Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 9 de 9
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
Front Psychiatry ; 13: 937516, 2022.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36620686

ABSTRACT

Background: Currently, there are no official statistics about the number of children with developmental disorders in Bulgaria. This is the first systematic investigation of the needs, access to services, and priorities of families of children with developmental disorders in the country. Aims: The study aims to: (1) characterize the needs of children with developmental disorders in Bulgaria; (2) to compare the needs and access to services of children with Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) and other neurodevelopmental disorders (oNDD); (3) and to examine the daily burden of their caregivers and how it varies based on their demographic characteristics, such as income and education. Methods: We used an online family needs assessment survey to collect data from caregivers of children with developmental disorders in Bulgaria between April and July 2020. 195 parents of children with ASD and 73 parents of children with oNDD completed the questionnaire. Results: Children with ASD waited longer than children with oNDD to receive a diagnosis. Caregivers in the ASD group also expressed first concerns about their child's development when their children were older and for different reasons than caregivers in the oNDD group. There were no significant differences between groups in service encounters, including access to and delay of medical, counseling, and educational services, with approximately 50% of all caregivers experiencing some delay and/or difficulties in access to services. There were no associations between access to services and caregiver education and family income, with the exception of higher education being linked to receiving a diagnosis earlier for the oNDD group. Discussion: This study has three main findings: (1) children with ASD and children with oNDD in Bulgaria have different needs and paths to diagnosis; (2) nevertheless, children in both groups experience similar challenges in accessing medical, counseling, and educational services, regardless of their demographic characteristics; and (3) parents' priorities focus on education, counseling, and medical support, protecting children's basic rights, and raising awareness. A comparison of our findings to past research in the region shows a relative improvement in diagnostic services with families not having to travel outside their city to receive a diagnosis. Based on our findings, we provide specific recommendations for changes in services and policy.

2.
Front Psychol ; 9: 1287, 2018.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30150947

ABSTRACT

To what extent is the choice of what to say driven by seemingly irrelevant cues in the visual world being described? Among such cues, how does prior description affect how we process spatial scenes? When people describe where objects are located their use of spatial language is often associated with a choice of reference frame. Two experiments employing between-participants designs (N = 490) examined the effects of visual cueing and previous description on reference frame choice as reflected in spatial prepositions (in front of, to the left of, etc.) to describe pictures of object pairs. Experiment 1 examined the effects of visual and linguistic cues on spatial description choice through movement of object(s) in spatial scenes, showing sizeable effects of visual cueing on reference frame choice. Experiment 2 monitored eye movements of participants following a linguistic example description, revealing two findings: eye movement "signatures" associated with distinct reference frames as expressed in language, and transfer of these eye movement patterns just prior to spatial description for different (later) picture descriptions. Both verbal description and visual cueing similarly influence language production choice through manipulation of visual attention, suggesting a unified theory of constraints affecting spatial language choice.

3.
Lang Speech ; 60(2): 318-329, 2017 06.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28697700

ABSTRACT

This paper addresses the impact of dialogue strategies and functional features of spatial arrangements on communicative success. To examine the sharing of cognition between two minds in order to achieve a joint goal, we collected a corpus of 24 extended German-language dialogues in a referential communication task that involved furnishing a dolls' house. Results show how successful communication, as evidenced by correct placement of furniture items, is affected by: (a) functionality of the furniture arrangement; (b) previous task experience; and (c) dialogue features such as description length and orientation information. To enhance research in this area, our 'Dolldialogue' corpus ( www.dolldialogue.space ) is now available as a free resource.


Subject(s)
Communication , Linguistics , Speech Acoustics , Voice Quality , Adolescent , Adult , Cognition , Comprehension , Female , Humans , Male , Young Adult
4.
Cogn Process ; 13 Suppl 1: S79-82, 2012 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22915258

ABSTRACT

Head movement is commonly used to communicate positive versus negative response. However, whereas in US culture, vertical head movement denotes positivity (nodding to say "yes") and horizontal head movement is associated with negativity (shaking heads to say "no"), in Bulgaria, this response pattern is reversed, that is, horizontal head movement means "yes" and vertical head movement means "no." Thus, these two cultures spatially "embody" agreement via different movement directions. We examined the effect of such cultural differences on cognitive processing that has no communicative intent by comparing ratings of likeability, brightness, and positive feeling associated with different color moving dots. Participants followed the dots' movement with their heads in a 2 (direction: vertical vs. horizontal) by 2 (speed: fast vs. slow) design. We found a two-way country by speed of movement interaction such that Bulgarian participants associated colors with more positive feeling when those were perceived in combination with slower head movement irrespective of movement direction. US participants, on the other hand, rated color dots as better mood-enhancing in combination with faster head movement. There was a similar two-way country by movement speed interaction for likeability ratings but none for brightness. Findings are discussed in terms of variability in gestural meaning and culture-specific embodiment patterns.


Subject(s)
Cognition/physiology , Cross-Cultural Comparison , Expressed Emotion/physiology , Head Movements/physiology , Analysis of Variance , Bulgaria , Color , Female , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation , United States
5.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 17(4): 575-80, 2010 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20702880

ABSTRACT

In the present study, we examined how context of instruction and information in the visual array to be described affect spatial information packaging across a range of levels of spatial description. Participants described complex scenes containing 3-D dollhouse furniture across two different arrays (functional vs. nonfunctional arrangements of objects) and across instructional contexts (living room context, furniture showroom context, no context). Knowledge about the visual scene and instructional context both had an impact on spatial descriptions, but separately, and at different levels of granularity. The influence of visual context was particularly striking, with marked differences across conditions at multiple levels of information packaging-descriptive trajectories (the order in which objects in the spatial array were described), amount of detail, and explicit mention of atypical object orientation. The importance of visual context as a means of accessing context frames in common ground is discussed.


Subject(s)
Attention , Knowledge of Results, Psychological , Orientation , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Semantics , Social Environment , Space Perception , Adolescent , Adult , Decision Making , Discrimination Learning , Female , Humans , Interior Design and Furnishings , Judgment , Male , Middle Aged , Psychomotor Performance , Young Adult
6.
Percept Psychophys ; 66(3): 496-507, 2004 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15283073

ABSTRACT

Two procedures were used to explore the effects of semantic and grammatical gender on the recognition and processing of Bulgarian nouns, in relation to other factors that are known to affect lexical access. This study in a three-gender language was modeled on previous work in Italian, a two-gender language (Bates, Devescovi, Pizzamiglio, D'Amico, & Hernandez, 1995). Words were presented auditorily in randomized lists in two tasks: (1) repeat the word as quickly as possible and (2) determine the grammatical gender of the noun as soon as possible and indicate the decision by pressing a button. Reaction times in both tasks were influenced by phonological factors, word frequency, and irregularity of gender marking, but semantic and grammatical gender affected only gender monitoring. The significant contribution of semantic gender to processing in Bulgarian contrasts with previous findings for Italian. Also, we obtained an interaction between sex of the subject and noun gender, reflecting a bias toward one's own grammatical gender "counterpart" (especially for females). Reanalysis of the prior study in Italian showed a similar interaction but confirmed no effects of the semantic gender of the noun, suggesting that these two natural gender effects can dissociate. Possible reasons for cross-linguistic differences are discussed, with implications for comparative studies of gender and lexical access.


Subject(s)
Language , Recognition, Psychology , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Humans , Linguistics , Male , Random Allocation , Semantics , Sex Factors
7.
J Mem Lang ; 51(2): 247-250, 2004 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23002322

ABSTRACT

Picture naming is a widely used technique in psycholinguistic studies. Here, we describe new on-line resources that our project has compiled and made available to researchers on the world wide web at http://crl.ucsd.edu/~aszekely/ipnp/. The website provides access to a wide range of picture stimuli and related norms in seven languages. Picture naming norms, including indices of name agreement and latency, for 520 black-and-white drawings of common objects and 275 concrete transitive and intransitive actions are presented. Norms for age-of-acquisition, word-frequency, familiarity, goodness-of-depiction, and visual complexity are included. An on-line database query system can be used to select a specific range of stimuli, based on parameters of interest for a wide range of studies on healthy and clinical populations, as well as studies of language development.

8.
Nonlinear Dynamics Psychol Life Sci ; 7(4): 315-28, 2003 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14523267

ABSTRACT

This study was undertaken to verify whether different output variables or biosignals, measured during performance of a cognitive task, manifest common dynamical properties. Nonlinear properties of both response times (RTs) and electroercephalograms (EEG) were tested. We asked subjects to generate mental images of actions following of auditorily presentation simple phrases suggesting the action. Analysis of RT series combined from many subjects and of EEG records from single subjects clearly manifested self-similarity and chaotic dynamics that provide insights into the self-organization of the brain/behavioral system.


Subject(s)
Cognition/physiology , Electroencephalography , Models, Theoretical , Adult , Auditory Perception , Female , Humans , Imagination , Male
9.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 10(2): 344-80, 2003 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12921412

ABSTRACT

Timed picture naming was compared in seven languages that vary along dimensions known to affect lexical access. Analyses over items focused on factors that determine cross-language universals and cross-language disparities. With regard to universals, number of alternative names had large effects on reaction time within and across languages after target-name agreement was controlled, suggesting inhibitory effects from lexical competitors. For all the languages, word frequency and goodness of depiction had large effects, but objective picture complexity did not. Effects of word structure variables (length, syllable structure, compounding, and initial frication) varied markedly over languages. Strong cross-language correlations were found in naming latencies, frequency, and length. Other-language frequency effects were observed (e.g., Chinese frequencies predicting Spanish reaction times) even after within-language effects were controlled (e.g., Spanish frequencies predicting Spanish reaction times). These surprising cross-language correlations challenge widely held assumptions about the lexical locus of length and frequency effects, suggesting instead that they may (at least in part) reflect familiarity and accessibility at a conceptual level that is shared over languages.


Subject(s)
Linguistics , Visual Perception , Cross-Cultural Comparison , Humans , Language , Phonetics , Reaction Time , Vocabulary
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...