Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 125
Filter
1.
Prog Neurobiol ; 98(1): 82-98, 2012 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22609044

ABSTRACT

This paper presents a theoretical review of rapid eye movement sleep with a special focus on pontine-geniculate-occipital waves and what they might tell us about the functional anatomy of sleep and consciousness. In particular, we review established ideas about the nature and purpose of sleep in terms of protoconsciousness and free energy minimization. By combining these theoretical perspectives, we discover answers to some fundamental questions about sleep: for example, why is homeothermy suspended during sleep? Why is sleep necessary? Why are we not surprised by our dreams? What is the role of synaptic regression in sleep? The imperatives for sleep that emerge also allow us to speculate about the functional role of PGO waves and make some empirical predictions that can, in principle, be tested using recent advances in the modeling of electrophysiological data.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Consciousness , Dreams , Neurons/physiology , Synaptic Transmission , Wakefulness , Animals , Brain/anatomy & histology , Geniculate Bodies/anatomy & histology , Geniculate Bodies/physiology , Humans , Models, Biological , Occipital Lobe/anatomy & histology , Occipital Lobe/physiology , Pons/anatomy & histology , Pons/physiology
2.
Water Sci Technol ; 59(7): 1377-84, 2009.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19381004

ABSTRACT

A technique is described based on the decay in concentration of added SF(6) to measure L(0), the rate of leakage from an enclosure with no extraction of air. It is believed this measurement is much more precise than measurements of E(0), the minimum rate of extraction which just prevents leakage. Three out of four enclosures studied had L(0) values equating to residence times of air that were well under one hour. Relationships were developed between extraction rate and concentration and emission rate for enclosed odour sources based on mass transfer from water to air. These could be used to assess the benefits of minimising extraction rates while remaining within concentration limits set on the grounds of corrosion or toxicity. From these relationships a critical flow can be identified, termed Q(50), at which both the emission rate and concentration of a particular species are at 50% of their maximum value. In any particular system, Q50 for one species, such as H(2)S, will in general not be the same as for another species, nor for odour concentration. As a consequence the benefit of reducing extraction rates based on H(2)S may not appear as good as it would based on an assessment of odour concentration. A second consequence is that as the rate of air extraction is varied, the ratio between two species or between H(2)S and odour concentration, is likely to vary.


Subject(s)
Air Pollutants/isolation & purification , Environmental Monitoring/methods , Odorants/prevention & control , Ventilation , Volatilization
3.
Int J Obstet Anesth ; 15(1): 18-23, 2006 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16256338

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: The primary aim was to investigate whether preoperative anxiety in women undergoing elective caesarean section predicts postoperative maternal satisfaction with the process, perceptions of recovery, analgesic use or length of hospital stay. Other factors that might influence postoperative satisfaction were also explored. METHOD: In 85 women awaiting elective caesarean section, anxiety, social support and aspects of preparation were measured in the 24 hours preceding surgery. Maternal satisfaction and perceptions of recovery were assessed around the third postoperative day. Satisfaction with the preoperative information from the anaesthetist and postoperative pain relief were also measured at this time. Medical notes were used to gather information on analgesia use and length of hospital stay. RESULTS: Preoperative anxiety scores were comparable with those of general surgical/medical patients. Preoperative trait anxiety and state anxiety were inversely associated with postoperative maternal satisfaction. State anxiety was also inversely associated with better recovery. Preoperative anxiety was not associated with analgesic use or length of hospital stay. Linear regression analysis indicated the degree of satisfaction with information from the anaesthetist and perceived emotional support from the partner explained 52% of the variance in postoperative maternal satisfaction. CONCLUSION: Lower preoperative anxiety is associated with greater maternal satisfaction with elective caesarean section and better recovery. Information provided by anaesthetists and perceived emotional support are also of importance. It may be possible to identify women with high anxiety and facilitate satisfaction and recovery through providing additional supportive input.


Subject(s)
Anxiety/etiology , Cesarean Section/psychology , Patient Satisfaction , Adult , Anesthesia, Obstetrical , Anesthesia, Spinal , Anxiety/diagnosis , Female , Humans , Pain, Postoperative/drug therapy , Patient Education as Topic , Pregnancy , Social Support
5.
Science ; 294(5544): 1052-7, 2001 Nov 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11691983

ABSTRACT

Converging evidence and new research methodologies from across the neurosciences permit the neuroscientific study of the role of sleep in off-line memory reprocessing, as well as the nature and function of dreaming. Evidence supports a role for sleep in the consolidation of an array of learning and memory tasks. In addition, new methodologies allow the experimental manipulation of dream content at sleep onset, permitting an objective and scientific study of this dream formation and a renewed search for the possible functions of dreaming and the biological processes subserving it.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Dreams/physiology , Learning/physiology , Memory/physiology , Sleep/physiology , Animals , Cognition , Emotions , Humans , Models, Neurological , Models, Psychological , Sleep Deprivation/physiopathology , Sleep, REM/physiology
6.
J Clin Psychiatry ; 62(8): 642-52, 2001 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11561938

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Acute and chronic administration of the selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) have been widely reported to disrupt sleep in laboratory studies. This study examines the naturalistic, longitudinal effects of paroxetine and fluvoxamine on sleep quality in the home setting. METHOD: Fourteen healthy volunteers free of medical and neuropsychiatric symptoms entered a 31-day protocol: 7 days of drug-free baseline (days 1-7), 19 days of drug treatment (steady state during days 18-26), and 5 days of acute withdrawal (days 27-31). On day 8, the subjects were randomly assigned to receive either 100 mg/day of fluvoxamine or 20 mg/day of paroxetine (half receiving each drug) in divided morning and evening oral doses. Investigators remained blinded to drug assignment until all sleep data had been analyzed. Sleep was monitored using the Nightcap ambulatory sleep monitor. Four standard and 3 novel measures were computed and compared using multivariate analysis of variance, analysis of variance, and Bonferroni-corrected comparison of means. RESULTS: Sleep disruption was most clearly demonstrated using the novel measures eyelid quiescence index, rhythmicity, and eyelid movements per minute in non-rapid eye movement sleep, but was also apparent as determined by standard measures of sleep efficiency, number of awakenings, and sleep onset latency. Paroxetine disrupted sleep more than fluvoxamine, and paroxetine-induced sleep disruption persisted into the withdrawal phase. Rapid eye movement sleep was suppressed during treatment (especially for fluvoxamine) and rebounded during withdrawal (especially for paroxetine). CONCLUSION: We confirm laboratory polysomnographic findings of SSRI-induced sleep quality changes and demonstrate the Nightcap's efficacy as an inexpensive longitudinal monitor for objective sleep changes induced by psychotropic medication.


Subject(s)
Fluvoxamine/pharmacology , Monitoring, Physiologic/statistics & numerical data , Paroxetine/pharmacology , Selective Serotonin Reuptake Inhibitors/pharmacology , Sleep/drug effects , Adult , Circadian Rhythm/drug effects , Circadian Rhythm/physiology , Circadian Rhythm/radiation effects , Drug Administration Schedule , Equipment Design/methods , Eyelids/physiology , Female , Fluvoxamine/administration & dosage , Fluvoxamine/adverse effects , Head/physiology , Humans , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Monitoring, Ambulatory/instrumentation , Monitoring, Ambulatory/methods , Monitoring, Physiologic/instrumentation , Movement/physiology , Paroxetine/administration & dosage , Paroxetine/adverse effects , Polysomnography/drug effects , Polysomnography/instrumentation , Selective Serotonin Reuptake Inhibitors/administration & dosage , Selective Serotonin Reuptake Inhibitors/adverse effects , Sleep/physiology , Sleep Stages/drug effects , Sleep Stages/physiology , Sleep, REM/drug effects , Sleep, REM/physiology , Substance Withdrawal Syndrome/diagnosis , Substance Withdrawal Syndrome/etiology
7.
J Sleep Res ; 10(2): 129-42, 2001 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11422727

ABSTRACT

Clinical lore and a small number of published studies report that the selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) intensify dreaming. This study examines the dream effects of paroxetine and fluvoxamine in order to both increase clinical knowledge of these agents and to test an important potential method for probing the relationship between REM sleep neurobiology and dreaming in humans. Fourteen normal, paid volunteers (4 males, 10 females; mean age 27.4 year, range 22--39) free of medical or neuropsychiatric symptoms as well as of psychotropic or sleep affecting drugs completed a 31-day home-based study consisting of: 7 days drug-free baseline; 19 days on either 100 mg fluvoxamine (7 Ss) or 20 mg paroxetine (7 Ss) in divided morning and evening doses; and 5 days acute discontinuation. Upon awakening, subjects wrote dream reports, self-scored specific emotions in their reports and rated seven general dream characteristics using 5-point Likert scales. Dream reports were independently scored for bizarreness, movement and number of visual nouns by three judges. REM sleep-related measures were obtained using the Nightcap ambulatory sleep monitor. Mean dream recall frequency decreased during treatment compared with baseline. Dream report length and judge-rated bizarreness were greater during acute discontinuation compared with both baseline and treatment and this effect was a result of the fluvoxamine-treated subjects. The subjective intensity of dreaming increased during both treatment and acute discontinuation compared with baseline. Propensity to enter REM sleep was decreased during treatment compared with baseline and acute discontinuation and the intensity of REM sleep increased during acute discontinuation compared with baseline and treatment. The decrease in dream frequency during SSRI treatment may reflect serotonergic REM suppression while the augmented report length and bizarreness during acute SSRI discontinuation may reflect cholinergic rebound from serotonergic suppression.


Subject(s)
Dreams/drug effects , Fluvoxamine/pharmacology , Mental Recall/drug effects , Paroxetine/pharmacology , Selective Serotonin Reuptake Inhibitors/pharmacology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Periodicity , Random Allocation , Sleep, REM/physiology , Surveys and Questionnaires
8.
Psychol Sci ; 12(1): 30-6, 2001 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11294225

ABSTRACT

The exclusion of thinking from recent studies of sleep mentation has hindered a full appreciation of how cognitive activity differs across the states of waking and sleep. To overcome this limitation, this study investigated thoughts and hallucinations using experience sampling, home-based sleep-wake monitoring, and formal analyses of the psychological data. The prevalence of thoughts decreased gradually from waking through sleep onset and non-REM sleep, to reach its nadir in REM sleep, whereas hallucinations increased sharply across these states. Furthermore, multiple occurrences of hallucinations but not of thoughts increased significantly from sleep onset through non-REM sleep, to a peak in REM sleep. This reciprocity in thoughts and hallucinations might reflect a progressive shift from high to low aminergic-to-cholinergic neuromodulatory ratios across wake-sleep states, accompanied by an array of changes in the regional activation patterns of the brain.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Hallucinations/epidemiology , Sleep Stages/physiology , Thinking/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Humans , Prevalence , Sleep, REM/physiology , Time Factors
9.
Sleep ; 24(2): 171-9, 2001 Mar 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11247053

ABSTRACT

STUDY OBJECTIVES: To collect and analyze reports of mental activity across sleep/wake states. DESIGN: Mentation reports were collected in a longitudinal design by combining our Nightcap sleep monitor with daytime experience sampling techniques. Reports were collected over 14 days and nights from active and quiet wake, after instrumental awakenings at sleep onset, and after both spontaneous and instrumental awakenings from REM and NREM sleep. SETTING: All reports were collected in the normal home, work and school environments of the subjects. PARTICIPANTS: Subjects included 8 male and 8 female undergraduate students (19-26 years of age). INTERVENTIONS: N/A. MEASUREMENTS AND RESULTS: A total of 1,748 reports, averaging 109 per subject, were collected from active wake across the day (n=894), from quiet wake in the pre-sleep onset period (n=58), from sleep onset (n=280), and from later REM (n=269) and nonREM (n=247) awakenings. Median report lengths varied more than 2-fold, in the order REM > active wake > quiet wake > NREM = sleep onset. The extended protocol allowed many novel comparisons between conditions. In addition, while spontaneous REM reports were longer than those from forced awakenings, the difference was explained by the time within the REM period at which the awakenings occurred. Finally, intersubject differences in REM report lengths were correlated with similar differences in waking report lengths. CONCLUSIONS: The use of the Nightcap sleep monitoring system along with waking experience sampling permits a more complete sampling and analysis of mental activity across the sleep/wake cycle than has been previously possible.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Dreams/psychology , Sleep Stages/physiology , Wakefulness/physiology , Adult , Circadian Rhythm , Dreams/physiology , Female , Humans , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Psychophysiology , Sleep, REM/physiology , Time Factors
10.
Sleep ; 24(8): 947-55, 2001 Dec 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11766165

ABSTRACT

STUDY OBJECTIVES: No consensus has been reached on the characteristics of emotional experience during rapid eye movement sleep (REM). Thus, the relationship between the emotional brain activation and mental activity in REM remains unclear. Our objective is to characterize emotional experience in REM in order to facilitate understanding of brain-mind correlations in this state. DESIGN: We combined instrumental awakenings from REM with the subjects' own ratings of the occurrence and intensity of discrete emotion types for each line in their REM mentation reports. SETTING: The study was performed in the subjects' own homes over three consecutive nights using ambulatory polysomnography. PARTICIPANTS: Nine normal healthy subjects, age 31-60 (mean=43.0). INTERVENTIONS: Awakenings 5-15 minutes into REM periods across the night. MEASUREMENTS AND RESULTS: Emotions were found in 74% of 88 mentation reports, with a balanced proportion of positive and negative emotions. Among the reports scored for emotions, 14% contained one emotion and 86% contained two or more different emotion types. Joy/elation was the most frequent emotion, found in 36% of the reports, followed by surprise (24%), anger (17%), anxiety/fear (11%), and sadness (10%). Anxiety/fear was significantly less intense than joy/elation, anger, and surprise. Except for surprise, no specific emotion type changed from the first to the second half of the night. Negative emotions and surprise but not positive emotions varied significantly across subjects. CONCLUSIONS: The analysis of subject reports of emotions following instrumental awakenings demonstrate a balanced and widespread occurrence of both positive and negative emotions in REM sleep dreams. Emotions in REM are likely to be powerfully modulated by the neurobiological processes which differentiate REM from waking.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Emotions/physiology , Mental Processes/physiology , Sleep, REM/physiology , Adult , Ambulatory Care , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Polysomnography/instrumentation , Time Factors , Wakefulness/physiology
12.
J Sleep Res ; 9(2): 185-91, 2000 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10849245

ABSTRACT

Microarousals (MAs) are brief transient events that occur during normal sleep in humans and with increased frequency in disordered sleep, especially in association with sleep apnoea. In a feline model, we discovered transient cardiorespiratory events during nonrapid eye movement (NREM) sleep that exhibited consistent features with similarities to human MAs. It was observed that MAs have two distinct phases. Phase I (MAI) is characterized by an abrupt increase in electromyogram (EMG) amplitude (> 50%), increased electrooculogram (EOG) activity and accelerated frequency of hippocampal electroencephalographic (EEG) activity. MAI lasts 4.1 +/- 0.3 s. Phase II (MAII), lasting 9.8 +/- 0.8 s, is characterized by high frequency EEG activity, but EMG, EOG and hippocampal activity remain at baseline levels. Mean inspiratory rate begins to increase 15 s before the onset of the MA, followed 10 s later by the increase in mean heart rate. Mean respiratory rate decreases sharply through MAII, and returns to baseline levels 15 s after the MA. During MAII mean heart rate decreases quickly; there is increased respiratory irregularity, followed by a prolonged ventilatory overshoot. The abrupt shift in heart rate is coincident with the change in breath timing seen during MAII. Heart rate returns to baseline levels 10 s following the MA. Integrating our findings with those described previously in humans, we propose that MAs may serve as a homeostatic mechanism which is designed to restore cardiorespiratory function allowing the continuity of sleep.


Subject(s)
Arousal/physiology , Brain/physiology , Heart Rate/physiology , Models, Biological , Respiration , Sleep, REM/physiology , Animals , Cats , Electroencephalography , Electromyography , Electrooculography , Homeostasis/physiology , Male , Polysomnography , Time Factors
13.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 12(2): 246-54, 2000 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10771409

ABSTRACT

Performance on a visual discrimination task shows long-term improvement after a single training session. When tested within 24 hr of training, improvement was not observed unless subjects obtained at least 6 hr of posttraining sleep prior to retesting, in which case improvement was proportional to the amount of sleep in excess of 6 hr. For subjects averaging 8 hr of sleep, overnight improvement was proportional to the amount of slow wave sleep (SWS) in the first quarter of the night, as well as the amount of rapid eye movement sleep (REM) in the last quarter. REM during the intervening 4 hr did not appear to contribute to improvement. A two-step process, modeling throughput as the product of the amount of early SWS and late REM, accounts for 80 percent of intersubject variance. These results suggest that, in the case of this visual discrimination task, both SWS and REM are required to consolidate experience-dependent neuronal changes into a form that supports improved task performance.


Subject(s)
Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Learning/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Sleep/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Wakefulness/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Electroencephalography , Electromyography , Electrooculography , Humans , Regression Analysis , Sleep Stages/physiology , Sleep, REM/physiology , Time Factors
14.
Int J Psychophysiol ; 35(2-3): 143-54, 2000 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10677643

ABSTRACT

Loneliness is a complex set of feelings encompassing reactions to unfulfilled intimate and social needs. Although transient for some individuals, loneliness can be a chronic state for others. Prior research has shown that loneliness is a major risk factor for psychological disturbances and for broad-based morbidity and mortality. We examined differences between lonely and socially embedded individuals that might explain differences in health outcomes. Satisfying social relationships were associated with more positive outlooks on life, more secure attachments and interactions with others, more autonomic activation when confronting acute psychological challenges, and more efficient restorative behaviors. Individuals who were chronically lonely were characterized by elevated mean salivary cortisol levels across the course of a day, suggesting more discharges of corticotropin-releasing hormone and elevated activation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocorticol axis. An experimental manipulation of loneliness further suggested that the way in which people construe their self in relation to others around them has powerful effects on their self concept and, possibly, on their physiology.


Subject(s)
Loneliness/psychology , Social Behavior , Humans , Psychophysiology , Risk Factors
15.
Behav Neurosci ; 114(6): 1239-44, 2000 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11142656

ABSTRACT

The aim of this study was to develop a sleep-wake recording system for rats that would yield results more comparable to those obtained from cats than those that are usually reported. For 18 male Sprague-Dawley rats, the authors combined measures of cortical and hippocampal electroencephalogram (EEG) and neck muscle electromyogram with the electrooculogram and pontine EEG, so that the behavioral states could be identified with greater confidence with the use of polygraphic criteria developed in the cat and so that the distinctive phasic events of REM sleep could be more easily studied in the rat. The results suggest that for many neurophysiological studies, the rat is a suitable alternative to the cat.


Subject(s)
Models, Neurological , Polysomnography , Sleep Stages/physiology , Wakefulness/physiology , Animals , Brain Mapping , Cats , Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Hippocampus/physiology , Male , Pons/physiology , Rats , Rats, Sprague-Dawley , Sleep, REM/physiology
17.
Behav Brain Sci ; 23(6): 793-842; discussion 904-1121, 2000 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11515143

ABSTRACT

Sleep researchers in different disciplines disagree about how fully dreaming can be explained in terms of brain physiology. Debate has focused on whether REM sleep dreaming is qualitatively different from nonREM (NREM) sleep and waking. A review of psychophysiological studies shows clear quantitative differences between REM and NREM mentation and between REM and waking mentation. Recent neuroimaging and neurophysiological studies also differentiate REM, NREM, and waking in features with phenomenological implications. Both evidence and theory suggest that there are isomorphisms between the phenomenology and the physiology of dreams. We present a three-dimensional model with specific examples from normally and abnormally changing conscious states.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Dreams/physiology , Sleep Stages/physiology , Wakefulness/physiology , Animals , Brain Mapping , Humans , Psychophysiology , Sleep, REM/physiology
18.
J Sleep Res ; 9(4): 317-25, 2000 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11386201

ABSTRACT

The formal features of dream characters were studied in a sample of 320 dream reports submitted by 33 adult subjects (13 male, 20 female) of varying ages in a university extension course. Subjects were queried by questionnaire about dream characters immediately after recording their dreams upon awakening in their normal home setting. It was found that 48% of characters represented a named personage known to the dreamer, 35% were generically identified by their social role (e.g., policeman) or abstract relation to the dreamer (e.g., a friend) while only 16% were wholly novel. Seventy-seven percent of characters were pseudosensorily present in the dream whereas 23% were present only by mention or thought. Subjects were allowed to endorse one or more of four bases of recognition and, among named characters, 32% were identified by 'appearance', 21% by 'behavior', 45% by 'face', and 44% by 'just knowing' (with the respective percentages for generic characters being 39%, 38%, 9% and 40%). Fourteen percent of named and generic characters had associated some element of bizarreness most frequently consisting of an incongruous feature. Comparing the 25 longest and 25 shortest reports, named subjects were significantly more common in the shortest reports whereas generic and unknown characters were more common in the longest reports. Results are interpreted in neurocognitive terms as possibly reflecting a decrease during dreaming relative to waking in the exchange of information between inferotemporal face identification areas and prefrontal areas subserving logic and working memory.


Subject(s)
Consciousness/physiology , Dreams , Wakefulness , Adult , Electroencephalography , Female , Humans , Male , Sleep, REM/physiology , Surveys and Questionnaires
19.
Am J Physiol ; 277(3): R843-9, 1999 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10484502

ABSTRACT

Rapid eye movement (REM) sleep is characterized by periods of profound cardiac autonomic activation evident in heart rate surges in humans and canines. Our goals were to determine whether or not the heart rate surge phenomenon occurs in cats and to characterize concurrent central nervous system activity. Cortical and hippocampal electroencephalogram, electromyogram, electrooculogram, pontogeniculooccipital (PGO) waves, subcutaneous electrocardiogram, and respiration were recorded. Bouts of sinus tachycardia lasting >/=3.5 s achieved a rate of 210 beats/min and were present predominantly during REM sleep. Heart rate during the surges rose an average of 26.4% from 132.5 +/- 2.0 beats/min before the surge to 167.5 +/- 2.6 beats/min (P < 0.001) and returned to 130.7 +/- 2.6 beats/min (P < 0.001). The heart rate surges were invariably accompanied by increased incidence and frequency of hippocampal theta waves and increased PGO wave frequency and incidence of PGO wave clusters and eye movement clusters. The occurrence of surges was dramatically reduced from 0.11 +/- 0.03 to 0.01 +/- 0.01/15 s of REM sleep (P = 0.02) by atenolol (0.6 mg/kg iv), indicating that the phenomenon is beta(1)-adrenergically mediated. These findings suggest a coupling between central activation of cardiac sympathetic nerves and the generation of hippocampal theta waves and PGO activity.


Subject(s)
Heart Rate/physiology , Hippocampus/physiology , Sleep, REM/physiology , Animals , Cats , Electroencephalography
20.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 11(2): 182-93, 1999 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10198133

ABSTRACT

The notion that dreaming might alter the strength of associative links in memory was first proposed almost 200 years ago. But no strong evidence of such altered associative links has been obtained. Semantic priming can be used to quantify the strength of associative links between pairs of words; it is thought to measure the automatic spread of activation from a "node" representing one word to nodes representing semantically related words. Semantic priming could thus be used to test for global alterations in the strengths of associative links across the wake-sleep cycle. Awakenings from REM and nonREM (NREM) sleep produce a period of state carry-over during which performance is altered as a result of the brain's slow transition to full wakefulness, and cognitive testing in this period can provide information about the functioning of the brain during the prior sleep period. When subjects were tested across the night--before and after a night's sleep as well as immediately following forced awakenings from REM and NREM sleep--weak priming (e. g., thief-wrong) was found to be state dependent (p = 0.016), whereas strong priming (e.g., hot-cold) was not (p = 0.89). Weak primes were most effective in the presleep and REM sleep conditions and least effective in NREM and postsleep conditions. Most striking are analyses comparing weak and strong priming within each wake-sleep state. Contrary to the normal pattern of priming, subjects awakened from REM sleep showed greater priming by weak primes than by strong primes (p = 0.01). This result was seen in each of three protocols. In contrast, strong priming exceeded weak priming in NREM sleep. The shift in weak priming seen after REM sleep awakenings suggests that cognition during REM sleep is qualitatively different from that of waking and NREM sleep and may reflect a shift in associative memory systems, a shift that we hypothesize underlies the bizarre and hyperassociative character of REM-sleep dreaming. Known changes in brainstem activity that control the transition into and maintenance of REM sleep provide a possible explanation of this shift.


Subject(s)
Association Learning/physiology , Dreams/physiology , Semantics , Sleep/physiology , Adult , Brain Mapping , Circadian Rhythm , Dominance, Cerebral/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Polysomnography , Reaction Time , Sleep Stages/physiology , Sleep, REM/physiology , Wakefulness/physiology
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...