Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 37.382
Filter
1.
J Vis ; 24(7): 4, 2024 Jul 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38975947

ABSTRACT

To dissociate aftereffects of size and density in the perception of relative numerosity, large or small adapter sizes were crossed with high or low adapter densities. A total of 48 participants were included in this preregistered design. To adapt the same retinotopic region as the large adapters, the small adapters were flashed in a sequence so as to "paint" the adapting density across the large region. Perceived numerosities and sizes in the adapted region were then compared to those in an unadapted region in separate blocks of trials, so that changes in density could be inferred. These density changes were found to be bidirectional and roughly symmetric, whereas the aftereffects of size and number were not symmetric. A simple account of these findings is that local adaptations to retinotopic density as well as global adaptations to size combine in producing numerosity aftereffects measured by assessing perceived relative number. Accounts based on number adaptation are contraindicated, in particular, by the result of adapting to a large, sparse adapter and testing with a stimulus with a double the density but half number of dots.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Physiological , Photic Stimulation , Size Perception , Humans , Size Perception/physiology , Female , Adult , Male , Photic Stimulation/methods , Adaptation, Physiological/physiology , Young Adult , Figural Aftereffect/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology
2.
Sci Rep ; 14(1): 16161, 2024 Jul 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38997432

ABSTRACT

Reading requires the transformation of a complex array of visual features into sounds and meaning. For deaf signers who experience changes in visual attention and have little or no access to the sounds of the language they read, understanding the visual constraints underlying reading is crucial. This study aims to explore a fundamental aspect of visual perception intertwined with reading: the crowding effect. This effect manifests as the struggle to distinguish a target letter when surrounded by flanker letters. Through a two-alternative forced choice task, we assessed the recognition of letters and symbols presented in isolation or flanked by two or four characters, positioned either to the left or right of fixation. Our findings reveal that while deaf individuals exhibit higher accuracy in processing letters compared to symbols, their performance falls short of that of their hearing counterparts. Interestingly, despite their proficiency with letters, deaf individuals didn't demonstrate quicker letter identification, particularly in the most challenging scenario where letters were flanked by four characters. These outcomes imply the development of a specialized letter processing system among deaf individuals, albeit one that may subtly diverge from that of their hearing counterparts.


Subject(s)
Deafness , Reading , Humans , Adult , Deafness/physiopathology , Male , Female , Visual Perception/physiology , Young Adult , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Attention/physiology , Middle Aged , Persons With Hearing Impairments/psychology
3.
J Vis ; 24(7): 8, 2024 Jul 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38990066

ABSTRACT

In the present study, we used Hierarchical Frequency Tagging (Gordon et al., 2017) to investigate in electroencephalography how different levels of the neural processing hierarchy interact with category-selective attention during visual object recognition. We constructed stimulus sequences of cyclic wavelet scrambled face and house stimuli at two different frequencies (f1 = 0.8 Hz and f2 = 1 Hz). For each trial, two stimulus sequences of different frequencies were superimposed and additionally augmented by a sinusoidal contrast modulation with f3 = 12.5 Hz. This allowed us to simultaneously assess higher level processing using semantic wavelet-induced frequency-tagging (SWIFT) and processing in earlier visual levels using steady-state visually evoked potentials (SSVEPs), along with their intermodulation (IM) components. To investigate the category specificity of the SWIFT signal, we manipulated the category congruence between target and distractor by superimposing two sequences containing stimuli from the same or different object categories. Participants attended to one stimulus (target) and ignored the other (distractor). Our results showed successful tagging of different levels of the cortical hierarchy. Using linear mixed-effects modeling, we detected different attentional modulation effects on lower versus higher processing levels. SWIFT and IM components were substantially increased for target versus distractor stimuli, reflecting attentional selection of the target stimuli. In addition, distractor stimuli from the same category as targets elicited stronger SWIFT signals than distractor stimuli from a different category indicating category-selective attention. In contrast, for IM components, this category-selective attention effect was largely absent, indicating that IM components probably reflect more stimulus-specific processing.


Subject(s)
Attention , Electroencephalography , Evoked Potentials, Visual , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Photic Stimulation , Humans , Attention/physiology , Male , Female , Electroencephalography/methods , Evoked Potentials, Visual/physiology , Young Adult , Adult , Photic Stimulation/methods , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology
4.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 121(28): e2321346121, 2024 Jul 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38954551

ABSTRACT

How does the brain process the faces of familiar people? Neuropsychological studies have argued for an area of the temporal pole (TP) linking faces with person identities, but magnetic susceptibility artifacts in this region have hampered its study with fMRI. Using data acquisition and analysis methods optimized to overcome this artifact, we identify a familiar face response in TP, reliably observed in individual brains. This area responds strongly to visual images of familiar faces over unfamiliar faces, objects, and scenes. However, TP did not just respond to images of faces, but also to a variety of high-level social cognitive tasks, including semantic, episodic, and theory of mind tasks. The response profile of TP contrasted with a nearby region of the perirhinal cortex that responded specifically to faces, but not to social cognition tasks. TP was functionally connected with a distributed network in the association cortex associated with social cognition, while PR was functionally connected with face-preferring areas of the ventral visual cortex. This work identifies a missing link in the human face processing system that specifically processes familiar faces, and is well placed to integrate visual information about faces with higher-order conceptual information about other people. The results suggest that separate streams for person and face processing reach anterior temporal areas positioned at the top of the cortical hierarchy.


Subject(s)
Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Temporal Lobe , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Temporal Lobe/physiology , Temporal Lobe/diagnostic imaging , Male , Female , Adult , Facial Recognition/physiology , Brain Mapping/methods , Recognition, Psychology/physiology , Face/physiology , Young Adult , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology
5.
J Vis ; 24(6): 6, 2024 Jun 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38843389

ABSTRACT

Infant primates see poorly, and most perceptual functions mature steadily beyond early infancy. Behavioral studies on human and macaque infants show that global form perception, as measured by the ability to integrate contour information into a coherent percept, improves dramatically throughout the first several years after birth. However, it is unknown when sensitivity to curvature and shape emerges in early life or how it develops. We studied the development of shape sensitivity in 18 macaques, aged 2 months to 10 years. Using radial frequency stimuli, circular targets whose radii are modulated sinusoidally, we tested monkeys' ability to radial frequency stimuli from circles as a function of the depth and frequency of sinusoidal modulation. We implemented a new four-choice oddity task and compared the resulting data with that from a traditional two-alternative forced choice task. We found that radial frequency pattern perception was measurable at the youngest age tested (2 months). Behavioral performance at all radial frequencies improved with age. Performance was better for higher radial frequencies, suggesting the developing visual system prioritizes processing of fine visual details that are ecologically relevant. By using two complementary methods, we were able to capture a comprehensive developmental trajectory for shape perception.


Subject(s)
Form Perception , Macaca mulatta , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Photic Stimulation , Animals , Form Perception/physiology , Photic Stimulation/methods , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Male , Female
6.
J Exp Psychol Gen ; 153(6): 1517-1536, 2024 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38884962

ABSTRACT

People can report summary statistics for various features about a group of objects. One theory is that different abilities support ensemble judgments about low-level features like color versus high-level features like identity. Existing research mostly evaluates such claims based on evidence of correlations within and between feature domains. However, correlations between two identical tasks that only differ in the type of feature that is used can be inflated by method variance. Another concern is that conclusions about high-level features are mostly based on faces. We used latent variable methods on data from 237 participants to investigate the abilities supporting low-level and high-level feature ensemble judgments. Ensemble judgment was measured with six distinct tests, each requiring judgments for a distinct low-level or high-level feature, using different task requirements. We also controlled for other general visual abilities when examining how low-level and high-level ensemble abilities relate to each other. Confirmatory factor analyses showed a perfect correlation between the two factors, suggesting a single ability. There was a unique relationship between these two factors beyond the influence of object recognition and perceptual speed. Additional results from 117 of the same participants also ruled out the role of working memory. This study provides strong evidence of a general ensemble judgment ability across a wide range of features at the latent level and characterizes its relationship to other visual abilities. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).


Subject(s)
Judgment , Humans , Male , Female , Adult , Young Adult , Adolescent , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Memory, Short-Term , Recognition, Psychology/physiology
8.
Neuroimage ; 296: 120668, 2024 Aug 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38848982

ABSTRACT

Our brain excels at recognizing objects, even when they flash by in a rapid sequence. However, the neural processes determining whether a target image in a rapid sequence can be recognized or not remains elusive. We used electroencephalography (EEG) to investigate the temporal dynamics of brain processes that shape perceptual outcomes in these challenging viewing conditions. Using naturalistic images and advanced multivariate pattern analysis (MVPA) techniques, we probed the brain dynamics governing conscious object recognition. Our results show that although initially similar, the processes for when an object can or cannot be recognized diverge around 180 ms post-appearance, coinciding with feedback neural processes. Decoding analyses indicate that gist perception (partial conscious perception) can occur at ∼120 ms through feedforward mechanisms. In contrast, object identification (full conscious perception of the image) is resolved at ∼190 ms after target onset, suggesting involvement of recurrent processing. These findings underscore the importance of recurrent neural connections in object recognition and awareness in rapid visual presentations.


Subject(s)
Consciousness , Electroencephalography , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Humans , Female , Male , Electroencephalography/methods , Adult , Consciousness/physiology , Young Adult , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Brain/physiology , Brain/diagnostic imaging , Recognition, Psychology/physiology , Photic Stimulation/methods
9.
Cereb Cortex ; 34(6)2024 Jun 04.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38832533

ABSTRACT

The two visual pathways model posits that visual information is processed through two distinct cortical systems: The ventral pathway promotes visual recognition, while the dorsal pathway supports visuomotor control. Recent evidence suggests the dorsal pathway is also involved in shape processing and may contribute to object perception, but it remains unclear whether this sensitivity is independent of attentional mechanisms that were localized to overlapping cortical regions. To address this question, we conducted two fMRI experiments that utilized different parametric scrambling manipulations in which human participants viewed novel objects in different levels of scrambling and were instructed to attend to either the object or to another aspect of the image (e.g. color of the background). Univariate and multivariate analyses revealed that the large-scale organization of shape selectivity along the dorsal and ventral pathways was preserved regardless of the focus of attention. Attention did modulate shape sensitivity, but these effects were similar across the two pathways. These findings support the idea that shape processing is at least partially dissociable from attentional processes and relies on a distributed set of cortical regions across the visual pathways.


Subject(s)
Attention , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Photic Stimulation , Visual Pathways , Humans , Attention/physiology , Male , Female , Visual Pathways/physiology , Visual Pathways/diagnostic imaging , Adult , Young Adult , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Photic Stimulation/methods , Brain Mapping/methods , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Form Perception/physiology , Visual Cortex/physiology , Visual Cortex/diagnostic imaging
10.
Nat Commun ; 15(1): 5477, 2024 Jun 28.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38942766

ABSTRACT

While human vision spans 220°, traditional functional MRI setups display images only up to central 10-15°. Thus, it remains unknown how the brain represents a scene perceived across the full visual field. Here, we introduce a method for ultra-wide angle display and probe signatures of immersive scene representation. An unobstructed view of 175° is achieved by bouncing the projected image off angled-mirrors onto a custom-built curved screen. To avoid perceptual distortion, scenes are created with wide field-of-view from custom virtual environments. We find that immersive scene representation drives medial cortex with far-peripheral preferences, but shows minimal modulation in classic scene regions. Further, scene and face-selective regions maintain their content preferences even with extreme far-periphery stimulation, highlighting that not all far-peripheral information is automatically integrated into scene regions computations. This work provides clarifying evidence on content vs. peripheral preferences in scene representation and opens new avenues to research immersive vision.


Subject(s)
Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Photic Stimulation , Visual Cortex , Visual Perception , Humans , Visual Cortex/physiology , Visual Cortex/diagnostic imaging , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Male , Adult , Female , Young Adult , Visual Perception/physiology , Brain Mapping/methods , Neuroimaging/methods , Visual Fields/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology
11.
J Vis ; 24(6): 7, 2024 Jun 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38848099

ABSTRACT

Which properties of a natural scene affect visual search? We consider the alternative hypotheses that low-level statistics, higher-level statistics, semantics, or layout affect search difficulty in natural scenes. Across three experiments (n = 20 each), we used four different backgrounds that preserve distinct scene properties: (a) natural scenes (all experiments); (b) 1/f noise (pink noise, which preserves only low-level statistics and was used in Experiments 1 and 2); (c) textures that preserve low-level and higher-level statistics but not semantics or layout (Experiments 2 and 3); and (d) inverted (upside-down) scenes that preserve statistics and semantics but not layout (Experiment 2). We included "split scenes" that contained different backgrounds left and right of the midline (Experiment 1, natural/noise; Experiment 3, natural/texture). Participants searched for a Gabor patch that occurred at one of six locations (all experiments). Reaction times were faster for targets on noise and slower on inverted images, compared to natural scenes and textures. The N2pc component of the event-related potential, a marker of attentional selection, had a shorter latency and a higher amplitude for targets in noise than for all other backgrounds. The background contralateral to the target had an effect similar to that on the target side: noise led to faster reactions and shorter N2pc latencies than natural scenes, although we observed no difference in N2pc amplitude. There were no interactions between the target side and the non-target side. Together, this shows that-at least when searching simple targets without own semantic content-natural scenes are more effective distractors than noise and that this results from higher-order statistics rather than from semantics or layout.


Subject(s)
Attention , Photic Stimulation , Reaction Time , Semantics , Humans , Attention/physiology , Male , Female , Young Adult , Adult , Reaction Time/physiology , Photic Stimulation/methods , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Electroencephalography/methods , Evoked Potentials, Visual/physiology
12.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 36(7): 1493-1522, 2024 Jun 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38829713

ABSTRACT

How does language background influence the neural correlates of visual word recognition in children? To address this question, we used an ERP lexical decision task to examine first-language (L1) and second-language (L2) visual word processing in monolingual and bilingual school-aged children and young adults (n = 123). In particular, we focused on the effects of word frequency (an index of lexical accessibility) on RTs and the N400 ERP component. Behaviorally, we found larger L1 versus L2 word frequency effects among bilingual children, driven by faster and more accurate responses to higher-frequency words (no other language or age group differences were observed). Neurophysiologically, we found larger L1 word frequency effects in bilinguals versus monolinguals (across both age groups), reflected in more negative ERP amplitudes to lower-frequency words. However, the bilingual groups processed L1 and L2 words similarly, despite lower levels of subjective and objective L2 proficiency. Taken together, our findings suggest that divided L1 experience (but not L2 experience) influences the neural correlates of visual word recognition across childhood and adulthood.


Subject(s)
Electroencephalography , Evoked Potentials , Multilingualism , Humans , Male , Female , Child , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Young Adult , Reaction Time/physiology , Adult , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Adolescent , Photic Stimulation , Reading , Vocabulary
13.
J Vis ; 24(6): 9, 2024 Jun 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38856981

ABSTRACT

Four experiments were conducted to gain a better understanding of the visual mechanisms related to how integration of partial shape cues provides for recognition of the full shape. In each experiment, letters formed as outline contours were displayed as a sequence of adjacent segments (fragments), each visible during a 17-ms time frame. The first experiment varied the contrast of the fragments. There were substantial individual differences in contrast sensitivity, so stimulus displays in the masking experiments that followed were calibrated to the sensitivity of each participant. Masks were displayed either as patterns that filled the entire screen (full field) or as successive strips that were sliced from the pattern, each strip lying across the location of the letter fragment that had been shown a moment before. Contrast of masks were varied to be lighter or darker than the letter fragments. Full-field masks, whether light or dark, provided relatively little impairment of recognition, as was the case for mask strips that were lighter than the letter fragments. However, dark strip masks proved to be very effective, with the degree of recognition impairment becoming larger as mask contrast was increased. A final experiment found the strip masks to be most effective when they overlapped the location where the letter fragments had been shown a moment before. They became progressively less effective with increased spatial separation from that location. Results are discussed with extensive reference to potential brain mechanisms for integrating shape cues.


Subject(s)
Contrast Sensitivity , Form Perception , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Perceptual Masking , Photic Stimulation , Humans , Perceptual Masking/physiology , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Photic Stimulation/methods , Adult , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Form Perception/physiology , Male , Female , Cues , Young Adult
14.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 50(8): 808-818, 2024 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38900526

ABSTRACT

Negative templates are based on foreknowledge of distractor features and can lead to more efficient visual search at the group level. However, large individual differences exist in the size of benefits induced by negative cues. The cognitive factors underlying these interindividual differences remain unknown. Previous research has suggested higher engagement of proactive control for negative templates compared to positive templates. We thus hypothesized that interindividual differences in proactive control efficiency may explain the large variability in negative cue benefits. A large data set made up of data from two previously published studies was reanalyzed (N = 139), with eye movements recorded in 36 participants. Individual proactive control efficiency was measured through reaction time (RT) variability. Participants with higher proactive control efficiency exhibited larger benefits after negative cues across two critical measures: Individuals with higher proactive control showed larger RT benefits following negative compared to neutral cues; similarly, individuals with higher proactive control exhibited lower first saccades to cued distractor items. No such relationship was observed for positive cues. Our results confirmed the existence of large interindividual differences in the benefits induced by negative attentional templates. Critically, we show that proactive control drives these interindividual differences in negative template use. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).


Subject(s)
Attention , Cues , Executive Function , Individuality , Humans , Attention/physiology , Adult , Male , Young Adult , Female , Executive Function/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Adolescent , Eye Movement Measurements
15.
Res Dev Disabil ; 151: 104784, 2024 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38941692

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Previous studies suggested that children with reading difficulty have impaired statistical learning ability in extracting distributional orthographic regularities. However, the neural mechanisms underlying have not been fully investigated. AIMS: The current study aimed to identify the electrophysiological markers and to examine the neural underpinnings of statistical learning of orthographic regularities in children with reading difficulties. METHODS AND PROCEDURES: Using the event-related potentials (ERPs) and the orthographic learning task, 157 children were exposed to a sequence of artificial pseudocharacters with varying levels of positional and semantic consistency (low at 60 %, moderate at 80 %, and high at 100 %). OUTCOMES AND RESULTS: Poor readers elicited an increased N170 response in the low consistency and a lack of left-lateralized P300 effect when learning positional regularities of radicals. Similarly, larger N170 effects were observed in poor readers, while similar N400 effects were found in both poor and average readers when learning semantic regularities of radicals. CONCLUSIONS AND IMPLICATIONS: Our findings indicate that poor readers may have trouble using statistical information for early-stage orthographic pattern extraction, yet they can identify semantic inconsistencies after sufficient exposure. These results deepen our understanding of the neural mechanisms involved in statistical learning for poor readers and aid in improving criteria for differentiating between typically developing children and those with reading challenges.


Subject(s)
Dyslexia , Electroencephalography , Evoked Potentials , Reading , Semantics , Humans , Child , Female , Male , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Dyslexia/physiopathology , Learning/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology
16.
eNeuro ; 11(6)2024 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38834301

ABSTRACT

How features of complex visual patterns are combined to drive perception and eye movements is not well understood. Here we simultaneously assessed human observers' perceptual direction estimates and ocular following responses (OFR) evoked by moving plaids made from two summed gratings with varying contrast ratios. When the gratings were of equal contrast, observers' eye movements and perceptual reports followed the motion of the plaid pattern. However, when the contrasts were unequal, eye movements and reports during early phases of the OFR were biased toward the direction of the high-contrast grating component; during later phases, both responses followed the plaid pattern direction. The shift from component- to pattern-driven behavior resembles the shift in tuning seen under similar conditions in neuronal responses recorded from monkey MT. Moreover, for some conditions, pattern tracking and perceptual reports were correlated on a trial-by-trial basis. The OFR may therefore provide a precise behavioral readout of the dynamics of neural motion integration for complex visual patterns.


Subject(s)
Eye Movements , Motion Perception , Photic Stimulation , Motion Perception/physiology , Humans , Eye Movements/physiology , Photic Stimulation/methods , Male , Female , Adult , Young Adult , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology
17.
Accid Anal Prev ; 205: 107637, 2024 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38852222

ABSTRACT

A two-factor experiment was devised to assess the appropriateness of the quantity and arrangement of information on multi-information guide signs at unique, spacious exits on elevated expressway sections. This experiment investigated 77 signs containing varying amounts of road name information and different placements of destination road names. The research entailed an indoor experiment that incorporated eye-tracking technology and involved the analysis of a total of twenty-eight indicators. A comprehensive index system was developed, identifying three key aspects: visual recognition efficiency, visual recognition difficulty, and visual fatigue. Utilizing repeated measure analysis of variances, the impact of these two factors was examined to identify significant indicators and establish a comprehensive assessment indicator system. The Technique for Order of Preference by Similarity to Ideal Solution method, in conjunction with the coefficient of entropy weight, was employed to assess the effectiveness of these two factors. The findings demonstrated that the 28 eye-movement indicators utilized in this study effectively constitute a valuable indicator system for evaluating drivers' visual recognition characteristics. These indicators capture the subtle psychophysical traits inherent in the process of recognizing signs, including visual recognition efficiency, difficulty, and fatigue. Regarding the first experimental factor, the number of sign road names significantly influences drivers' visual recognition characteristics (Sig < 0.05). Specifically, an increase in the number of sign road names leads to diminished visual recognition efficiency and heightened visual recognition difficulty and fatigue. Consequently, it is advisable to restrict the number of sign road names to a maximum of six per sign under typical circumstances, with nine being the limit under special conditions. As for the second experimental factor, the placement of the destination road name within the sign layout exerts a significant impact on visual recognition characteristics (Sig < 0.05). Each type of multi-information sign exhibits a distinct visual recognition pattern. Generally, the upper portion of the sign is more easily recognized, while the lower part poses greater recognition challenges. Therefore, to mitigate visual recognition risks, it is recommended that road information placement be prioritized based on actual usage conditions.


Subject(s)
Automobile Driving , Eye Movements , Eye-Tracking Technology , Humans , Male , Adult , Female , Young Adult , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Visual Perception , Location Directories and Signs
18.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 50(8): 842-858, 2024 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38900524

ABSTRACT

The visual Simon task is widely employed to explore the underlying mechanisms of sensorimotor processing in the presence of task-relevant (targets) and task-irrelevant (distracting) location information. Critically, the Simon effect is considered as an indicator of action-related interference resulting from distractor-based activation, which fades out over time. In this study, we tested whether attenuated Simon effects with slower task processing may be fully explained by the fading of distractor-based response activation. To that end, we selectively manipulated perceptual target discriminability by varying the ratio of differently colored dots within (Experiment 1) and between blocks (Experiment 2). According to pure fading activation accounts, the negative-going delta plots of the two discriminability conditions should overlap across the entire reaction time (RT) distribution. In contrast to this prediction, the negative-going DPs for the two discriminability conditions did not overlap in either experiment. Instead, the Simon effect was either consistently smaller (Experiment 1) or larger (Experiment 2) across the entire RT distribution in the easy condition compared to the hard condition. This result pattern indicates that perceptual target discriminability affected conflict resolution beyond the mere fading of distractor-based activation. Exploratory model-based analyses suggest a stronger processing of relevant perceptual information with more discriminable targets, which may counteract the influence of distracting location information. However, as the exact effects of discriminability on conflict processing seem to depend on variation mode (trialwise vs. blockwise), the importance of global strategic effects is also highlighted. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).


Subject(s)
Attention , Psychomotor Performance , Humans , Adult , Young Adult , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Male , Female , Attention/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Adolescent , Models, Psychological , Visual Perception/physiology
19.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 50(8): 875-891, 2024 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38900523

ABSTRACT

Object perception and action are closely interrelated: Various grasping components are evoked when perceiving visual objects ("object affordances"). Yet little is known about the impact of the evocation of multiobject affordances on object perceptual processing. This study aimed to determine whether object processing may be affected by the similarity of affordances evoked by multiple objects and whether semantic relations between objects modulate this effect. Adult students were presented with three-dimensional scenes involving pairs of graspable objects. Each object evoked grasp size affordances (precision or power grasps). Affordances of the two objects could be similar or dissimilar and objects could be thematically related (spatula-pan) or unrelated (spatula-snow globe). Participants had to judge the color of a target object by performing power and precision grasps compatible or incompatible with the target evoked grasp. Results showed slower responses on compatible targets when unrelated distractors evoked similar compared to dissimilar affordances. This cost of similar affordances disappeared when objects were thematically related. Findings corroborate predictions of recent models hypothesizing automatic inhibition of distractor affordances when selecting one object among others. We further provide novel evidence for a role of thematic relations between objects in the perception of multiple affordances. Findings have implications for object processing in naturalistic scenes. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).


Subject(s)
Pattern Recognition, Visual , Psychomotor Performance , Humans , Young Adult , Adult , Female , Male , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Hand Strength/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adolescent
20.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 50(8): 819-841, 2024 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38900528

ABSTRACT

Simon effects have been observed to arise from different modes of spatial information (e.g., physical location, arrow direction, and location word). The present study investigated whether different modes of spatial information elicit a unitary set of spatial codes when triggering a spatially corresponding response code. A pair of two different Simon tasks was presented in alternation: location- and arrow-based Simon tasks in Experiments 1 and 2, word- and location-based Simon tasks in Experiment 3, and arrow- and word-based Simon tasks in Experiment 4. Responses were collected using unimanual aimed-movement responses. Cross-task congruency sequence effects (CSEs) were found in Experiments 1 and 2, indicating a shared set of spatial codes between physical locations and arrow directions. Conversely, the absence of CSEs in Experiment 3 suggested that physical locations and location words elicited different sets of spatial codes. In Experiment 4, a CSE was evident in the arrow-based Simon task but not in the word-based one, implying an overlap in the spatial attributes of arrow directions with those of location words. Distributional analyses of the Simon effects revealed that different modes of spatial information yielded distinct temporal patterns of its activation and dissipation, implying quantitative differences in the Simon effects. The cross-comparisons of the CSE and delta function data indicated that the quantitative similarities in spatial modes did not correspond to the qualitative similarities, suggesting a crucial finding that each set of data reflects different aspects of the nature of the spatial codes. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).


Subject(s)
Psychomotor Performance , Space Perception , Humans , Space Perception/physiology , Young Adult , Adult , Male , Female , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Adolescent
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL