Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
: 20 | 50 | 100
1 - 20 de 7.446
1.
Vestn Oftalmol ; 140(2. Vyp. 2): 116-122, 2024.
Article Ru | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38739140

PURPOSE: This study assesses the light sensitivity and its variability in each point of the visual field in patients without glaucoma and with different stages of glaucoma. MATERIAL AND METHODS: The data of a prospective analytical case-control study involving 500 patients were analyzed. The initial examination of all patients was performed using basic ophthalmological methods, including static perimetry. Retinal light sensitivity and its variability were assessed in 54 points corresponding to the Humphrey 24-2 program. Mean deviation and pattern standard deviation of light sensitivity were calculated for each point. RESULTS: The lowest light sensitivity values in patients with moderate glaucoma were found in the periphery of the nasal sector, at point No. 27 - 14.4 dB, and at points No. 24-26 along the horizontal axis from the nasal side - from 17.7 to 22.7 dB. The maximum variability of light sensitivity was found in the nasal sector on both sides of the horizontal line - from 10.7 to 11.5 dB. The average light sensitivity above the horizontal axis in patients with advanced glaucoma was 10.8 dB, which is 2 dB higher than in the lower half of the visual field - 8.8 dB. The highest light sensitivity values were found at points No. 24 - 17.7 dB and No. 31 - 16.78 dB, the lowest - at point No. 32 - 4.5 dB. The average variability values of light sensitivity in the upper half of the visual field were 9.6 dB, which is 1 dB less than in the lower half of the visual field - 10.6 dB. CONCLUSION: According to our data, points No. 32 and No. 40 are of particular interest in the diagnostic plan. In these loci, the highest light sensitivity values were determined in early and moderate glaucoma. However, the values in these points decrease significantly in advanced glaucoma. It can be assumed that changes in light sensitivity in these loci at the early stages of glaucoma may be a predictor of glaucoma progression.


Glaucoma , Retina , Visual Field Tests , Visual Fields , Humans , Visual Fields/physiology , Visual Field Tests/methods , Glaucoma/physiopathology , Glaucoma/diagnosis , Middle Aged , Male , Female , Retina/physiopathology , Prospective Studies , Adult , Light , Aged , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Case-Control Studies , Reproducibility of Results
2.
J Vis ; 24(5): 8, 2024 May 01.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38780934

Perceptual learning is a multifaceted process, encompassing general learning, between-session forgetting or consolidation, and within-session fast relearning and deterioration. The learning curve constructed from threshold estimates in blocks or sessions, based on tens or hundreds of trials, may obscure component processes; high temporal resolution is necessary. We developed two nonparametric inference procedures: a Bayesian inference procedure (BIP) to estimate the posterior distribution of contrast threshold in each learning block for each learner independently and a hierarchical Bayesian model (HBM) that computes the joint posterior distribution of contrast threshold across all learning blocks at the population, subject, and test levels via the covariance of contrast thresholds across blocks. We applied the procedures to the data from two studies that investigated the interaction between feedback and training accuracy in Gabor orientation identification over 1920 trials across six sessions and estimated learning curve with block sizes L = 10, 20, 40, 80, 160, and 320 trials. The HBM generated significantly better fits to the data, smaller standard deviations, and more precise estimates, compared to the BIP across all block sizes. In addition, the HBM generated unbiased estimates, whereas the BIP only generated unbiased estimates with large block sizes but exhibited increased bias with small block sizes. With L = 10, 20, and 40, we were able to consistently identify general learning, between-session forgetting, and rapid relearning and adaptation within sessions. The nonparametric HBM provides a general framework for fine-grained assessment of the learning curve and enables identification of component processes in perceptual learning.


Bayes Theorem , Learning , Sensory Thresholds , Humans , Learning/physiology , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Learning Curve , Photic Stimulation/methods
3.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci ; 65(5): 33, 2024 May 01.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38771569

Purpose: This study explored early (contrast discrimination) and intermediate (global form perception) visual processing in primary subtypes of glaucoma: primary open-angle glaucoma (POAG) and primary angle-closure glaucoma (PACG). We aimed to understand early and intermediate visual processing in POAG and PACG, matched for similar visual field defect severity. Methods: Early visual processing was measured using a contrast discrimination task described by Porkorny and Smith (1997), and intermediate processing using a global form perception task using glass pattern coherence thresholds. Thresholds were determined centrally and at a single midperipheral location (12.5°) in a quadrant without visual field defects. Controls were tested in corresponding quadrants to individuals with glaucoma. Results: Sixty participants (20 POAG, 20 PACG, and 20 age-matched controls), aged 50 to 77 years, were included. Visual field defects were matched between POAG and PACG, with mean deviation values of -6.53 ± 4.46 (range: -1.5 to -16.85) dB and -6.2 ± 4.24 (range: -1.37 to -16.42) dB, respectively. Two-Way ANOVA revealed significant differences in thresholds between the glaucoma groups and the control group for both contrast discrimination and global form perception tasks, with higher thresholds in the glaucoma groups. Post hoc analyses showed no significant contrast discrimination difference between POAG and PACG, but POAG had significantly higher thresholds than PACG for form perception. Conclusions: In form perception, POAG showed slightly worse performance than PACG, suggesting that individuals with POAG may experience more severe functional damage than PACG of similar visual field severity.


Contrast Sensitivity , Form Perception , Glaucoma, Angle-Closure , Glaucoma, Open-Angle , Visual Fields , Humans , Glaucoma, Open-Angle/physiopathology , Glaucoma, Angle-Closure/physiopathology , Middle Aged , Aged , Male , Female , Visual Fields/physiology , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Form Perception/physiology , Intraocular Pressure/physiology , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Visual Field Tests
4.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci ; 65(5): 38, 2024 May 01.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38787547

Purpose: Visual snow is the hallmark of the neurological condition visual snow syndrome (VSS) but the characteristics of the visual snow percept remain poorly defined. This study aimed to quantify its appearance, interobserver variability, and effect on measured visual performance and self-reported visual quality. Methods: Twenty-three participants with VSS estimated their visual snow dot size, separation, luminance, and flicker rate by matching to a simulation. To assess whether visual snow masks vision, we compared pattern discrimination thresholds for textures that were similar in spatial scale to visual snow as well as more coarse than visual snow, in participants with VSS, and with and without external noise simulating visual snow in 23 controls. Results: Mean and 95% confidence intervals for visual snow appearance were: size (6.0, 5.8-6.3 arcseconds), separation (2.0, 1.7-2.3 arcmin), luminance (72.4, 58.1-86.8 cd/m2), and flicker rate (25.8, 18.9-32.8 frames per image at 120 hertz [Hz]). Participants with finer dot spacing estimates also reported greater visibility of their visual snow (τb = -0.41, 95% confidence interval [CI] = -0.62 to -0.13, P = 0.01). In controls, adding simulated fine-scale visual snow to textures increased thresholds for fine but not coarse textures (F(1, 22) = 4.98, P = 0.036, ηp2 = 0.19). In VSS, thresholds for fine and coarse textures were similar (t(22) = 0.54, P = 0.60), suggesting that inherent visual snow does not act like external noise in controls. Conclusions: Our quantitative estimates of visual snow constrain its likely neural origins, may aid differential diagnosis, and inform future investigations of how it affects vision. Methods to quantify visual snow are needed for evaluation of potential treatments.


Visual Acuity , Humans , Male , Female , Adult , Middle Aged , Visual Acuity/physiology , Young Adult , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Vision Disorders/physiopathology , Vision Disorders/diagnosis , Aged , Visual Perception/physiology , Observer Variation , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Perceptual Disorders
5.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38692474

Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) is commonly delivered at an intensity defined by the resting motor threshold (rMT), which is thought to represent cortical excitability, even if the TMS target area falls outside of the motor cortex. This approach rests on the assumption that cortical excitability, as measured through the motor cortex, represents a 'global' measure of excitability. Another common approach to measure cortical excitability relies on the phosphene threshold (PT), measured through the visual cortex of the brain. However, it remains unclear whether either estimate can serve as a singular measure to infer cortical excitability across different brain regions. If PT and rMT can indeed be used to infer cortical excitability across brain regions, they should be correlated. To test this, we systematically identified previous studies that measured PT and rMT to calculate an overall correlation between the two estimates. Our results, based on 16 effect sizes from eight studies, indicated that PT and rMT are correlated (ρ = 0.4), and thus one measure could potentially serve as a measure to infer cortical excitability across brain regions. Three exploratory meta-analyses revealed that the strength of the correlation is affected by different methodologies, and that PT intensities are higher than rMT. Evidence for a PT-rMT correlation remained robust across all analyses. Further research is necessary for an in-depth understanding of how cortical excitability is reflected through TMS.


Motor Cortex , Phosphenes , Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation , Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation/methods , Humans , Phosphenes/physiology , Motor Cortex/physiology , Evoked Potentials, Motor/physiology , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Cortical Excitability/physiology
6.
Vision Res ; 219: 108394, 2024 06.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38579407

Contour Integration (CI) is the ability to integrate elemental features into objects and is a basic visual process essential for object perception and recognition, and for functioning in visual environments. It is now well documented that people with schizophrenia (SZ), in addition to having cognitive impairments, also have several visual perceptual deficits, including in CI. Here, we retrospectively characterize the performance of both SZ and neurotypical individuals (NT) on a series of contour shapes, made up of Gabor elements, that varied in terms of closure and curvature. Participants in both groups performed a CI training task that included 7 different families of shapes (Lines, Ellipse, Blobs, Squiggles, Spiral, Circle and Letters) for up to 40 sessions. Two parameters were manipulated in the training task: Orientation Jitter (OJ, i.e., orientation deviations of individual Gabor elements from ideal for each shape) and Inducer Number (IN, i.e., number of Gabor elements defining the shape). Results show that both OJ and IN thresholds significantly differed between the groups, with higher (OJ) and lower (IN) thresholds observed in the controls. Furthermore, we found significant effects as a function of the contour shapes, with differences between groups emerging with contours that were considered more complex, e.g., due to having a higher degree of curvature (Blobs, Spiral, Letters). These data can inform future work that aims to characterize visual integration impairments in schizophrenia.


Form Perception , Schizophrenia , Humans , Form Perception/physiology , Schizophrenia/physiopathology , Adult , Female , Male , Middle Aged , Retrospective Studies , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Photic Stimulation/methods , Case-Control Studies , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Young Adult
7.
eNeuro ; 11(4)2024 Apr.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38575352

The cerebellum has the reputation of being a primitive part of the brain that mostly is involved in motor coordination and motor control. Older lesion studies and more recent electrophysiological studies have, however, indicated that it is involved in temporal perception and temporal expectation building. An outstanding question is whether this temporal expectation building cerebellar activity has functional relevance. In this study, we collected magnetoencephalographic data from 30 healthy participants performing a detection task on at-threshold stimulation that was presented at the end of a sequence of temporally regular or irregular above-threshold stimulation. We found that behavioral detection rates depended on the degree of irregularity in the sequence preceding it. We also found cerebellar responses evoked by above-threshold and at-threshold stimulation. The evoked responses to at-threshold stimulation differed significantly, depending on whether it was preceded by a regular or an irregular sequence. Finally, we found that detection performance across participants correlated significantly with the differences in cerebellar evoked responses to the at-threshold stimulation, demonstrating the functional relevance of cerebellar activity in sensory expectation building. We furthermore found evidence of thalamic involvement, as indicated by responses in the beta band (14-30 Hz) and by significant modulations of cerebello-thalamic connectivity by the regularity of the sequence and the kind of stimulation terminating the sequence. These results provide evidence that the temporal expectation building mechanism of the cerebellum, what we and others have called an internal clock, shows functional relevance by regulating behavior and performance in sensory action that requires acting and integrating evidence over precise timescales.


Cerebellum , Magnetoencephalography , Time Perception , Humans , Male , Cerebellum/physiology , Female , Adult , Young Adult , Time Perception/physiology , Thalamus/physiology , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Neural Pathways/physiology
8.
Vision Res ; 220: 108413, 2024 07.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38613969

Visual performance across the visual fields interacts with visual tasks and visual stimuli, and visual resolution decreases as a function of eccentricity, varying at isoeccentric locations. In this study, we investigated the extent of asymmetry and the rate of change in visual acuity threshold for visual word form (VWF) identification at horizontal and vertical azimuths across the fovea, and at eccentricities of 1°, 2°, 4°, 6° and 8° for 10%, 20%, 40%, and 80% contrast levels, to determine whether and how the eccentricities, meridians, and contrasts modulated the VWF identification acuity threshold. The stimuli were 16 traditional Chinese characters of similar legibility. Participants pressed a key to indicate the character presented, either monocularly or binocularly, at one of 21 randomly selected locations. A staircase procedure was used to determine the threshold, and a multiple linear regression model was used to fit the linear cortical magnification factor (CMF). We found that (1) the asymmetry was most pronounced on the vertical and superior azimuths, (2) the asymmetry between the right and left azimuths was not significant, (3) the CMF was significantly smaller on the vertical azimuth than on the horizontal azimuth, (4) the CMF was smaller on the superior vertical azimuth than on the inferior azimuth, and (5) monocular viewing and low contrast enhanced the CMF difference between azimuths. In conclusion, vertical and horizontal azimuths, location of eccentricity, contrast levels of word symbols, and monocular/binocular viewing have different effects on visual field asymmetry and cortical magnification factors.


Visual Acuity , Visual Fields , Humans , Visual Fields/physiology , Female , Male , Young Adult , Adult , Visual Acuity/physiology , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Photic Stimulation/methods , Reading , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology
9.
Vision Res ; 220: 108406, 2024 07.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38626536

Incorporating statistical characteristics of stimuli in perceptual processing can be highly beneficial for reliable estimation from noisy sensory measurements but may generate perceptual bias. According to Bayesian inference, perceptual biases arise from the integration of internal priors with noisy sensory inputs. In this study, we used a Bayesian observer model to derive biases and priors in hue perception based on discrimination data for hue ensembles with varying levels of chromatic noise. Our results showed that discrimination thresholds for isoluminant stimuli with hue defined by azimuth angle in cone-opponent color space exhibited a bimodal pattern, with lowest thresholds near a non-cardinal blue-yellow axis that aligns closely with the variation of natural daylights. Perceptual biases showed zero crossings around this axis, indicating repulsion away from yellow and attraction towards blue. These biases could be explained by the Bayesian observer model through a non-uniform prior with a preference for blue. Our findings suggest that visual processing takes advantage of knowledge of the distribution of colors in natural environments for hue perception.


Bayes Theorem , Color Perception , Sensory Thresholds , Humans , Color Perception/physiology , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Photic Stimulation/methods , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Adult , Female , Male , Young Adult
10.
Vision Res ; 219: 108396, 2024 06.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38640684

Recent studies suggest that binocular adding S+ and differencing S- channels play an important role in binocular vision. To test for such a role in the context of binocular contrast detection and binocular summation, we employed a surround masking paradigm consisting of a central target disk surrounded by a mask annulus. All stimuli were horizontally oriented 0.5c/d sinusoidal gratings. Correlated stimuli were identical in interocular spatial phase while anticorrelated stimuli were opposite in interocular spatial phase. There were four target conditions: monocular left eye, monocular right eye, binocular correlated and binocular anticorrelated, and three surround mask conditions: no surround, binocularly correlated and binocularly anticorrelated. We observed consistent elevation of detection thresholds for monocular and binocular targets across the two binocular surround mask conditions. In addition, we found an interaction between the type of surround and the type of binocular target: both detection and summation were relatively enhanced by surround masks and targets with opposite interocular phase relationships and reduced by surround masks and targets with the same interocular phase relationships. The data were reasonably well accounted for by a model of binocular combination termed MAX (S+S-), in which the decision variable is the probability summation of modeled S+ and S- channel responses, with a free parameter determining the relative gains of the two channels. Our results support the existence of two channels involved in binocular combination, S+ and S-, whose relative gains are adjustable by surround context.


Contrast Sensitivity , Perceptual Masking , Photic Stimulation , Sensory Thresholds , Vision, Binocular , Humans , Vision, Binocular/physiology , Perceptual Masking/physiology , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Photic Stimulation/methods , Psychophysics , Vision, Monocular/physiology , Adult
11.
J Neurophysiol ; 131(6): 1143-1155, 2024 Jun 01.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38658179

Although perceptual thresholds have been widely studied, vestibuloocular reflex (VOR) thresholds have received less attention, so the relationship between VOR and perceptual thresholds remains unclear. We compared the frequency dependence of human VOR thresholds to human perceptual thresholds for yaw head rotation in both upright ("yaw rotation") and supine ("yaw tilt") positions, using the same human subjects and motion device. VOR thresholds were generally a little smaller than perceptual thresholds. We also found that horizontal VOR thresholds for both yaw rotation about an Earth-vertical axis and yaw tilt (yaw rotation about an Earth-horizontal axis) were relatively constant across four frequencies (0.2, 0.5, 1, and 2 Hz), with little difference between yaw rotation and yaw tilt VOR thresholds. For yaw tilt stimuli, perceptual thresholds were slightly lower at the lowest frequency and nearly constant at all other (higher) frequencies. However, for yaw rotation, perceptual thresholds increased significantly at the lowest frequency (0.2 Hz). We conclude 1) that VOR thresholds were relatively constant across frequency for both yaw rotation and yaw tilt, 2) that the known contributions of velocity storage to the VOR likely yielded these VOR thresholds that were similar for yaw rotation and yaw tilt for all frequencies tested, and 3) that the integration of otolith and horizontal canal signals during yaw tilt when supine contributes to stable perceptual thresholds, especially relative to the low-frequency perceptual thresholds recorded during yaw rotation.NEW & NOTEWORTHY We describe for the first time that human VOR thresholds differ from human forced-choice perceptual thresholds, with the difference especially evident at frequencies below 0.5 Hz. We also report that VOR thresholds are relatively constant across frequency for both yaw rotation and yaw tilt. These findings are consistent with the idea that high-pass filtering in cortical pathways impacts cognitive decision-making.


Reflex, Vestibulo-Ocular , Sensory Thresholds , Humans , Reflex, Vestibulo-Ocular/physiology , Male , Female , Adult , Rotation , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Head Movements/physiology , Young Adult
12.
Neuropsychologia ; 198: 108879, 2024 Jun 06.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38570111

After stroke, patients can experience visual hypersensitivity, an increase in their sensitivity for visual stimuli as compared to their state prior to the stroke. Candidate behavioural mechanisms for these subjective symptoms are atypical bottom-up sensory processing and impaired selective attention, but empirical evidence is currently lacking. In the current study, we aimed to investigate the relationship between post-stroke visual hypersensitivity and sensory thresholds, sensory processing speed, and selective attention using computational modelling of behavioural data. During a whole/partial report task, participants (51 stroke patients, 76 orthopedic patients, and 77 neurotypical adults) had to correctly identify a single target letter that was presented alone (for 17-100 ms) or along a distractor (for 83ms). Performance on this task was used to estimate the sensory threshold, sensory processing speed, and selective attention abilities of each participant. In the stroke population, both on a group and individual level, there was evidence for impaired selective attention and -to a lesser extent- lower sensory thresholds in patients with post-stroke visual hypersensitivity as compared to neurotypical adults, orthopedic patients, or stroke patients without post-stroke sensory hypersensitivity. These results provide a significant advancement in our comprehension of post-stroke visual hypersensitivity and can serve as a catalyst for further investigations into the underlying mechanisms of sensory hypersensitivity after other types of acquired brain injury as well as post-injury hypersensitivity for other sensory modalities.


Attention , Stroke , Humans , Male , Female , Stroke/complications , Stroke/physiopathology , Middle Aged , Attention/physiology , Aged , Adult , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Photic Stimulation , Visual Perception/physiology
13.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 86(4): 1417-1434, 2024 May.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38658516

Vestibular perceptual thresholds quantify sensory noise associated with reliable perception of small self-motions. Previous studies have identified substantial variation between even healthy individuals' thresholds. However, it remains unclear if or how an individual's vestibular threshold varies over repeated measures across various time scales (repeated measurements on the same day, across days, weeks, or months). Here, we assessed yaw rotation and roll tilt thresholds in four individuals and compared this intra-individual variability to inter-individual variability of thresholds measured across a large age-matched cohort each measured only once. For analysis, we performed simulations of threshold measurements where there was no underlying variability (or it was manipulated) to compare to that observed empirically. We found remarkable consistency in vestibular thresholds within individuals, for both yaw rotation and roll tilt; this contrasts with substantial inter-individual differences. Thus, we conclude that vestibular perceptual thresholds are an innate characteristic, which validates pooling measures across sessions and potentially serves as a stable clinical diagnostic and/or biomarker.


Sensory Thresholds , Vestibule, Labyrinth , Humans , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Male , Female , Adult , Vestibule, Labyrinth/physiology , Motion Perception/physiology , Rotation , Individuality , Young Adult , Middle Aged
14.
Acta Otolaryngol ; 144(2): 142-146, 2024 Feb.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38469861

BACKGROUND: The effect of wearing masks on olfaction remains unclear. OBJECTIVES: This study aimed to clarify the differences between the effects of no masks, surgical masks, and N95 respirator masks by conducting both identification and threshold olfaction tests. METHODS: Young, healthy volunteers aged ≥ 18 years and < 30 years without awareness of apparent olfactory disorder were included. All participants filled out a questionnaire on olfaction and completed an acuity smell identification test (Open Essence test) and an olfactory threshold test (T&T olfactometry) while wearing no masks, surgical masks, or N95 respirator masks. RESULTS: In the Open Essence tests, the no-mask group score was significantly higher than those of the surgical- and N95-mask groups. Using T&T olfactometry, the median-detection threshold of the no-mask group was significantly lower than that of the surgical-mask group, and the surgical-mask group threshold was significantly lower than that of the N95-mask group. Similar patterns were observed for the median-recognition threshold. CONCLUSIONS: Wearing masks, especially an N95 mask, reduces the ability to detect and identify odors. This disadvantage should be considered by professionals such as healthcare workers, who require proper olfaction to perform appropriate tasks.


Masks , Odorants , Smell , Humans , Masks/adverse effects , Male , Adult , Female , Young Adult , Smell/physiology , N95 Respirators/adverse effects , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Adolescent , Healthy Volunteers
15.
Nutrients ; 16(6)2024 Mar 13.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38542732

The sense of smell plays an important role in influencing the eating habits of individuals and consequently, their body weight, and its impairment has been associated with modified eating behaviors and malnutrition problems. The inter-individual variability of olfactory function depends on several factors, including genetic and physiological ones. In this study, we evaluated the role of the Kv1.3 channel genotype and age, as well as their mutual relationships, on the olfactory function and BMI of individuals divided into young, adult and elderly groups. We assessed olfactory performance in 112 healthy individuals (young n = 39, adult n = 36, elderly n = 37) based on their TDI olfactory score obtained through the Sniffin' Sticks test and their BMI. Participants were genotyped for the rs2821557 polymorphism of the human gene encoding Kv1.3 channels, the minor C allele of which was associated with a decreased sense of smell and higher BMIs compared to the major T allele. The results show that TT homozygous subjects obtained higher TDI olfactory scores and showed lower BMIs than CC homozygous subjects, in all age groups considered. Furthermore, the positive effect of the T allele on olfactory function and BMI decreased with increasing age. The contribution of the genetic factor is less evident with advancing age, while the importance of the age factor is compensated for by genetics. These results show that genetic and physiological factors such as age act to balance each other.


Olfaction Disorders , Adult , Humans , Aged , Odorants , Body Mass Index , Smell/genetics , Polymorphism, Genetic , Sensory Thresholds/physiology
16.
Mult Scler Relat Disord ; 85: 105545, 2024 May.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38479044

BACKGROUND: Olfactory threshold (OT) is a marker of short-term inflammatory activity in multiple sclerosis (MS). OBJECTIVE: To investigate whether OT predicts long-term MS clinical disease course. METHODS: This was a 6-year prospective longitudinal study on MS patients at the MS clinic Innsbruck. Clinical visits assessing the occurrence of relapses, Expanded Disability Status Scale (EDSS) scores, and disease-modifying treatment (DMT), were conducted biannually. OT testing was performed at baseline (BL), year 1 (Y1), year 2 (Y2) and year 6 (Y6), using the threshold subscore of the "Sniffin' Sticks" test. Cognitive function was assessed by the Symbol Digit Modalities Test. RESULTS: Of 139 MS patients, 92 were eligible for Y6 follow-up. 68% experienced relapses, 53% EDSS worsening, 29% progression independent of relapse activity (PIRA) and 41% cognitive deterioration. OT scores were lower at BL, Y1 and Y2 in patients requiring DMT escalation. In multivariable analysis, higher OT scores at BL, Y1, Y2 and Y6 were associated with lower risk of relapse (hazard ratio, HR: 0.65-0.92) and EDSS worsening (HR: 0.86-0.89), while no associations were found for PIRA and cognitive deterioration. CONCLUSIONS: OT is a potential surrogate marker for long-term inflammatory disease activity and DMT failure in MS.


Disease Progression , Recurrence , Humans , Female , Male , Adult , Longitudinal Studies , Middle Aged , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Prospective Studies , Biomarkers , Cognitive Dysfunction/etiology , Cognitive Dysfunction/physiopathology , Multiple Sclerosis, Relapsing-Remitting/physiopathology , Multiple Sclerosis/physiopathology , Multiple Sclerosis/complications , Disability Evaluation , Smell/physiology
17.
J Neurol ; 271(6): 3227-3238, 2024 Jun.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38441610

Persistent postural-perceptual dizziness (PPPD) is a chronic disorder of perceived unsteadiness. Symptoms can be exacerbated in visually complex stationary or moving environment. Visual dependence and increased motion sensitivity are predictors for PPPD but its pathophysiology remains unknown. We hypothesized an abnormal sensory-perceptual scaling mechanism in PPPD and tested visual- and vestibular perceptional thresholds in 32 patients and 28 age-matched healthy control subjects (HC). All participants showed normal vestibular function tests on quantitative testing. Visual motion coherence thresholds were assessed by random dot kinetomatograms. Vestibular perceptional thresholds of egomotion were assessed by binaural galvanic vestibular stimulation (GVS) and passive chair rotation around an earth-vertical axis. Chair rotation trials were contrasted with no-motion (sham) stimulus trials. Mean thresholds of visual motion perception were higher in patients compared to HC. The perception threshold of GVS was lower in patients but the threshold of correctly perceived egomotion during chair rotation did not differ. Interestingly, the number of trials with correct perception in the no-motion condition increased with the threshold of correct responses for rotatory egomotion in patients. Unlike expected, PPPD patients required more coherently moving random dots than HC to perceive visual motion. A poorer complex visual motion recognition, e.g., traffic visual stimuli, may increase anxiety and levels of uncertainty as visuomotor reactions might occur delayed. The vestibular rotatory perception threshold predicted the probability of making false assignments in the sham condition in PPPD, i.e., patients who readily recognize the correct egomotion direction are prone to perceive egomotion in the no-motion condition. As this relation was not found in healthy subjects, it may reflect an abnormal sensory-perceptual scaling feature of PPPD.


Dizziness , Motion Perception , Humans , Motion Perception/physiology , Male , Female , Dizziness/physiopathology , Middle Aged , Adult , Aged , Vestibule, Labyrinth/physiopathology , Vestibule, Labyrinth/physiology , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Postural Balance/physiology , Perceptual Disorders/physiopathology , Perceptual Disorders/etiology , Rotation , Photic Stimulation/methods
18.
J Vis Exp ; (204)2024 02 23.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38465936

The standard visual acuity measurements rely on stationary stimuli, either letters (Snellen charts), vertical lines (vernier acuity) or grating charts, processed by those regions of the visual system most sensitive to the stationary stimulation, receiving visual input from the central part of the visual field. Here, an acuity measurement is proposed based on discrimination of simple shapes, that are defined by motion of the dots in the random dot kinematograms (RDK) processed by visual regions sensitive to motion stimulation and receiving input also from the peripheral visual field. In the motion-acuity test, participants are asked to distinguish between a circle and an ellipse, with matching surfaces, built from RDKs, and separated from the background RDK either by coherence, direction, or velocity of dots. The acuity measurement is based on ellipse detection, which with every correct response becomes more circular until reaching the acuity threshold. The motion-acuity test can be presented in negative contrast (black dots on white background) or in positive contrast (white dots on black background). The motion defined shapes are located centrally within 8 visual degrees and are surrounded by RDK background. To test the influence of visual peripheries on centrally measured acuity, a mechanical narrowing of the visual field to 10 degrees is proposed, using opaque goggles with centrally located holes. This easy and replicable narrowing system is suitable for MRI protocols, allowing further investigations of the functions of the peripheral visual input. Here, a simple measurement of shape and motion perception simultaneously is proposed. This straightforward test assesses vision impairments depending on the central and peripheral visual field inputs. The proposed motion-acuity test advances the capability of standard tests to reveal spare or even strengthened vision functions in patients with injured visual system, that until now remained undetected.


Motion Perception , Visual Fields , Humans , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Visual Acuity , Motion Perception/physiology , Psychophysics
19.
Int J Mol Sci ; 25(3)2024 Feb 02.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38339094

Neuropathy is a serious and frequent complication of type 2 diabetes (T2DM). This study was carried out to search for genetic factors associated with the development of diabetic neuropathy by whole exome sequencing. For this study, 24 patients with long-term type 2 diabetes with neuropathy and 24 without underwent detailed neurological assessment and whole exome sequencing. Cardiovascular autonomic function was evaluated by cardiovascular reflex tests. Heart rate variability was measured by the triangle index. Sensory nerve function was estimated by Neurometer and Medoc devices. Neuropathic symptoms were characterized by the neuropathy total symptom score (NTSS). Whole exome sequencing (WES) was performed on a Thermo Ion GeneStudio S5 system determining the coding sequences of approximately 32,000 genes comprising 50 million base pairs. Variants were detected by Ion Reporter software and annotated using ANNOVAR, integrating database information from dbSNP, ClinVar, gnomAD, and OMIM. Integrative genomics viewer (IGV) was used for visualization of the mapped reads. We have identified genetic variants that were significantly associated with increased (22-49-fold) risk of neuropathy (rs2032930 and rs2032931 of recQ-mediated genome instability protein 2 (RMI2) gene), rs604349 of myosin binding protein H like (MYBPHL) gene and with reduced (0.07-0.08-fold) risk (rs917778 of multivesicular body subunit 12B (MVB12B) and rs2234753 of retinoic acid X receptor alpha (RXRA) genes). The rs2032930 showed a significant correlation with current perception thresholds measured at 5 Hz and 250 Hz for n. medianus (p = 0.042 and p = 0.003, respectively) and at 5 Hz for n. peroneus (p = 0.037), as well as the deep breath test (p = 0.022) and the NTSS (p = 0.023). The rs2032931 was associated with current perception thresholds (p = 0.003 and p = 0.037, respectively), deep breath test (p = 0.022), and NTSS (p = 0.023). The rs604349 correlated with values measured at 2000 (p = 0.049), 250 (p = 0.018), and 5 Hz (p = 0.005) for n. medianus, as well as warm perception threshold measured by Medoc device (p = 0.042). The rs2234753 showed correlations with a current perception threshold measured at 2000 Hz for n. medianus (p = 0.020), deep breath test (p = 0.040), and NTSS (p = 0.003). There was a significant relationship between rs91778 and cold perception threshold (p = 0.013). In our study, genetic variants have been identified that may have an impact on the risk of neuropathy developing in type 2 diabetic patients. These results could open up new opportunities for early preventive measures and might provide targets for new drug developments in the future.


Diabetes Mellitus, Type 2 , Diabetic Neuropathies , Humans , Diabetes Mellitus, Type 2/complications , Diabetes Mellitus, Type 2/genetics , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Diabetic Neuropathies/genetics , Diabetic Neuropathies/diagnosis , Autonomic Nervous System , Sensation
20.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 86(1): 213-220, 2024 Jan.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38030820

Theoretically, the pulsed- and steady-pedestal paradigms are thought to track contrast-increment thresholds (ΔC) as a function of pedestal contrast (C) for the parvocellular (P) and magnocellular (M) systems, respectively, yielding linear ΔC versus C functions for the pulsed- and nonlinear functions for the steady-pedestal paradigm. A recent study utilizing these paradigms to isolate the P and M systems reported no evidence of the M system being suppressed by red light, contrary to previous physiological and psychophysical findings. Curious as to why this may have occurred, we examined how ΔC varies with C for the P and M systems using the pulsed- and steady-pedestal paradigms and stimuli biased towards the P or M systems based on their sensitivity to spatial frequency (SF) and color. We found no effect of color and little influence of SF. To explain this lack of color effects, we used a quantitative model of ΔC (as it changes with C) to obtain Csat and contrast-gain values. The contrast-gain values (i) contradicted the hypothesis that the steady-pedestal paradigm tracks the M-system response, and (ii) our obtained Csat values indicated strongly that both pulsed- and steady-pedestal paradigms track primarily the P-system response.


Contrast Sensitivity , Visual Pathways , Humans , Psychophysics , Photic Stimulation , Visual Pathways/physiology , Red Light , Sensory Thresholds/physiology
...