Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 17 de 17
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
Chem Senses ; 472022 01 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36056921

ABSTRACT

Odor stimuli are widely reported to promote relaxation and reduce anxiety in humans and rodents. However, it remains unclear if this anxiolytic efficacy can be further enhanced by association with positive experiences. Therefore, we compared the effects of a novel odor to a familiar odor previously paired with a positive experience on anxiety-like behaviors in rats. One group of Wistar-Imamichi female and male pups was exposed to an odor stimulus with their dams during postnatal days (PNDs) 8-12, whereas another control group was exposed to perfused air during the same period. Starting on PND 42, all animals were examined in the open field test (OFT) and elevated plus maze (EPM) test during exposure to scent-free air (vehicle), a novel odor, or the positive-familiar odor from postnatal exposure. In the EPM, female rats entered open arms with all 4 paws (complete entry) more frequently and spent more time on open arms during exposure to the positive-familiar odor than during exposure to air or a novel odor, whereas partial open arm entries with forepaws only were increased during exposure to both novel and positive-familiar odors compared to air. In contrast, male rats demonstrated no significant increase in open arm activity during positive-familiar odor exposure, but did show equally reduced grooming frequency during novel and familiar-positive odor exposure in the OFT. Exposure to positive-familiar odors may be an effective and safe method for anxiety reduction, especially in females.


Subject(s)
Anti-Anxiety Agents , Animals , Anti-Anxiety Agents/pharmacology , Anti-Anxiety Agents/therapeutic use , Anxiety , Female , Humans , Male , Maze Learning , Odorants , Rats , Rats, Wistar
3.
Exp Gerontol ; 149: 111335, 2021 07 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33785396

ABSTRACT

C57BL/6J and DBA/2J mice are often used for hearing research because of their early onset and progression of age-related hearing loss (AHL). Here, we report that the hardness of the diet affects the progression of AHL in these mice. When C57BL/6J mice and DBA/2J mice were fed a pellet-type or powder-type standard AIN93M diet, the pellet diet significantly promoted AHL. AHL promotion was eliminated by crushing the pellet diet to a powder. Subsequently, when C57BL/6J mice were fed the pellet-type AIN93M diet obtained from three different manufacturers, two of them significantly promoted AHL. The hardness of the diets was measured, and it was found that the two diets that promoted AHL were significantly harder than the other diet. Next, we attempted to reduce diet hardness by replacing some nutritional ingredients with dried eggs or phosphatidylcholine (PC), and we succeeded in obtaining brittle diets with lower hardness values. Then, C57BL/6J mice were bred with brittle diets for 6 months and the promotion of AHL was suppressed to the equivalent level as the powder diet. Furthermore, when senescence-accelerated mice, SAMP8, were fed a brittle diet for one year, the progression of AHL was also suppressed; however, it did not affect other aging indexes, such as mental and physical performance. We also confirmed that a high-fat pellet diet, which is soft even in pellet form, did not promote AHL. Time-restricted feeding (tRF), which is a chrono-nutritional method to delay aging, ameliorated the promotion of AHL by the hard AIN93M pellets in C57BL/6J mice. These results indicate that the physical form and hardness of diets affect the progression of AHL in mouse models.


Subject(s)
Presbycusis , Animals , Hardness , Hearing , Mice , Mice, Inbred C57BL , Mice, Inbred DBA
4.
Front Psychol ; 9: 1328, 2018.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30123151

ABSTRACT

We attempted to clarify positive benefits in cognitive abilities and motivation during our cognitive intervention [structured floral arrangement (SFA) program] for patients with neurocognitive disorder due to stroke, traumatic brain injury (TBI), and other related disorders. In this SFA program, participants are required to arrange cut flowers and leaves on absorbent foam according to an instruction sheet. In a previous study of patients with schizophrenia, our SFA program encouraged participants and contributed to stimulating their visuospatial process and memory. Here, 27 patients with neurocognitive disorders participated in this study. Sixteen patients were assigned to an SFA-treated group and participated in six sessions during two phases plus to daily activities. Eleven non-treated patients engaged only daily activities during the same period. We compared Apathy Scale scores and neuropsychological scores between the SFA-treated and non-treated patients. Their mean attendance rate was more than 90% during the two phases. SFA-treated patients copied a Rey-Osterrieth complex figure more accurately than non-treated patients (p < 0.05) during the later intervention phase, whereas during the earlier phase, accuracy was comparable between treated and non-treated groups. In the SFA-treated group, recall scores also improved (p < 0.01), and the positive outcomes were maintained for about 3 months (p < 0.05). The Apathy Scale scores did not show significant change in either the SFA-treated or non-treated groups. Our present results suggest that the SFA program encouraged continuous participation to cognitive intervention and was useful for ameliorating dysfunctions in visuospatial memory and recognition in patients with neurocognitive disorder.

5.
Sleep Med ; 47: 72-76, 2018 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29778917

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: As connections between nightmares and various psychiatric disorders have been reported, the clinical significance of studying dream emotionality is now growing in importance. Because the olfactory bulb connects directly to the amygdala odor presentation may be a crucial tool to study dream emotions. Previous studies have demonstrated that presentation of positive/negative odors during rapid eye movement (REM) sleep affects various aspects of dreaming. Although olfactory perception can be influenced by personal experiences, the role of individual preferences in the effects of olfactory stimuli on dreaming has not yet been clarified. The purpose of the current study was to clarify the effects of odor on dreaming during REM sleep, taking individual preferences into account. METHODS: Phenyl ethyl alcohol (rose-like smell) airflow was presented as an experimental stimulus, and odorless airflow was presented as the control. Participants who like (n = 7) and dislike (n = 8) the odor of phenyl ethyl alcohol were presented air with and without the odor of phenyl ethyl alcohol, respectively, during REM sleep and then awakened to report and rate their dream contents. Thereafter, the transcribed dream reports were rated by independent raters. RESULTS AND CONCLUSIONS: Participants who liked the odor of phenyl ethyl alcohol reported more emotionally negative dreams when they were presented with phenyl ethyl alcohol airflow than that with odorless airflow. In other words, the participant's favorite odor was associated with emotionally negative dreams. These findings could be attributed to the nature of odor perception and the characteristics of brain activities during REM sleep.


Subject(s)
Dreams/psychology , Emotions , Odorants , Sleep, REM/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Polysomnography/methods , Surveys and Questionnaires , Young Adult
6.
PLoS One ; 10(11): e0143502, 2015.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26605547

ABSTRACT

In the petals of some species of flowers, programmed cell death (PCD) begins earlier in mesophyll cells than in epidermal cells. However, PCD progression in each cell type has not been characterized in detail. We separately constructed a time course of biochemical signs and expression patterns of PCD-associated genes in epidermal and mesophyll cells in Lilium cv. Yelloween petals. Before visible signs of senescence could be observed, we found signs of PCD, including DNA degradation and decreased protein content in mesophyll cells only. In these cells, the total proteinase activity increased on the day after anthesis. Within 3 days after anthesis, the protein content decreased by 61.8%, and 22.8% of mesophyll cells was lost. A second peak of proteinase activity was observed on day 6, and the number of mesophyll cells decreased again from days 4 to 7. These biochemical and morphological results suggest that PCD progressed in steps during flower life in the mesophyll cells. PCD began in epidermal cells on day 5, in temporal synchrony with the time course of visible senescence. In the mesophyll cells, the KDEL-tailed cysteine proteinase (LoCYP) and S1/P1 nuclease (LoNUC) genes were upregulated before petal wilting, earlier than in epidermal cells. In contrast, relative to that in the mesophyll cells, the expression of the SAG12 cysteine proteinase homolog (LoSAG12) drastically increased in epidermal cells in the final stage of senescence. These results suggest that multiple PCD-associated genes differentially contribute to the time lag of PCD progression between epidermal and mesophyll cells of lily petals.


Subject(s)
Apoptosis/genetics , Flowers/genetics , Lilium/genetics , Mesophyll Cells/metabolism , Aging/genetics , Cell Survival/genetics , DNA Fragmentation , Gene Expression Regulation, Plant , Lilium/metabolism , Nitrogen/metabolism , Peptide Hydrolases/metabolism , Plant Proteins/genetics , Plant Proteins/metabolism
7.
Neuropsychol Rehabil ; 20(4): 624-36, 2010 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20467963

ABSTRACT

Several cognitive therapies have been developed for patients with schizophrenia. However, little is known about the outcomes of these therapies in terms of non-verbal/visuospatial working memory, even though this may affect patients' social outcomes. In the present pilot study, we investigated the effect of a structured floral arrangement (SFA) programme, where participants were required to create symmetrical floral arrangements. In this programme, the arrangement pattern and the order of placing each of the natural materials was predetermined. Participants have to identify where to place each material, and memorise the position temporarily to complete the floral arrangement. The schizophrenic patients who participated in this programme showed significant improvement in their scores for a block-tapping task backward version; whereas, non-treated control patients did not show such an improvement. The present results suggest that the SFA programme may positively stimulate visuospatial working memory in patients.


Subject(s)
Cognitive Behavioral Therapy/methods , Memory Disorders/etiology , Memory Disorders/rehabilitation , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Schizophrenia/complications , Spatial Behavior/physiology , Adult , Female , Flowers , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Psychomotor Performance/physiology
8.
Exp Brain Res ; 202(1): 147-53, 2010 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20020114

ABSTRACT

A 2 x 8 button-press task is a sequential hand movement task in which subjects are required to press eight pairs of buttons as accurately and quickly as possible. The 2 x 8 task allows us to examine flexible sequential learning, more aptly called sequence-unselective learning. Sequence-unselective learning is observed after repeated experiences with the task, when subjects have shown good progress in learning, with new sequences as well as previously learned ones. Although cognitive inflexibility has been reported in patients with Parkinson's disease (PD), there have been few studies investigating their flexibility in sequential learning. We examined PD patients' ability for sequence-unselective learning through the use of a 2 x 8 button-press task. In the first session, PD patients and subjects from the control group performed a sequential 2 x 8 task until the learning criterion was fulfilled (Session 1). After 1 month, they participated in other sessions: one involving the learned sequence (Session 2) and another involving the new sequence (Session 3). We found that PD patients made more errors than the normal control subjects only when learning the new sequence (Session 3) (P < 0.01). In Session 3, control subjects reached the learning target with fewer errors than in the Session 1 (normal sequence-unselective learning), whereas the PD patients did not exhibit such an improvement. Our results revealed a sequence-unselective deficit in PD patients. The deficit may help to emphasize the cognitive and physical inflexibility of PD.


Subject(s)
Learning Disabilities , Motor Skills , Parkinson Disease , Aged , Dopamine Agents/therapeutic use , Female , Hand , Humans , Learning , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Parkinson Disease/drug therapy , Psychomotor Performance , Time Factors
9.
Neurosci Res ; 63(3): 177-83, 2009 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19110014

ABSTRACT

Episodic memory retrieval and reasoning are fundamental psychological components of our daily lives. Although previous studies have investigated the brain regions associated with these processes separately, the neural mechanisms of reasoning based on episodic memory retrieval are largely unknown. Here, we investigated the neural correlates underlying episodic memory-based reasoning using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). During fMRI scanning, subjects performed three tasks: reasoning, episodic memory retrieval, and episodic memory-based reasoning. We identified dissociable activations related to reasoning, episodic memory retrieval, and linking processes between the two. Regions related to reasoning were identified in the left ventral prefrontal cortices (PFC), and those related to episodic memory retrieval were found in the right medial temporal lobe (MTL) regions. In addition, activations predominant in the linking process between the two were found in the left dorsal and right ventral PFC. These findings suggest that episodic memory-based reasoning is composed of at least three processes, i.e., reasoning, episodic memory retrieval, and linking processes between the two, and that activation of both the PFC and MTL is crucial in episodic memory-based reasoning. These findings are the first to demonstrate that PFC and MTL regions contribute differentially to each process in episodic memory-based reasoning.


Subject(s)
Memory/physiology , Prefrontal Cortex/physiology , Temporal Lobe/physiology , Thinking/physiology , Analysis of Variance , Brain Mapping , Female , Functional Laterality , Humans , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted/methods , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Oxygen/blood , Photic Stimulation/methods , Prefrontal Cortex/blood supply , Reaction Time/physiology , Temporal Lobe/blood supply , Young Adult
10.
Brain Nerve ; 60(7): 825-32, 2008 Jul.
Article in Japanese | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18646622

ABSTRACT

Procedural memory is acquired by trial and error. Our daily life is supported by a number of procedural memories such as those for riding bicycle, typing, reading words, etc. Procedural memory is divided into 3 types; motor, perceptual, and cognitive. Here, the author reviews the cognitive and neural basis of procedural memory according to these 3 types. It is reported that the basal ganglia or cerebellum dysfunction causes deficits in procedural memory. Compared with age-matched healthy participants, patients with Parkinson disease (PD), Huntington disease (HD) or spinocerebellar degeneration (SCD) show deterioration in improvements in motor-type procedural memory tasks. Previous neuroimaging studies have reported that motor-type procedural memory may be supported by multiple brain regions, including the frontal and parietal regions as well as the basal ganglia (cerebellum); this was found with a serial reaction time task (SRT task). Although 2 other types of procedural memory are also maintained by multiple brain regions, the related cerebral areas depend on the type of memory. For example, it was suggested that acquisition of the perceptual type of procedural memory (e.g., ability to read mirror images of words) might be maintained by the bilateral fusiform region, while the acquisition of cognitive procedural memory might be supported by the frontal, parietal, or cerebellar regions as well as the basal ganglia. In the future, we need to cleary understand the neural "network" related to the procedural memory.


Subject(s)
Basal Ganglia/physiology , Cerebellum/physiology , Memory/physiology , Cognition/physiology , Frontal Lobe/physiology , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Motor Activity/physiology , Parietal Lobe/physiology , Parkinson Disease/psychology , Perception/physiology
11.
J Telemed Telecare ; 14(4): 215-8, 2008.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18534958

ABSTRACT

We conducted videophone conversations with elderly adults living in a nursing home to discover whether their verbal ability was improved by repeated remote conversations. The control group comprised five elderly adults. The experimental group comprised six elderly adults, including three patients with dementia. They took part in three videophone conversations and their verbal ability was measured before and after the conversations. During the sessions, the participants and volunteers talked about themselves and participated in a quiz. In the experimental group, the mean number of words generated increased from 4.8 to 6.5 after the session (P < 0.05); this increase was not observed in the control group. The experimental group also showed an improvement in words generated after indirect conversations. Videophone conversations appear to have some positive effects on verbal ability and may be beneficial in the cognitive rehabilitation of elderly adults.


Subject(s)
Communication , Verbal Behavior/physiology , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Cognition Disorders/rehabilitation , Female , Frail Elderly/psychology , Humans , Male , Pilot Projects , Videoconferencing/statistics & numerical data
12.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 29(12): 1343-54, 2008 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17948885

ABSTRACT

Although previous studies have suggested the importance of the bilateral anterior temporal (ATL) and medial temporal lobes (MTL) in the retrieval of person identity information, there is little evidence concerning how these regions differentially contribute to the process. Here we investigated this question using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Before scanning, subjects learned associations among faces (F), names (N), and job titles (as a form of person-related semantics, S). During retrieval with fMRI, subjects were presented with previously learned and new S stimuli, and judged whether the stimuli were old or new. Successful retrieval (H) trials were divided into three conditions: retrieval of S and associated F and N (HSFN); retrieval of S and associated F (HSF); and retrieval of S only (HS). The left ATL was significantly activated in HSFN, compared to HSF or HS, whereas the right ATL and MTL were significantly activated in HSFN and HSF relative to HS. In addition, activity in bilateral ATL was significantly correlated with reaction time for HSFN, whereas we found no significant correlation between activity in the right MTL and reaction time in any condition. The present findings suggest that the left ATL may mediate associations between names and person-related semantic information, whereas the right ATL mediates the association between faces and person-related semantic information in memory for person identity information. In addition, activation of the right MTL region implies that this area may contribute to a more general relational processing of associative components, including memory for person identity information.


Subject(s)
Face , Memory/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Temporal Lobe/anatomy & histology , Temporal Lobe/physiology , Association Learning/physiology , Brain Mapping , Entorhinal Cortex/anatomy & histology , Entorhinal Cortex/physiology , Female , Functional Laterality/physiology , Hippocampus/anatomy & histology , Hippocampus/physiology , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Nerve Net/anatomy & histology , Nerve Net/physiology , Neuropsychological Tests , Parahippocampal Gyrus/anatomy & histology , Parahippocampal Gyrus/physiology , Photic Stimulation , Reaction Time/physiology , Semantics , Social Behavior , Verbal Behavior/physiology , Young Adult
13.
Brain Res ; 1122(1): 154-60, 2006 Nov 29.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17059812

ABSTRACT

A previous neuroimaging study has indicated that the visual dorsal stream may contribute to accurate reading of mirror-reversed words. However, the role of the visual ventral stream in the learning of mirror reading skill remains ambiguous. In the present fMRI study, we investigated learning-related changes in brain activation in the visual ventral stream in a mirror reading task. Subjects participated in three successive runs of the mirror reading task, in each of which they were asked to read mirror-reversed words and normal words as accurately and as quickly as possible. The behavioral data for the mirror reading condition showed significant improvement in reaction time but not in performance accuracy across the three runs. The activation data showed different learning-associated patterns related to the right and left visual ventral streams. On the right side, activity related to the reading of mirror stimuli was significantly greater than that related to normal stimuli in the first run only, whereas on the left side it was greater in all runs. Additional correlation analysis between response time data and percentage signal changes only in the mirror reading condition showed significant correlation on the right visual ventral stream in the first run only, whereas that on the left visual ventral stream was found only in the third run. The dissociable response between the right and left visual ventral streams may reflect learning-related changes in reading strategy and may be critical in improving the speed of reading mirror-reversed words.


Subject(s)
Discrimination Learning/physiology , Form Perception/physiology , Reading , Visual Pathways/physiology , Adult , Female , Functional Laterality/physiology , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Photic Stimulation , Reaction Time/physiology , Reference Values
14.
Neuropsychologia ; 44(7): 1114-9, 2006.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16321406

ABSTRACT

Previous studies suggested that perceptual memory as indexed by visual priming is normal in patients with Alzheimer's disease (AD). However, these studies did not specifically test the long-term effects of visual priming, which may differ significantly between Alzheimer's patients and normal subjects. To test this possibility, we examined long-term visual priming in AD patients, 1 hour, 1 month, and 3 months after training. Our results indicated a significant difference in visual priming between AD patients and normal subjects after 3 months, but not 1 month. For AD patients, there was a strong positive correlation between the 3-month priming effect and the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) scores-severely demented patients were less likely to exhibit priming after 3 months. It appears that severe cortical degeneration may render AD patients unable to consolidate their perceptual memories. Our results suggest that lack of visual priming in AD patients is linked to the inability to maintain fragmented perceptual memories.


Subject(s)
Alzheimer Disease/diagnosis , Amnesia/diagnosis , Attention , Cues , Mental Recall , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Alzheimer Disease/psychology , Amnesia/psychology , Female , Follow-Up Studies , Humans , Male , Mental Status Schedule , Middle Aged , Neurodegenerative Diseases/diagnosis , Neurodegenerative Diseases/psychology , Perceptual Closure , Reference Values , Retention, Psychology , Statistics as Topic
15.
Neuroimage ; 30(2): 617-26, 2006 Apr 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16275140

ABSTRACT

Previous studies have suggested the importance of bilateral anterior temporal regions in face-name associations, but there is little evidence concerning their precise role. In this fMRI study, we investigated the effects of person-related semantics (PS) and repeated learning (R) on activations in these regions during the retrieval of face-name associations. For encoding stimuli, we prepared four lists of faces. To control the factor of PS, people's and occupation names were attached to the faces in lists A and B, whereas only people's names were attached to the faces in lists C and D. To control the factor of R, the stimuli in lists A and C were learned twice, whereas the stimuli in lists B and D were learned seven times during encoding before fMRI. During fMRI after the encoding, subjects participated in the retrieval task of people's names from faces or in the retrieval task of faces from people's names. The left anterior temporal lobe was significantly activated during the retrieval of people's names from faces encoded with, compared to without, PS; whereas the right anterior temporal lobe was activated during the retrieval of people's faces from names, compared to without, this encoding. Also, activation of the left (but not the right) anterior temporal lobe was significantly reduced after R of face-name associations. These findings suggest that the three components of faces, names, and PS may be mutually mediated by the bilateral anterior temporal lobe, whose activity may be dynamically changed by the level of consolidation of face-name associations.


Subject(s)
Face , Recognition, Psychology/physiology , Temporal Lobe/physiology , Adult , Data Interpretation, Statistical , Female , Humans , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Memory/physiology , Photic Stimulation , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Semantics
16.
Neuroimage ; 25(2): 452-61, 2005 Apr 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15784424

ABSTRACT

It is known that manipulation of the encoding strategy affects behavioral and activation data during later retrieval. In the present fMRI study, we examined brain activity during the recognition of words encoded using three different strategies formed by the combination of two factors of relational and self-performed processes. The first encoding strategy involved subjects learning words using both relational and self-performed processes (R+S+). In the second, subjects learned words using only a relational process (R+S-). In the third, subjects learned words without using either process (R-S-). During fMRI after encoding, subjects were randomly presented with words encoded previously and with new words (New) and were required to judge whether or not the word presented had been previously encoded. The fMRI experiment was performed with the event-related design. Compared to New, activation of the left medial temporal lobe (MTL) occurred during the recognition of words encoded using R+S+ and R+S-, whereas right MTL activations only occurred with the R+S+ strategy. ROI analysis for the bilateral hippocampus and parahippocampal gyrus showed a linear increase in left MTL activity (hippocampus and parahippocampal gyrus) during the recognition of words encoded with the R-S-, R+S-, to R+S+, whereas right MTL activity (parahippocampal gyrus) was only increased with the R+S+ strategy. The findings suggest that the left and right MTL structures may contribute differentially to the processes involved in the recognition of stimuli and that these differential activities may depend on the encoding strategies formed by the two factors of relational and self-performed processes.


Subject(s)
Language , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Memory/physiology , Temporal Lobe/physiology , Adult , Humans , Male
17.
Neuropsychologia ; 42(13): 1858-63, 2004.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15351634

ABSTRACT

We investigated the acquisition and long-term retention of new skills in patients with cortical (Alzheimer's disease, AD) and subcortical (progressive supranuclear palsy, PSP; Parkinson's disease, PD) degeneration. The motor skill task performance of the PD and PSP patients improved with training, but the improvement disappeared within a few months, whereas AD patients retained learned skills for 3-18 months. The results of our experiments show that subcortical dysfunction induces a retention deficit for newly learned motor skills. Our present study suggests that a normal striatum is necessary for the formation of long-lasting motor skills, and that the striatum plays an important role as a motor skill consolidation system.


Subject(s)
Alzheimer Disease/physiopathology , Motor Skills/physiology , Parkinson Disease/physiopathology , Retention, Psychology/physiology , Supranuclear Palsy, Progressive/physiopathology , Follow-Up Studies , Humans , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Time Factors
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...