pathology , Neurons/physiology , Neurosurgical Procedures , Sclerosis/physiopathology , Young Adult
11.
Rev Neurol (Paris) ; 173(7-8): 498-505, 2017.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28843413

Temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE) is a type of epilepsy that often has a negative impact on patients' memory. Despite the importance of patients' complaints in this regard, the difficulties described by these patients are often not easy to demonstrate through a standard neuropsychological assessment. Accelerated long-term forgetting and autobiographical memory disorders are the two main memory impairments reported in the literature in patients with TLE. However, the methods used by different authors to evaluate long-term memory and autobiographical memory are heterogeneous. This heterogeneity can lead to differences in the observed results as well as how they are interpreted. Yet, despite the methodological differences, objectification of such memory deficits appears to be both specific and robust within this patient population. Analysis of the literature shows that accelerated long-term forgetting and autobiographical memory disorders share the same clinical characteristics. This leads to the assumption that they are, in fact, only one entity and that their evaluation may be done through a single procedure. Our proposal is to place this evaluation within the context of memory consolidation disorders. With such a perspective, evaluation of accelerated forgetting in autobiographical memory should consist of identifying a disorder in the formation and/or recovery of new memory traces.


Epilepsy, Temporal Lobe/complications , Epilepsy, Temporal Lobe/psychology , Memory Disorders/classification , Memory Disorders/etiology , Memory, Episodic , Memory, Long-Term/physiology , Humans , Memory Disorders/diagnosis , Neuropsychological Tests
12.
Clin Gerontol ; 40(4): 249-257, 2017.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28459304

OBJECTIVES: To pilot two new cognitive screening tools for use in an urban Malaysian population and to compare their criterion validity against a gold standard, the well-established Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE). METHODS: The IDEA cognitive screen, Picture-based Memory Impairment Scale (PMIS), and MMSE were administered to a convenience sample of elderly (≥ 65 years) from the community and outpatient clinics at an urban teaching hospital. Consensus diagnosis was performed by two geriatricians blinded to PMIS and IDEA cognitive screen scores using the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-V) clinical criteria. The MMSE performance was used as a reference. RESULTS: The study enrolled 66 participants, with a median age of 78.5 years (interquartile range [IQR], 72.5-83.0) years and 11.0 median years of education (IQR, 9.0-13.0). Forty-three (65.2%) were female, and 32 (48.4%) were Chinese. The area under the receiver operating characteristic (AUROC) curve values were .962 (IDEA cognitive screen), .970 (PMIS), and .935 (MMSE). The optimal cutoff values for sensitivity and specificity were: IDEA cognitive screen: ≤ 11, 90.9% and 89.7%; PMIS: ≤ 6, 97.3% and 69.0%; and MMSE: ≤ 23, 84.6% and 76.0%. Although the sample size was small, multivariable logistic regression modelling suggested that all three screen scores did not appear to be educationally biased. CONCLUSION: The IDEA and PMIS tools are potentially valid screening tools for dementia in urban Malaysia, and perform at least as well as the MMSE. Further work on larger representative, cohorts is needed to further assess the psychometric properties. CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS: Study provides alternative screening tools for dementia for both non-specialists and specialists.


Cognitive Dysfunction/diagnosis , Dementia/diagnosis , Mass Screening/instrumentation , Memory Disorders/diagnosis , Mental Status and Dementia Tests/standards , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Cognition/physiology , Cognitive Dysfunction/ethnology , Dementia/ethnology , Female , Humans , Malaysia/epidemiology , Male , Mass Screening/methods , Memory Disorders/classification , Psychometrics/instrumentation , Psychometrics/methods , Sensitivity and Specificity
13.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc ; 23(5): 421-430, 2017 05.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28481196

OBJECTIVES: In complex real life situations, memories for temporal and spatial information are naturally linked since sequential events coincide in time and space. Whether this connection is inseparable or instead whether these processes are functionally dissociable was investigated in this patient study. METHODS: Spatial object-location and temporal order memory tasks were administered to 36 stroke patients and 44 healthy control participants. RESULTS: On group level, patients with a stroke in the left hemisphere performed worse on temporal order memory, compared to the control participants. On individual level, using a multiple case-study approach, a clear pattern of dissociations was found between memory for temporal and for spatial features. CONCLUSIONS: These findings indicate that location and temporal order memory contain functionally separable processes. This adds to our understanding of how context information is processed in human memory. (JINS, 2017, 23, 421-430).


Memory Disorders/diagnosis , Mental Recall/physiology , Neuropsychological Tests , Spatial Memory/physiology , Adult , Aged , Female , Functional Laterality/physiology , Humans , Male , Memory Disorders/classification , Memory Disorders/etiology , Middle Aged , Stroke/complications
14.
J Alzheimers Dis ; 58(2): 373-387, 2017.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28436388

BACKGROUND: The early diagnosis of dementia is of great clinical and social importance. A recent study using the qualitative methodology of conversation analysis (CA) demonstrated that language and communication problems are evident during interactions between patients and neurologists, and that interactional observations can be used to differentiate between cognitive difficulties due to neurodegenerative disorders (ND) or functional memory disorders (FMD). OBJECTIVE: This study explores whether the differential diagnostic analysis of doctor-patient interactions in a memory clinic can be automated. METHODS: Verbatim transcripts of conversations between neurologists and patients initially presenting with memory problems to a specialist clinic were produced manually (15 with FMD, and 15 with ND). A range of automatically detectable features focusing on acoustic, lexical, semantic, and visual information contained in the transcripts were defined aiming to replicate the diagnostic qualitative observations. The features were used to train a set of five machine learning classifiers to distinguish between ND and FMD. RESULTS: The mean rate of correct classification between ND and FMD was 93% ranging from 97% by the Perceptron classifier to 90% by the Random Forest classifier.Using only the ten best features, the mean correct classification score increased to 95%. CONCLUSION: This pilot study provides proof-of-principle that a machine learning approach to analyzing transcripts of interactions between neurologists and patients describing memory problems can distinguish people with neurodegenerative dementia from people with FMD.


Communication , Memory Disorders/diagnosis , Neurodegenerative Diseases/diagnosis , Physician-Patient Relations , Aged , Automation , Diagnosis, Differential , Female , Humans , Machine Learning , Male , Memory Disorders/classification , Memory Disorders/psychology , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Retrospective Studies
15.
Arch Clin Neuropsychol ; 32(4): 391-400, 2017 Jun 01.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28334378

OBJECTIVE: Increased false memory recognition in patients with Huntington's disease (HD) has been widely reported; however, the underlying memory constructive processes remain unclear. The present study explored gist memory, item-specific memory, and monitoring ability in patients with HD. METHOD: Twenty-five patients (including 13 patients with mild HD and 12 patients with moderate-to-severe HD) and 30 healthy comparison participants (HC) were recruited. We used the Deese-Roediger-McDermott (DRM) paradigm to investigate participants' false recognition patterns, along with neuropsychological tests to assess general cognitive function. RESULTS: Both mild and moderate-to-severe patients with HD showed significant executive functioning and episodic memory impairment. On the DRM tasks, both HD patient groups showed significantly impaired performance in tasks assessing unrelated false recognition and item-specific memory as compared to the HC group; moderate-to-severe patients performed more poorly than mild patients did. Only moderate-severe patients exhibited significantly poorer related false recognition index scores than HCs in the verbal DRM task; performance of HD patient groups was comparable to the HC group on the pictorial DRM task. CONCLUSIONS: It appears that diminished verbatim memory and monitoring ability are early signs of cognitive decline during the HD course. Conversely, gist memory is relatively robust, with only partial decline during advanced-stage HD. Our findings suggest that medial temporal lobe function is relatively preserved compared to that of frontal-related structures in early HD. Thus, gist-based memory rehabilitation programs might be beneficial for patients with HD.


Huntington Disease/complications , Memory Disorders/etiology , Repression, Psychology , Adult , Cognition/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Memory Disorders/classification , Memory Disorders/diagnosis , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Photic Stimulation , Severity of Illness Index , Statistics, Nonparametric
16.
Int J Lang Commun Disord ; 52(2): 214-226, 2017 03.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27349587

BACKGROUND: Word-retrieval difficulties are commonly experienced by people with aphasia (PwA) and also by typically ageing persons. Differentiation between true naming impairments and naming difficulties found in healthy persons may, therefore, be challenging. AIMS: To investigate the extent to which the Maltese adaptation of the Boston Naming Test (BNT) can identify people with lexical retrieval difficulties and to differentiate them from people with unimpaired word finding. METHODS & PROCEDURES: Naming performance of a group of PwA was compared with the performance of a control group. Performance on the Maltese adaptation of the BNT was investigated in terms of scores, range of scores and error profiles of the two groups. OUTCOMES & RESULTS: All PwA scored below the mean score of the controls, indicating that persons who scored above the mean score may be considered as unimpaired. However, a number of the controls obtained very low scores that overlapped with the scores obtained by the PwA. This indicated that scores alone cannot be used to differentiate between impaired and unimpaired people. Some types of errors were only produced by people with impaired naming, and did not appear at all in error profiles of unimpaired individuals. CONCLUSIONS & IMPLICATIONS: Mild-moderate anomic impairments may be missed if naming impairment is assessed and diagnosed using a cut-off score. In order to differentiate between people with impaired and unimpaired naming, it is necessary to look at error profiles, apart from the number of errors, as the presence of atypical errors may be an important indicator of naming impairments.


Anomia/diagnosis , Neuropsychological Tests/statistics & numerical data , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Anomia/classification , Anomia/psychology , Diagnosis, Differential , Female , Humans , Male , Malta , Memory Disorders/classification , Memory Disorders/diagnosis , Memory Disorders/psychology , Middle Aged , Psychometrics/statistics & numerical data , Reference Values , Young Adult
17.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol ; 38(2): 171-82, 2016.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26588602

Confabulation is often observed in amnesic patients after brain damage. However, evidence regarding the relationship between confabulation and other neuropsychological functions is scarce. In addition, previous studies have proposed two possibilities of the relationship between confabulation and false memory, in which patients with confabulation are likely to retrieve false memories, or confabulations are relatively independent of false memories. The present study investigated how confabulation is related to various cognitive functions, including orientation, attention, frontal lobe function, memory, and mental status, and to false memories, as assessed by the Deese-Roediger-Mcdermott (DRM) paradigm. Patients with organic amnesia participated, and confabulations were evaluated using the Confabulation Battery. Amnestic patients were classified into two groups: confabulating (CP) and nonconfabulating patients (NCP). The CP group was significantly impaired in time orientation, attention, and verbal memory, compared to the NCP group and age-matched healthy controls (HC). Results of the DRM paradigm revealed no significant difference in false memory retrieval induced by critical lures across CP, NCP, and HC groups. Confabulating responses in organic amnesia could be in part induced by disturbance of time consciousness and attention control in severe impairment of verbal memories, and confabulation and false memory could be modulated by different cognitive systems.


Amnesia/physiopathology , Attention/physiology , Memory Disorders/physiopathology , Mental Recall/physiology , Orientation/physiology , Amnesia/classification , Amnesia/complications , Female , Humans , Male , Memory Disorders/classification , Memory Disorders/complications , Middle Aged , Repression, Psychology , Verbal Learning/physiology
18.
Continuum (Minneap Minn) ; 21(3 Behavioral Neurology and Neuropsychiatry): 613-26, 2015 Jun.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26039844

PURPOSE OF REVIEW: This article highlights the dissociable human memory systems of episodic, semantic, and procedural memory in the context of neurologic illnesses known to adversely affect specific neuroanatomic structures relevant to each memory system. RECENT FINDINGS: Advances in functional neuroimaging and refinement of neuropsychological and bedside assessment tools continue to support a model of multiple memory systems that are distinct yet complementary and to support the potential for one system to be engaged as a compensatory strategy when a counterpart system fails. SUMMARY: Episodic memory, the ability to recall personal episodes, is the subtype of memory most often perceived as dysfunctional by patients and informants. Medial temporal lobe structures, especially the hippocampal formation and associated cortical and subcortical structures, are most often associated with episodic memory loss. Episodic memory dysfunction may present acutely, as in concussion; transiently, as in transient global amnesia (TGA); subacutely, as in thiamine deficiency; or chronically, as in Alzheimer disease. Semantic memory refers to acquired knowledge about the world. Anterior and inferior temporal lobe structures are most often associated with semantic memory loss. The semantic variant of primary progressive aphasia (svPPA) is the paradigmatic disorder resulting in predominant semantic memory dysfunction. Working memory, associated with frontal lobe function, is the active maintenance of information in the mind that can be potentially manipulated to complete goal-directed tasks. Procedural memory, the ability to learn skills that become automatic, involves the basal ganglia, cerebellum, and supplementary motor cortex. Parkinson disease and related disorders result in procedural memory deficits. Most memory concerns warrant bedside cognitive or neuropsychological evaluation and neuroimaging to assess for specific neuropathologies and guide treatment.


Brain/pathology , Memory Disorders/diagnosis , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Female , Humans , Male , Memory Disorders/classification , Memory Disorders/etiology , Memory, Episodic , Memory, Short-Term , Middle Aged , Nervous System Diseases/complications , Neuroimaging , Neuropsychological Tests
19.
J Bras Pneumol ; 41(2): 182-90, 2015.
Article En, Pt | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25909154

The objectives of this study were to characterize and clarify the relationships between the various cognitive domains affected in COPD patients and the disease itself, as well as to determine the prevalence of impairment in the various cognitive domains in such patients. To that end, we performed a systematic review using the following databases: PubMed, Scopus, and ScienceDirect. We included articles that provided information on cognitive impairment in COPD patients. The review of the findings of the articles showed a significant relationship between COPD and cognitive impairment. The most widely studied cognitive domains are memory and attention. Verbal memory and learning constitute the second most commonly impaired cognitive domain in patients with COPD. The prevalence of impairment in visuospatial memory and intermediate visual memory is 26.9% and 19.2%, respectively. We found that cognitive impairment is associated with the profile of COPD severity and its comorbidities. The articles reviewed demonstrated that there is considerable impairment of the cognitive domains memory and attention in patients with COPD. Future studies should address impairments in different cognitive domains according to the disease stage in patients with COPD.


Cognition Disorders/etiology , Memory Disorders/etiology , Pulmonary Disease, Chronic Obstructive/complications , Cognition Disorders/classification , Cross-Sectional Studies , Female , Humans , Learning Disabilities/etiology , Male , Memory Disorders/classification , Neuropsychological Tests , Observational Studies as Topic
20.
J. bras. pneumol ; 41(2): 182-190, Mar-Apr/2015. tab, graf
Article En | LILACS | ID: lil-745925

The objectives of this study were to characterize and clarify the relationships between the various cognitive domains affected in COPD patients and the disease itself, as well as to determine the prevalence of impairment in the various cognitive domains in such patients. To that end, we performed a systematic review using the following databases: PubMed, Scopus, and ScienceDirect. We included articles that provided information on cognitive impairment in COPD patients. The review of the findings of the articles showed a significant relationship between COPD and cognitive impairment. The most widely studied cognitive domains are memory and attention. Verbal memory and learning constitute the second most commonly impaired cognitive domain in patients with COPD. The prevalence of impairment in visuospatial memory and intermediate visual memory is 26.9% and 19.2%, respectively. We found that cognitive impairment is associated with the profile of COPD severity and its comorbidities. The articles reviewed demonstrated that there is considerable impairment of the cognitive domains memory and attention in patients with COPD. Future studies should address impairments in different cognitive domains according to the disease stage in patients with COPD.


Os objetivos deste estudo foram caracterizar e esclarecer as relações entre os vários domínios cognitivos afetados em pacientes com DPOC e a doença em si, assim como determinar a prevalência de comprometimentos cognitivos em tais pacientes. Para tanto, foi realizada uma revisão sistemática utilizando as seguintes bases de dados: PubMed, Scopus e ScienceDirect. Os artigos incluídos forneciam informações sobre os comprometimentos cognitivos em pacientes com DPOC. A revisão dos achados de tais artigos mostrou uma relação significativa entre DPOC e comprometimento cognitivo. Os domínios cognitivos mais estudados são a memória e a atenção. Memória verbal e aprendizagem constituem o segundo domínio cognitivo mais comumente prejudicado em pacientes com DPOC. A prevalência de comprometimento da memória visuoespacial e da memória visual intermediária é 26,9% e 19.2%, respectivamente. Observamos que o comprometimento cognitivo está associado ao perfil de gravidade da DPOC e suas comorbidades. A revisão dos artigos demonstrou que há um comprometimento considerável dos domínios memória e atenção em pacientes com DPOC. Investigações futuras devem abordar os comprometimentos em diferentes domínios cognitivos em conformidade com o estágio da doença em pacientes com DPOC.


Female , Humans , Male , Cognition Disorders/etiology , Memory Disorders/etiology , Pulmonary Disease, Chronic Obstructive/complications , Cross-Sectional Studies , Cognition Disorders/classification , Learning Disabilities/etiology , Memory Disorders/classification , Neuropsychological Tests , Observational Studies as Topic
...