Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 32
Filter
1.
Nat Hum Behav ; 7(11): 1855-1868, 2023 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37985914

ABSTRACT

The ability of humans to create and disseminate culture is often credited as the single most important factor of our success as a species. In this Perspective, we explore the notion of 'machine culture', culture mediated or generated by machines. We argue that intelligent machines simultaneously transform the cultural evolutionary processes of variation, transmission and selection. Recommender algorithms are altering social learning dynamics. Chatbots are forming a new mode of cultural transmission, serving as cultural models. Furthermore, intelligent machines are evolving as contributors in generating cultural traits-from game strategies and visual art to scientific results. We provide a conceptual framework for studying the present and anticipated future impact of machines on cultural evolution, and present a research agenda for the study of machine culture.


Subject(s)
Cultural Evolution , Hominidae , Humans , Animals , Culture , Learning
2.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 120(44): e2313790120, 2023 Oct 31.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37883432

ABSTRACT

As the use of large language models (LLMs) grows, it is important to examine whether they exhibit biases in their output. Research in cultural evolution, using transmission chain experiments, demonstrates that humans have biases to attend to, remember, and transmit some types of content over others. Here, in five preregistered experiments using material from previous studies with human participants, we use the same, transmission chain-like methodology, and find that the LLM ChatGPT-3 shows biases analogous to humans for content that is gender-stereotype-consistent, social, negative, threat-related, and biologically counterintuitive, over other content. The presence of these biases in LLM output suggests that such content is widespread in its training data and could have consequential downstream effects, by magnifying preexisting human tendencies for cognitively appealing and not necessarily informative, or valuable, content.


Subject(s)
Cultural Evolution , Language , Humans , Mental Recall , Bias , Ethical Theory
3.
Conserv Biol ; 37(4): e14060, 2023 08.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36661052

ABSTRACT

The role of nature documentaries in shaping public attitudes and behavior toward conservation and wildlife issues is unclear. We analyzed the emotional content of over 2 million tweets related to Our Planet, a major nature documentary released on Netflix, with dictionary and rule-based automatic sentiment analysis. We also compared the sentiment associated with species mentioned in Our Planet and a set of control species with similar features but not mentioned in the documentary. Tweets were largely negative in sentiment at the time of release of the series. This effect was primarily linked to the highly skewed distributions of retweets and, in particular, to a single negatively valenced and massively retweeted tweet (>150,000 retweets). Species mentioned in Our Planet were associated with more negative sentiment than the control species, and this effect coincided with a short period following the airing of the series. Our results are consistent with a general negativity bias in cultural transmission and document the difficulty of evoking positive sentiment, on social media and elsewhere, in response to environmental problems.


Análisis de sentimientos de la respuesta en Twitter al documental Nuestro Planeta de Netflix Resumen No está claro el papel que tienen los documentales sobre naturaleza en la formación de actitudes públicas y respuestas a los temas de conservación y vida silvestre. Aplicamos un análisis automático de sentimientos basado en reglas y el diccionario al contenido emocional de más de dos millones de tuits relacionados a Nuestro Planeta, un importante documental estrenado en Netflix. También comparamos entre los sentimientos asociados a las especies mencionadas en el documental y un conjunto de especies control con características similares pero que no mencionan en el documental. En general, los tuits contenían sentimientos negativos cuando se estrenó la serie. Relacionamos este efecto a la distribución sesgada de retuits particularmente de un solo tuit negativo con retuits masivos (>150,000). Las especies mencionadas estuvieron asociadas con más sentimientos negativos que las especies control. Este efecto coincidió con un periodo corto después de la emisión de la serie. Nuestros resultados son coherentes con un sesgo generalizado de negatividad en la transmisión cultural y documentan lo difícil que es provocar sentimientos positivos, en redes sociales o en demás sitios, como respuesta a los problemas ambientales.


Subject(s)
Social Media , Humans , Planets , Sentiment Analysis , Conservation of Natural Resources , Attitude
4.
Sci Rep ; 12(1): 21680, 2022 12 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36522390

ABSTRACT

The method of exclusion identifies patterns of distributions of behaviours and/or artefact forms among different groups, where these patterns are deemed unlikely to arise from purely genetic and/or ecological factors. The presence of such patterns is often used to establish whether a species is cultural or not-i.e. whether a species uses social learning or not. Researchers using or describing this method have often pointed out that the method cannot pinpoint which specific type(s) of social learning resulted in the observed patterns. However, the literature continues to contain such inferences. In a new attempt to warn against these logically unwarranted conclusions, we illustrate this error using a novel approach. We use an individual-based model, focused on wild ape cultural patterns-as these patterns are the best-known cases of animal culture and as they also contain the most frequent usage of the unwarranted inference for specific social learning mechanisms. We built a model that contained agents unable to copy specifics of behavioural or artefact forms beyond their individual reach (which we define as "copying"). We did so, as some of the previous inference claims related to social learning mechanisms revolve around copying defined in this way. The results of our model however show that non-copying social learning can already reproduce the defining-even iconic-features of observed ape cultural patterns detected by the method of exclusion. This shows, using a novel model approach, that copying processes are not necessary to produce the cultural patterns that are sometimes still used in an attempt to identify copying processes. Additionally, our model could fully control for both environmental and genetic factors (impossible in real life) and thus offers a new validity check for the method of exclusion as related to general cultural claims-a check that the method passed. Our model also led to new and additional findings, which we likewise discuss.


Subject(s)
Cultural Evolution , Hominidae , Social Learning , Animals , Social Behavior , Databases, Genetic
5.
Hum Nat ; 33(2): 132-144, 2022 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35488999

ABSTRACT

Cultural evolution researchers use transmission chain experiments to investigate which content is more likely to survive when transmitted from one individual to another. These experiments resemble oral storytelling, wherein individuals need to understand, memorize, and reproduce the content. However, prominent contemporary forms of cultural transmission-think an online sharing-only involve the willingness to transmit the content. Here I present two fully preregistered online experiments that explicitly investigated the differences between these two modalities of transmission. The first experiment (N = 1,080 participants) examined whether negative content, information eliciting disgust, and threat-related information were better transmitted than their neutral counterpart in a traditional transmission chain setup. The second experiment (N = 1,200 participants) used the same material, but participants were asked whether or not they would share the content in two conditions: in a large anonymous social network or with their friends, in their favorite social network. Negative content was both better transmitted in transmission chain experiments and shared more than its neutral counterpart. Threat-related information was successful in transmission chain experiments but not when sharing, and finally, information eliciting disgust was not advantaged in either. Overall, the results present a composite picture, suggesting that the interactions between the specific content and the medium of transmission are important and, possibly, that content biases are stronger when memorization and reproduction are involved in the transmission-as in oral transmission-than when they are not-as in online sharing. Negative content seems to be reliably favored in both modalities of transmission.


Subject(s)
Cultural Evolution , Social Media , Bias , Communication , Humans
6.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 376(1828): 20200050, 2021 07 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33993759

ABSTRACT

Cultural evolution requires the social transmission of information. For this reason, scholars have emphasized social learning when explaining how and why culture evolves. Yet cultural evolution results from many mechanisms operating in concert. Here, we argue that the emphasis on social learning has distracted scholars from appreciating both the full range of mechanisms contributing to cultural evolution and how interactions among those mechanisms and other factors affect the output of cultural evolution. We examine understudied mechanisms and other factors and call for a more inclusive programme of investigation that probes multiple levels of the organization, spanning the neural, cognitive-behavioural and populational levels. To guide our discussion, we focus on factors involved in three core topics of cultural evolution: the emergence of culture, the emergence of cumulative cultural evolution and the design of cultural traits. Studying mechanisms across levels can add explanatory power while revealing gaps and misconceptions in our knowledge. This article is part of the theme issue 'Foundations of cultural evolution'.


Subject(s)
Cultural Evolution , Social Learning , Humans
7.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 376(1828): 20200052, 2021 07 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33993762

ABSTRACT

Social information is immensely valuable. Yet we waste it. The information we get from observing other humans and from communicating with them is a cheap and reliable informational resource. It is considered the backbone of human cultural evolution. Theories and models focused on the evolution of social learning show the great adaptive benefits of evolving cognitive tools to process it. In spite of this, human adults in the experimental literature use social information quite inefficiently: they do not take it sufficiently into account. A comprehensive review of the literature on five experimental tasks documented 45 studies showing social information waste, and four studies showing social information being over-used. These studies cover 'egocentric discounting' phenomena as studied by social psychology, but also include experimental social learning studies. Social information waste means that human adults fail to give social information its optimal weight. Both proximal explanations and accounts derived from evolutionary theory leave crucial aspects of the phenomenon unaccounted for: egocentric discounting is a pervasive effect that no single unifying explanation fully captures. Cultural evolutionary theory's insistence on the power and benefits of social influence is to be balanced against this phenomenon. This article is part of the theme issue 'Foundations of cultural evolution'.


Subject(s)
Cultural Evolution , Information Dissemination , Social Learning , Humans , Psychology, Social
8.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 376(1828): 20200045, 2021 07 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33993768

ABSTRACT

A specific goal of the field of cultural evolution is to understand how processes of transmission and selection at the individual level lead to population-wide patterns of cultural diversity and change. Models of cultural evolution have typically assumed that traits are independent of one another and essentially exchangeable. But culture has a structure: traits bear relationships to one another that affect the transmission and selection process itself. Here, we introduce a modelling framework to explore the effect of interdependencies on the process of learning. Through simulations, we find that introducing a simple structure changes the cultural dynamics. Based on a basic filtering mechanism for parsing trait relationships, more elaborate cultural filters emerge. In a mostly incompatible cultural domain of traits, these filters organize culture into mostly (but not fully) consistent and stable systems. Incompatible domains produce small homogeneous cultures, while more compatibility increases size, diversity and group divergence. When individuals copy based on a trait's features (here, its compatibility relationships), they produce more homogeneous cultures than when they copy based on the agent carrying the cultural trait. We discuss the implications of considering cultural systems and filters in the dynamics of cultural change. This article is part of the theme issue 'Foundations of cultural evolution'.


Subject(s)
Cultural Diversity , Cultural Evolution , Learning , Humans , Models, Psychological
9.
Evol Hum Sci ; 3: e50, 2021.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37588566

ABSTRACT

Typical examples of cultural phenomena all exhibit a degree of similarity across time and space at the level of the population. As such, a fundamental question for any science of culture is, what ensures this stability in the first place? Here we focus on the evolutionary and stabilising role of 'convergent transformation', in which one item causes the production of another item whose form tends to deviate from the original in a directed, non-random way. We present a series of stochastic models of cultural evolution investigating its effects. The results show that cultural stability can emerge and be maintained by virtue of convergent transformation alone, in the absence of any form of copying or selection process. We show how high-fidelity copying and convergent transformation need not be opposing forces, and can jointly contribute to cultural stability. We finally analyse how non-random transformation and high-fidelity copying can have different evolutionary signatures at population level, and hence how their distinct effects can be distinguished in empirical records. Collectively, these results supplement existing approaches to cultural evolution based on the Darwinian analogy, while also providing formal support for other frameworks - such as Cultural Attraction Theory - that entail its further loosening. Social media summary: Culture can be produced and maintained by convergent transformation, without copying or selection involved.

10.
Sci Rep ; 9(1): 6241, 2019 Apr 17.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30996230

ABSTRACT

A correction to this article has been published and is linked from the HTML and PDF versions of this paper. The error has not been fixed in the paper.

11.
PLoS One ; 14(1): e0210748, 2019.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30682728

ABSTRACT

Cultural evolution is the product of the psychological mechanisms that underlie individual decision making. One commonly studied learning mechanism is a disproportionate preference for majority opinions, known as conformist transmission. While most theoretical and experimental work approaches the majority in terms of the number of individuals that perform a behaviour or hold a belief, some recent experimental studies approach the majority in terms of the number of instances a behaviour is performed. Here, we use a mathematical model to show that disagreement between these two notions of the majority can arise when behavioural variants are performed at different rates, with different salience or in different contexts (variant overrepresentation) and when a subset of the population act as demonstrators to the whole population (model biases). We also show that because conformist transmission changes the distribution of behaviours in a population, how observers approach the majority can cause populations to diverge, and that this can happen even when the two approaches to the majority agree with regards to which behaviour is in the majority. We discuss these results in light of existing findings, ranging from political extremism on twitter to studies of animal foraging behaviour. We conclude that the factors we considered (variant overrepresentation and model biases) are plausibly widespread. As such, it is important to understand how individuals approach the majority in order to understand the effects of majority influence in cultural evolution.


Subject(s)
Cultural Evolution , Decision Making , Social Conformity , Animals , Behavior, Animal , Humans , Learning , Mathematical Concepts , Models, Psychological , Observer Variation
12.
Evol Hum Sci ; 1: e11, 2019.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37588398

ABSTRACT

Popular music offers a rich source of data that provides insights into long-term cultural evolutionary dynamics. One major trend in popular music, as well as other cultural products such as literary fiction, is an increase over time in negatively valenced emotional content, and a decrease in positively valenced emotional content. Here we use two large datasets containing lyrics from n = 4913 and n = 159,015 pop songs respectively and spanning 1965-2015, to test whether cultural transmission biases derived from the cultural evolution literature can explain this trend towards emotional negativity. We find some evidence of content bias (negative lyrics do better in the charts), prestige bias (best-selling artists are copied) and success bias (best-selling songs are copied) in the proliferation of negative lyrics. However, the effects of prestige and success bias largely disappear when unbiased transmission is included in the models, which assumes that the occurrence of negative lyrics is predicted by their past frequency. We conclude that the proliferation of negative song lyrics may be explained partly by content bias, and partly by undirected, unbiased cultural transmission.

13.
Cartilage ; 10(3): 314-320, 2019 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29629574

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: The International Cartilage Repair Society classification is the one mainly used to define chondral defects. However, this classification does not include delamination. The objective of the study is to describe the characteristics of this lesion to better explain its classification in the context of chondral lesions of the hip. DESIGN: We performed a retrospective analysis of 613 patients who underwent hip arthroscopy. In this group, the incidence, localization, histological characteristics, and association to femoroacetabular impingement as well as to other intraarticular lesions of acetabular delamination (AD) were analyzed. Preoperative magnetic resonance imaging accuracy and the different treatment options were also evaluated. RESULTS: In our series, the incidence of the AD was 37% (226 patients over 613). The average age of this group was significantly lower (39.3 years) than the entire group of patients. Isolated cam (P < 0.01) and pincer morphologies (P < 0.05) had a significant statistical association with the AD. This lesion was primarily localized at the acetabular chondrolabral junction, mainly on the anterosuperior quadrant. The intraarticular lesions more frequently associated to AD were labral lesions (94.25%, P < 0.01), ligamentum teres lesions (28.32%, P < 0.05), and femoral head chondral lesions (19.9%, P < 0.01). The histological examination of the AD was characterized by hypocellularity and structural disorder of the matrix, with fissures. Treatment remains controversial. CONCLUSION: AD represents an intermediate stage in chondral damage and can be classified as a "2a" grade lesion. Histological examination confirms the intermediate and progressive character of this injury.


Subject(s)
Acetabulum/pathology , Arthroscopy/methods , Cartilage, Articular/pathology , Femoracetabular Impingement/pathology , Hip Joint/pathology , Acetabulum/transplantation , Adipose Tissue/transplantation , Adult , Bone Matrix/pathology , Cartilage Diseases/classification , Cartilage Diseases/diagnostic imaging , Cartilage Diseases/epidemiology , Cartilage Diseases/pathology , Cartilage, Articular/abnormalities , Cartilage, Articular/transplantation , Chondrocytes/transplantation , Femoracetabular Impingement/epidemiology , Femoracetabular Impingement/surgery , Femur Head/pathology , Fractures, Stress , Hip Joint/diagnostic imaging , Hip Joint/surgery , Humans , Incidence , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Middle Aged , Preoperative Period , Retrospective Studies , Round Ligaments/pathology
14.
Sci Rep ; 8(1): 14016, 2018 09 18.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30228345

ABSTRACT

In the Smaldino et al. study 'Sigmoidal Acquisition Curves are Good Indicators of Conformist Transmission', our original findings regarding the conditional validity of using population-level sigmoidal acquisition curves as means to evidence individual-level conformity are contested. We acknowledge the identification of useful nuances, yet conclude that our original findings remain relevant for the study of conformist learning mechanisms.Replying to: Smaldino, P. E., Aplin, L. M. & Farine, D. R. Sigmoidal Acquisition Curves Are Good Indicators of Conformist Transmission. Sci. Rep. 8, https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-30248-5 (2018).


Subject(s)
Learning , Social Behavior
15.
Behav Brain Sci ; 41: e159, 2018 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31064493

ABSTRACT

Folk-economic beliefs may be regarded as "evidential fictions" that exploit the natural tendency of human cognition to organize itself in narrative form. Narrative counter-arguments are likely more effective than logical debunking. The challenge is to convey sound economic reasoning in narratively conspicuous forms - an opportunity for economics to rethink its role and agency in public discourse, in the spirit of its old classics.


Subject(s)
Cognition , Logic , Biological Evolution , Humans , Problem Solving
16.
Cogn Emot ; 31(8): 1663-1675, 2017 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27910735

ABSTRACT

The presence of emotional words and content in stories has been shown to enhance a story's memorability, and its cultural success. Yet, recent cultural trends run in the opposite direction. Using the Google Books corpus, coupled with two metadata-rich corpora of Anglophone fiction books, we show a decrease in emotionality in English-speaking literature starting plausibly in the nineteenth century. We show that this decrease cannot be explained by changes unrelated to emotionality (such as demographic dynamics concerning age or gender balance, changes in vocabulary richness, or changes in the prevalence of literary genres), and that, in our three corpora, the decrease is driven almost entirely by a decline in the proportion of positive emotion-related words, while the frequency of negative emotion-related words shows little if any decline. Consistently with previous studies, we also find a link between ageing and negative emotionality at the individual level.


Subject(s)
Emotions , Literature, Modern/history , Age Factors , Cultural Evolution , Databases, Factual/statistics & numerical data , History, 18th Century , History, 19th Century , History, 20th Century , History, 21st Century , Humans
17.
Front Hum Neurosci ; 10: 636, 2016.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28018200

ABSTRACT

Digital media have today an enormous diffusion, and their influence on the behavior of a vast part of the human population can hardly be underestimated. In this review I propose that cultural evolution theory, including both a sophisticated view of human behavior and a methodological attitude to modeling and quantitative analysis, provides a useful framework to study the effects and the developments of media in the digital age. I will first give a general presentation of the cultural evolution framework, and I will then introduce this more specific research program with two illustrative topics. The first topic concerns how cultural transmission biases, that is, simple heuristics such as "copy prestigious individuals" or "copy the majority," operate in the novel context of digital media. The existence of transmission biases is generally justified with their adaptivity in small-scale societies. How do they operate in an environment where, for example, prestigious individuals possess not-relevant skills, or popularity is explicitly quantified and advertised? The second aspect relates to fidelity of cultural transmission. Digitally-mediated interactions support cheap and immediate high-fidelity transmission, in opposition, for example, to oral traditions. How does this change the content that is more likely to spread? Overall, I suggest the usefulness of a "long view" to our contemporary digital environment, contextualized in cognitive science and cultural evolution theory, and I discuss how this perspective could help us to understand what is genuinely new and what is not.

18.
Sci Rep ; 6: 36068, 2016 10 31.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27796373

ABSTRACT

Conformist transmission, defined as a disproportionate likelihood to copy the majority, is considered a potent mechanism underlying the emergence and stabilization of cultural diversity. However, ambiguity within and across disciplines remains as to how to identify conformist transmission empirically. In most studies, a population level outcome has been taken as the benchmark to evidence conformist transmission: a sigmoidal relation between individuals' probability to copy the majority and the proportional majority size. Using an individual-based model, we show that, under ecologically plausible conditions, this sigmoidal relation can also be detected without equipping individuals with a conformist bias. Situations in which individuals copy randomly from a fixed subset of demonstrators in the population, or in which they have a preference for one of the possible variants, yield similar sigmoidal patterns as a conformist bias would. Our findings warrant a revisiting of studies that base their conformist transmission conclusions solely on the sigmoidal curve. More generally, our results indicate that population level outcomes interpreted as conformist transmission could potentially be explained by other individual-level strategies, and that more empirical support is needed to prove the existence of an individual-level conformist bias in human and other animals.


Subject(s)
Social Conformity , Humans , Knowledge , Learning
19.
R Soc Open Sci ; 3(9): 160215, 2016 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27703687

ABSTRACT

The extensive use of social learning is considered a major reason for the ecological success of humans. Theoretical considerations, models and experiments have explored the evolutionary basis of social learning, showing the conditions under which learning from others is more adaptive than individual learning. Here we present an extension of a previous experimental set-up, in which individuals go on simulated 'hunts' and their success depends on the features of a 'virtual arrowhead' they design. Individuals can modify their arrowhead either by individual trial and error or by copying others. We study how, in a multimodal adaptive landscape, the smoothness of the peaks influences learning. We compare narrow peaks, in which solutions close to optima do not provide useful feedback to individuals, to wide peaks, where smooth landscapes allow an effective hill-climbing individual learning strategy. We show that individual learning is more difficult in narrow-peaked landscapes, but that social learners perform almost equally well in both narrow- and wide-peaked search spaces. There was a weak trend for more copying in the narrow than wide condition, although as in previous experiments social information was generally underutilized. Our results highlight the importance of tasks' design space when studying the adaptiveness of high-fidelity social learning.

20.
Hip Int ; 26 Suppl 1: 17-22, 2016 May 14.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27174061

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Hip arthroscopy has allowed the diagnosis and treatment of chondral injuries. METHODS: We retrospectively analysed the intraoperative data of 359 patients treated with hip arthroscopy from January 2012 to December 2013. We estimated the frequency, location and extension of acetabular cartilage (AC) injuries and their correlation to femoroacetabular impingement (FAI). RESULTS: Grade 1 and 2 acetabular chondral lesions were not statistically significant in incidence, location and extension. Chondral lesions were absent on the acetabulum in 3.9% of cases, significantly lower compared to the femoral head (32.0%). 101 (28.1%) were affected by an acetabular chondral lesion. In 244 (68.0%) the chondral lesion was located on both acetabulum and femoral head. On the acetabulum, peripheral superior and superior-posterior area were frequently involved. Grade 4 acetabular chondral defects showed a significant high location on the superior-anterior, superior and superior-posterior area, involving both the peripheral and central surface. Delamination was present in 113 (31.5%) patients. Patients affected by acetabular delamination showed a reduced injury extension if compared to grade 3 or 4. 81 patients (25%) revealed acetabular chondral lesions grade 2a, 3 and 4 without any radiological, clinical or arthroscopic sign of FAI. CONCLUSIONS: Location, extension and degree of hip chondral lesions show a progression toward a progressive degeneration. The high percentage of chondral delamination in the hip must be taken in consideration. The presence of symptomatic chondral lesion in cases with no radiological evidence of FAI, suggests that their aetiopathogenesis could be related to biomechanical alterations.


Subject(s)
Arthroscopy/methods , Cartilage, Articular/injuries , Cartilage, Articular/pathology , Femoracetabular Impingement/diagnostic imaging , Hip Joint/physiopathology , Adolescent , Adult , Arthralgia/etiology , Arthralgia/physiopathology , Cohort Studies , Female , Femoracetabular Impingement/epidemiology , Femoracetabular Impingement/surgery , Humans , Incidence , Male , Middle Aged , Pain Measurement , Prognosis , Retrospective Studies , Risk Assessment , Severity of Illness Index , Treatment Outcome , Young Adult
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...