Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 31
Filter
1.
J Prev Alzheimers Dis ; 10(4): 875-885, 2023.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37874110

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND/OBJECTIVES: CAN-THUMBS UP is designed as a comprehensive and innovative fully remote program to 1) develop an interactive and compelling online Brain Health Support Program intervention, with potential to positively influence dementia literacy, self-efficacy and lifestyle risk factors; 2) enroll and retain a community-dwelling Platform Trial Cohort of individuals at risk of dementia who will participate in the intervention; 3) support an open platform trial to test a variety of multidomain interventions that might further benefit individuals at risk of dementia. This manuscript presents the Brain Health Support Program Study protocol. DESIGN/SETTING: Twelve-month prospective multi-center longitudinal study to evaluate a fully remote web-based educational intervention. Participants will subsequently be part of a Platform Trial Cohort and may be eligible to participate in further dementia prevention clinical trials. PARTICIPANTS: Three hundred fifty older adults who are cognitively unimpaired or have mild cognitive impairment, with at least 1 well established dementia risk factor. INTERVENTION: Participants engage in the Brain Health Support Program intervention for 45-weeks and complete pre/post intervention measures. This intervention is designed to convey best available evidence for dementia prevention, consists of 181 chapters within 8 modules that are progressively delivered, and is available online in English and French. The program has been developed as a collaborative effort by investigators with recognized expertise in the program's content areas, along with input from older-adult citizen advisors. MEASUREMENTS: This study utilizes adapted remote assessments with accessible technologies (e.g. videoconferencing, cognitive testing via computer and mobile phone, wearable devices to track physical activity and sleep, self-administered saliva sample collection). The primary outcome is change in dementia literacy, as measured by the Alzheimer's Disease Knowledge Scale. Secondary outcomes include change in self-efficacy; engagement using the online program; user satisfaction ratings; and evaluation of usability and acceptance. Exploratory outcomes include changes in attitudes toward dementia, modifiable risk factors, performance on the Neuropsychological Test Battery, performance on self-administered online cognitive assessments, and levels of physical activity and sleep; success of the national recruitment plan; and the distribution of age adjusted polygenic hazard scores. CONCLUSIONS: This fully remote study provides an accessible approach to research with all study activities being completed in the participants' home environment. This approach may reduce barriers to participation, provide an easier and less demanding participant experience, and reach a broader geography with recruitment from all regions of Canada. CAN-THUMBS UP represents a Canadian contribution to the global World-Wide FINGERS program (alz.org/wwfingers).


Subject(s)
Alzheimer Disease , Brain , Aged , Humans , Canada , Longitudinal Studies , Prospective Studies
2.
BMC Geriatr ; 20(1): 315, 2020 08 28.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32859156

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: In older adults, multidomain training that includes physical and cognitive activities has been associated with improvement of physical and cognitive health. The goal of the multisite StayFitLonger study is to assess a home-based computerised training programme, which combines physical exercises, stimulating cognitive activities and virtual coaching. METHODS: One hundred twenty-eight cognitively healthy older adults will be recruited from the community in Switzerland, Canada and Belgium. The study will comprise (1) a 26-week double-blind randomized controlled efficacy trial and (2) a 22-week pragmatic adherence sub-study. In the efficacy trial, participants will be randomly assigned to an experimental or an active control intervention. In the experimental intervention, participants will use the StayFitLonger programme, which is computerised on a tablet and provides content that combines physical activities with a focus on strength and balance, as well as divided attention, problem solving and memory training. Outcomes will be measured before and after 26 weeks of training. The primary efficacy outcome will be performance on the "Timed-Up & Go" test. Secondary outcomes will include measures of frailty, cognition, mood, fear of falling, quality of life, and activities of daily living. Age, sex, education, baseline cognition, expectation, and adherence will be used as moderators of efficacy. Following the 26-week efficacy trial, all participants will use the experimental programme meaning that participants in the control group will 'cross over' to receive the StayFitLonger programme for 22 weeks. Adherence will be measured in both groups based on dose, volume and frequency of use. In addition, participants' perception of the programme and its functionalities will be characterised through usability, acceptability and user experience. DISCUSSION: This study will determine the efficacy, adherence and participants' perception of a home-based multidomain intervention programme and its functionalities. This will allow for further development and possible commercialization of a scientifically validated training programme. TRIAL REGISTRATION: ClinicalTrials.gov , NCT04237519 Registered on January 22, 2020 - Retrospectively registered.


Subject(s)
Accidental Falls , Activities of Daily Living , Aged , Belgium , Canada , Exercise Therapy , Fear , Humans , Quality of Life , Switzerland
3.
Trials ; 20(1): 282, 2019 May 22.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31118095

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Leisure activities can be both enjoyable and cognitively stimulating, and participation in such activities has been associated with reduced age-related cognitive decline. Thus, integrating stimulating leisure activities in cognitive training programs may represent a powerful and innovative approach to promote cognition in older adults at risk of dementia. The ENGAGE study is a randomized controlled, double-blind preference trial with a comprehensive cohort design that will test the efficacy and long-term impact of an intervention that combines cognitive training and cognitively stimulating leisure activities. METHODS: One hundred and forty-four older adults with a memory complaint will be recruited in Montreal and Toronto. A particular effort will be made to reach persons with low cognitive reserve. Participants will be randomly assigned to one of two conditions: cognitive + leisure training (ENGAGE-MUSIC/SPANISH) or active control (ENGAGE-DISCOVERY). The ENGAGE-MUSIC/SPANISH training will include teaching of mnemonic and attentional control strategies, casual videogames selected to train attention, and classes in music or Spanish as a second language. The ENGAGE-DISCOVERY condition will comprise psychoeducation on cognition and the brain, low-stimulating casual videogames and documentary viewing with discussions. To retain the leisure aspect of the activities, participants will be allowed to exclude either music or Spanish at study entry if they strongly dislike one of these activities. Participants randomized to ENGAGE-MUSIC/SPANISH who did not exclude any activity will be assigned to music or Spanish based on a second random assignment. Training will be provided in 24 2-h sessions over 4 months. Outcomes will be measured at baseline, at 4-month follow-up, and at 24-month follow-up. The primary outcome will be cognitive performance on a composite measure of episodic memory (delayed recall scores for words and face-name associations) measured at baseline and at the 4-month follow-up. Secondary outcomes will include a composite measure of attention (speed of processing, inhibition, dual tasking, and shifting), psychological health, activities of daily living, and brain structure and function and long-term maintenance measured at the 24-month follow-up. Information on cognitive reserve proxies (education and lifestyle questionnaires), sex and genotype (apolipoprotein (Apo)E4, brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF), and catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT)) will be collected and considered as moderators of training efficacy. DISCUSSION: This study will test whether a program combining cognitive training with stimulating leisure activities can increase cognition and reduce cognitive decline in persons at risk of dementia. TRIAL REGISTRATION: NCT03271190 . Registered on 5 September 2017.


Subject(s)
Cognition , Leisure Activities , Memory Disorders/therapy , Randomized Controlled Trials as Topic , Activities of Daily Living , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Attention , Brain/pathology , Brain/physiopathology , Cohort Studies , Double-Blind Method , Humans , Memory Disorders/psychology , Middle Aged , Outcome Assessment, Health Care , Research Design
4.
Int Psychogeriatr ; 29(8): 1247-1259, 2017 08.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28462742

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Evidence-based data on prevalence and risk factors of suicidal intentions and behavior in dementia are as scarce as the data on assisted dying. The present literature review aimed on summarizing the current knowledge and provides a critical discussion of the results. METHODS: A systematic narrative literature review was performed using Medline, Cochrane Library, EMBASE, PSYNDEX, PSYCINFO, Sowiport, and Social Sciences Citation Index literature. RESULTS: Dementia as a whole does not appear to be a risk factor for suicide completion. Nonetheless some subgroups of patients with dementia apparently have an increased risk for suicidal behavior, such as patients with psychiatric comorbidities (particularly depression) and of younger age. Furthermore, a recent diagnosis of dementia, semantic dementia, and previous suicide attempts most probably elevate the risk for suicidal intentions and behavior. The impact of other potential risk factors, such as patient's cognitive impairment profile, behavioral disturbances, social isolation, or a biomarker based presymptomatic diagnosis has not yet been investigated. Assisted dying in dementia is rare but numbers seem to increase in regions where it is legally permitted. CONCLUSION: Most studies that had investigated the prevalence and risk factors for suicide in dementia had significant methodological limitations. Large prospective studies need to be conducted in order to evaluate risk factors for suicide and assisted suicide in patients with dementia and persons with very early or presymptomatic diagnoses of dementia. In clinical practice, known risk factors for suicide should be assessed in a standardized way so that appropriate action can be taken when necessary.


Subject(s)
Dementia/psychology , Suicide, Assisted/psychology , Suicide, Attempted/psychology , Comorbidity , Humans , Risk Factors
5.
Neuropsychol Rehabil ; 25(6): 913-35, 2015.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25558769

ABSTRACT

Relying on procedural memory is a promising approach for interventions that address the cognitive difficulties found in semantic dementia. The aim of this study was to determine if procedural memory could be used to optimise learning of relevant smartphone functions in MH, a 55-year-old man with semantic dementia. The impact of learning to use specific smartphone applications, which display concepts and their semantic characteristics, on relearning useful significant concepts, was also explored in MH. This patient, who showed no deficits in procedural learning on a serial reaction time paradigm, was able to learn manipulations related to 15 smartphone functions although, because of his deficit in word comprehension, he generally needed verbal cues to clarify which functions he was asked to perform. Six months after the end of the intervention, he was still using 8 of the 15 functions regularly. However, repeated exposure to concepts through the use of two applications did not improve naming or retrieval of semantic attributes. This study showed the potential of relying on procedural memory to optimise learning of new technologies in the ecological rehabilitation of semantic dementia.


Subject(s)
Activities of Daily Living , Frontotemporal Dementia/rehabilitation , Memory, Long-Term , Mobile Applications/statistics & numerical data , Smartphone/statistics & numerical data , Brain/pathology , Cues , Frontotemporal Dementia/pathology , Frontotemporal Dementia/psychology , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Motor Skills , Neuropsychological Tests , Practice, Psychological , Reaction Time
6.
J Prev Alzheimers Dis ; 1(1): 13-22, 2014 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26594639

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: The Multidomain Alzheimer Preventive Trial (MAPT study) was designed to assess the efficacy of isolated supplementation with omega-3 fatty acid, an isolated multidomain intervention (consisting of nutritional counseling, physical exercise, cognitive stimulation) or a combination of the two interventions on the change of cognitive functions in frail subjects aged 70 years and older for a period of 3 years. Ancillary neuroimaging studies were additionally implemented to evaluate the impact of interventions on cerebral metabolism (FDG PET scans) and atrophy rate (MRIs), as well as brain amyloïd deposit (AV45 PET scans). DESIGN PATIENTS: 1680 subjects (mean age: 75.3 years; female: 64.8 %), enrolled by 13 memory clinics, were randomized into one of the following four groups: omega-3 supplementation alone, multidomain intervention alone, omega-3 plus multidomain intervention, or placebo. Participants underwent cognitive, functional and biological assessments at M6, M12, M24 and M36 visits. The primary endpoint is a change of memory function at 3 years, as assessed by the Free and Cued Selective Reminding test. All participants will be followed for 2 additional years after the 3-years intervention (MAPT PLUS extension study). INTERVENTIONS: 1/Omega-3 supplementation: two soft capsules daily as a single dose, containing a total of 400 mg docosahexaenoic acid (DHA), i.e., 800 mg docosahexaenoic acid per day, for 3 years. 2/ Multidomain intervention: collective training sessions conducted in small groups (6-8 participants) in twelve 120-minute sessions over the first 2 months (two sessions a week for the first month, and one session a week the second month) then a 60-minute session per month in the following three areas: nutrition, physical activity, and cognition until the end of the 3 years. In addition to the collective sessions, individualized preventive outpatient visits exploring possible risk factors for cognitive decline are performed at baseline, M12 and M24. BASELINE POPULATION: For cognition, the mean MMSE at baseline was 28.1 (± 1.6). About 58% and 42% of participants had a CDR score equal to 0 and 0.5, respectively. Regarding mobility status, 200 (11.9%) had a 4-m gait speed lower or equal to 0.8 m/s. According to the Fried criteria, 673 (42.1%) participants were considered pre frail, and 51 (3.2%) frail. The red blood cell DHA content was 26.1 ± 8.1 µg/g. Five hundred and three participants underwent baseline MRI. AV45 PET scans were performed in 271 individuals and preliminary results showed that 38.0% had a cortical SUVR > 1.17, which gave an indication of significant brain amyloïd deposit. DISCUSSION: The MAPT trial is presently the first largest and longest multidomain preventive trial relevant to cognitive decline in older adults with subjective memory complaints. The multidomain intervention designed for the MAPT trial is likely to be easily implemented within the general population.

7.
Brain Cogn ; 75(2): 111-8, 2011 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21094574

ABSTRACT

The primary goal of this study was to evaluate the false recognition phenomenon in persons with frontotemporal dementia (FTD) and those with Lewy-body disease (LBD). Patients with LBD (n=10) or FTD (n=15) and their corresponding controls (n=30) were subjected to the Deese-Roediger-McDermott (DRM) paradigm to induce false recognition. Patients were first presented with items semantically related to a nonpresented critical target. The critical target was later included in a word list shown to patients to assess level of recognition. Both groups of patients showed a reduced level of false recognition of the critical target when controlling for their overall level of false alarms. This reduction was greater in persons with LBD than in those with FTD. Correlational analyses of performance on neuropsychological tests and the DRM variables indicated that the reduced DRM effect was associated with inhibition deficits in patients with LBD and with inhibition deficits and verbal memory in those with FTD. Our results support current models suggesting that these cognitive components contribute to the false recognition effect.


Subject(s)
Frontotemporal Dementia/physiopathology , Lewy Body Disease/physiopathology , Recognition, Psychology/physiology , Verbal Learning/physiology , Analysis of Variance , Humans , Inhibition, Psychological , Male , Neuropsychological Tests
8.
Rev Neurol (Paris) ; 164 Suppl 3: S63-72, 2008 May.
Article in French | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18675049

ABSTRACT

This article reviews how neuropsychology, in the French-speaking world, has evolved as a discipline focused on research, teaching and clinical work. The article targets the last 30 years as this corresponds to the time at which the Société de Neuropsychologie de Langue Française (French-Speaking Neuropsychological Society) was created. The review addresses how the cognitive neuropsychology approach and the advent of brain imaging have shaped the field of neuropsychology in recent years. It presents the status of the discipline in the main French-speaking countries (where neuropsychology is currently developed) including France, Belgium, Switzerland and Canada. It also analyzes a number of indicators that reflect the vitality of the discipline and its cohesion as a science and as a clinical domain. These indicators include the creation of specialized journals, organization of scientific meetings, accessibility to training programs in neuropsychology, development of discipline-oriented clinical programs, and the increase in scientific productivity. The Quebec academic environment is used as an illustration, whereby structured clinical doctoral training programs that meet national standards in neuropsychology were implemented to train clinical neuropsychologists. Finally, the authors emphasize the major role that the discipline is likely to play at different levels of society in the near future.


Subject(s)
Neuropsychology/trends , France , Humans , Quebec , Research/trends
10.
Rev Neurol (Paris) ; 158(1): 29-39, 2002 Jan.
Article in French | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11938320

ABSTRACT

The aging of the population leads to increased awareness of problems associated with age-related degenerative dementias. Given that these dementias are progressive in onset, many clinicians and researchers have proposed criteria that allow for the identification of older subjects manifesting cognitive impairment, but not responding to the criteria for dementia. Our knowledge of subjects with mild cognitive impairment is limited; it is, however, established that they present a high risk of developing dementia in the future. Although it is essential to increase our comprehension of their cognitive and functional decline, the study of subjects presenting mild cognitive impairment is compromised due to the existence of numerous non-converging classifications. The goal of the present article is to conduct a critical review of the different classifications of mild cognitive impairment in the elderly in order to attempt to identify the optimal criteria, allowing for a distinction to be made between subjects with mild cognitive impairment, who remain in a stable state and those whose condition evolves to a dementia. These criteria may enable us to describe a homogenuous group of individuals presenting with different rates of dementia risk factors. We present the classifications most frequently used in clinical and research settings. After listing them according to categorial, clinical or dimensional approaches, we performed a critical analysis for each one. Depending on the diagnostic criteria applied, major variations are revealed for the prevalence of cognitive impairment and the incidence of dementia. They are explained by methodological and theoretical shortcomings that we point out and discuss (e.g., reference group, lack of diagnostic criteria or exclusion criteria, high level of subjectivity). Beyond these criticisms, we discuss the challenges to be met in order to reach the optimal identification criteria. Notably, the impact of mild cognitive impairment on daily living activities should be tested with the use of more specific questionnaires/tasks. The goal of the objective definition of cognitive impairment should be to minimize subjectivity in the diagnosis. It is also suggested that sensitive cognitive measures would be used on all aspects of cognition, while recognizing and taking into account all confounding factors (e.g., age, education level). Given the nature and consequences of mild cognitive impairment, an inter-disciplinary approach is suggested (e.g., neurobiological, psychiatric, and genetic cues). A consensus on optimal diagnostic criteria is essential to propose cognitive and pharmacological treatments for the effective prevention of ementia.


Subject(s)
Cognition Disorders , Age Factors , Aged , Aging , Cognition Disorders/classification , Cognition Disorders/diagnosis , Humans , Severity of Illness Index
11.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry ; 42(2): 253-60, 2001 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11280421

ABSTRACT

Memory tasks were administered to 14 high-functioning individuals with autism and 14 typically developing individuals matched on chronological age and verbal intelligence. The tasks consisted of free and cued recall of 15 semantically unrelated words in 3 encoding conditions: phonological encoding, semantic encoding, and a no encoding orientation. In both groups, semantic orientation led to better free recall than did orientation toward syllabic encoding or absence of orientation. In contrast, semantic cues at retrieval led to better cued recall than phonological cues in typically developing individuals, whereas both types of cue had the same effect in prompting cued recall for individuals with autism. These findings are incompatible with the hypothesis of an amnesic deficit and do not support the notion of executive or semantic deficits in the memory problems of autistic individuals, at least for those with a high level of functioning. It is proposed that these findings can be accounted for by enhanced phonological processing in autism. This interpretation is consistent with other findings of enhanced processing of low-level perceptual information in the visual and auditory modality in autism.


Subject(s)
Autistic Disorder/psychology , Memory , Mental Processes , Semantics , Adolescent , Adult , Case-Control Studies , Child , Cognition , Female , Humans , Male
12.
Dev Neuropsychol ; 19(3): 237-51, 2001.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11758667

ABSTRACT

In this article, we discuss the effects of education level and age on short-term memory. The performance of young and elderly persons was compared on an adapted version of the Brown-Peterson procedure. Participants were asked to report consonant trigrams, after variable time periods, during which they performed a mental addition task or an articulation task. A control condition consisted of a no-interference task. Both age groups were divided according to individual educational level (greater or less than the median number of school years in each age group). The results revealed a significant effect of education. Moreover, the education effect interacted with the task: participants with less education were more impaired in mental addition than in articulation. However, neither the age effect nor the interactions involving age reached significance. These results indicate that education, to a greater extent than age, should be considered a determining factor of performance when interpolated tasks of high demand are used with the Brown-Peterson procedure.


Subject(s)
Education , Adult , Age Factors , Aged , Aging , Female , Humans , Male , Random Allocation
13.
Rev Neurol (Paris) ; 156(8-9): 759-66, 2000 Sep.
Article in French | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10992120

ABSTRACT

Normal aging, as well as most of the degenerative diseases of the central nervous system characterizing old age, have an impact on cognitive abilities which are obvious as well as increasingly understood. Cognitive changes in normal ageing realize a plurality of possible patterns of cognitive disabilities. Dementias are also well known to be characterized by a plurality of cognitive impairments patterns. The different cognitive patterns are likely to be due to distinct levels of impairments of different domains of cognition, or of different components within one given domain of cognition. These differences are such that in some cases they can even realize double dissociations. The plurality of cognitive impairments patterns in normal aging and in dementia will be discussed as well as the different factors that could contribute to such a plurality. Some of these factors relate to pre-morbid characteristics of the individuals known to influence the functional organization of the brain for cognition. Others--of major importance--are related to the nature and the distribution of the pathophysiological processes underlying normal ageing or a given degenerative disease. It is also reminded that some or many of these factors can interact with more general cognitive factors, such as speed of execution. It is however suggested that, despite of their presence, these general factors partly contribute to the cognitive impairments in neurodegenerative dementia. In this case, the impact of different disease-related factors over a general cognitive factor is probably much greater than in the case of normal aging.


Subject(s)
Aging/psychology , Alzheimer Disease/psychology , Cognition Disorders/psychology , Cognition/physiology , Aged , Aging/physiology , Alzheimer Disease/physiopathology , Cognition Disorders/physiopathology , Humans
14.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry ; 40(5): 743-55, 1999 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10433408

ABSTRACT

In the present study, copying tasks were used to assess hierarchical aspects of visual perception in a group of 10 nonsavant autistic individuals with normal intelligence. In Experiment 1, the hierarchical order of graphic construction and the constancy of this order were measured for the copying of objects and nonobjects. In comparison to control participants, autistic individuals produced more local features at the start of the copying. However, they did not differ from controls with respect to graphic constancy. Experiment 2 measured the effect of geometrical impossibility on the copying of figures. Results revealed that autistic individuals were less affected by figure impossibility than were controls. Therefore, these experiments seem to support the notion of a local bias for visual information processing in individuals with autism. Two interpretations are proposed to account for this effect. According to the hierarchical deficit hypothesis, individuals with autism do not manifest the normal global bias in perceiving scenes and objects. Alternatively, the executive function hypothesis suggests that autism brings about limitations in the complexity of information that can be manipulated in short-term visual memory during graphic planning.


Subject(s)
Attention , Autistic Disorder/diagnosis , Mental Recall , Psychomotor Performance , Adolescent , Adult , Autistic Disorder/psychology , Depth Perception , Discrimination Learning , Female , Humans , Intelligence , Male , Optical Illusions , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Problem Solving
15.
Brain Cogn ; 40(3): 464-78, 1999 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10415132

ABSTRACT

This critical review examines constructional apraxia from a cognitive neuropsychological perspective. To our knowledge, van Sommers (1989) is the only researcher to present a global cognitive model of drawing abilities. He organizes it into two hierarchical systems: Marr's model of visual perception and a graphic production system. The latter comprises four hierarchically organized components: depiction decisions, production strategy, contingent planning, and articulatory and economic constraints. Van Sommers' model will be discussed in light of other models and on the basis of empirical neuropsychological studies (Farah, 1984; Kosslyn & Koenig, 1992; Roncato, Sartori, Masterson, & Rumiati, 1987; van Sommers, 1989). We find that: (1) the Kosslyn and Koenig visual perception model describes more accurately the perceptual components underlying copying than the visual perception system of van Sommers' drawing model, (2) Van Sommers' arguments in favor of a depiction processing as opposed to visual imagery are not convincing, (3) Van Sommers' assumption that a production strategy is a component is unclear, and (4) articulatory and economic constraints are not cognitive components, but constraints imposed during action programming. This literature review leads to a discussion of future research topics and the specificity of constructional apraxia.


Subject(s)
Apraxias/diagnosis , Cognition/physiology , Decision Making/physiology , Eidetic Imagery , Humans , Neuropsychological Tests , Visual Perception/physiology
16.
Brain Res Cogn Brain Res ; 7(3): 411-7, 1999 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9838207

ABSTRACT

Most previous PET studies investigating the central executive (CE) component of working memory found activation in the prefrontal cortex. However, the tasks used did not always permit to distinguish precisely the functions of the CE from the storage function of the slave systems. The aim of the present study was to isolate brain areas that subserve manipulation of information by the CE when the influence of storage function was removed. A PET activation study was performed with four cognitive tasks, crossing conditions of temporary storage and manipulation of information. The manipulation of information induced an activation in the right (BA 10/46) and left (BA 9/6) middle frontal gyrus and in the left parietal area (BA7). The interaction between the storage and manipulation conditions did not reveal any significant changes in activation. These results are in agreement with the hypothesis that CE functions are distributed between anterior and posterior brain areas, but could also reflect a simultaneous involvement of controlled (frontal) and automatic (parietal) attentional systems. In the other hand, the absence of interaction between the storage and manipulation conditions demonstrates that the CE is not necessarily related to the presence of a memory load.


Subject(s)
Frontal Lobe/physiology , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Parietal Lobe/physiology , Adult , Cognition/physiology , Frontal Lobe/diagnostic imaging , Humans , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Parietal Lobe/diagnostic imaging , Tomography, Emission-Computed
17.
Mem Cognit ; 26(3): 572-83, 1998 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9610125

ABSTRACT

The goal of this study is to examine the central executive of working memory in normal aging, specifically focusing on its capacities to manipulate or modify the format of the to-be-recalled material. The central executive was measured with the alphabetical span procedure, during which subjects were asked to recall a random series of words in their alphabetical order. The storage demand was equalized across subjects by adjusting the list lengths according to individual span. Experiments 1, 2, and 3 showed that elderly subjects were not impaired in manipulating information, relative to young controls, even when the difficulty of the task was increased. In Experiment 4, validity was tested by asking young subjects to perform the task under the conditions of full or divided attention. Alphabetical recall was more impaired than direct recall during the divided attention condition, which suggests a larger involvement of the central executive component in the former. These results are discussed in relation to the hypothesis of a central executive impairment being associated with normal aging.


Subject(s)
Aging/psychology , Mental Recall , Serial Learning , Adult , Aged , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Reference Values
18.
Memory ; 6(6): 593-607, 1998 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10320865

ABSTRACT

This study explored the mechanisms underlying the hypermnesia of an autistic savant (NM) through three experiments. The first two served to assess whether absence of interference was responsible for NM's exceptional list memory. The third investigated the type of cues used in recall. Results indicated absence of retroactive interference but presence of slight proactive interference in list recall of proper names. Normal interference effects were found, however, in list recall of common nouns. Exceptional performance was also demonstrated in a missing-name task involving spatial and verbal recall cues. The findings suggest that the outstanding episodic memory presented by some savant persons with autism might be related to an abnormally high resistance to interference.


Subject(s)
Autistic Disorder/psychology , Memory , Adolescent , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Cues , Humans , Male , Mental Recall , Names
19.
Can J Exp Psychol ; 51(4): 354-68, 1997 Dec.
Article in French | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9687196

ABSTRACT

We present the neuropsychological study of a patient, I.R., who sustained bilateral damage to the temporal lobes and to the right frontal lobe as a result of successive brain surgeries that occurred ten years earlier. The patient is 40 years old and right-handed; she had no special training in music or in language, representing, therefore, the large majority of listeners. Her performance is compared to that of four neurologically intact subjects who are closely matched in terms of education, sex and age. In the present study, we report I.R.'s performance on various tests aiming at assessing her general cognitive functioning with a particular focus on auditory aspects. The results show that, despite extensive damage to her auditory cortex, I.R.'s speech abilities are essentially intact (see Tables 1 and 2). The only impairments that are detected in the language domain are related to a short-term memory deficit, to some abnormal sensitivity to retroactive interference in long-term memory (see Table 3) and to articulation. These difficulties do not, however, affect linguistic communication, which is obviously undisturbed I.R. is not aphasic). Similarly, I.R. does not experience any difficulty in the recognition and memorization of familiar sounds such as animal cries, traffic noises and the like (see Tables 5 and 7). In contrast, I.R. is severely impaired in most musical abilities: She can no longer discriminate nor identify melodies that were once highly familiar to her; she can no longer discriminate nor memorize novel melodies (see Table 4). Her pattern of musical losses is compatible with a basic and severe perceptual deficit that compromises access to and registration in memory systems. The observation that the auditory impairment affects music and spares language and environmental sounds refers to a neuropsychological condition that is known as music agnosia. I.R. represents, to our knowledge, the fourth case of music agnosia available in the literature (Peretz et al., 1994; Griffiths et al., 1997). The existence of such cases suggests that music processing is not mediated by a general-purpose auditory architecture but by specialized cortical subsystems. Not only does I.R. suffer from music agnosia, but she is also impaired in the discrimination and recognition of musical instruments and of human voices (see Table 5). These latter two deficits probably do not result from the music agnosic condition. Rather, they seem to reflect damage to adjacent brain areas that are specialized in timbre processing (see Peretz. et al., 1994, for the relevant discussion). It is also worth mentioning that I.R. appears to be impaired in musical expressive abilities as well: I.R. can no longer sing a single note. Thus, her losses are rather general in the musical domain, hence justifying the classification of her case as amusia. Cases of amusia without aphasia are relatively frequent in the neuropsychological literature. However, all of these reported cases are anecdotal. Thus, in the present study, special focus is given to the measurement and direct comparison of performance in the language and music domain; in both domains, task characteristics and materials were as similar as possible. To this aim, the lyrics and the tune of the same popular song excerpts were used. The musical and the spoken parts were presented separately in a primed familiarity decision task and in a memory recognition task. In both situations, I.R. performs at or close to chance when she has to deal with music, whereas she recognizes easily and performs normally on the spoken material (see Tables 6 and 7). These results clearly argue for the autonomy of music and language in the processing of auditory information.


Subject(s)
Agnosia , Auditory Perception , Cognition Disorders/classification , Music/psychology , Perceptual Disorders , Temporal Lobe , Adult , Agnosia/classification , Agnosia/etiology , Aphasia/etiology , Auditory Perception/classification , Auditory Perception/physiology , Cerebral Decortication/adverse effects , Cognition Disorders/etiology , Female , Humans , Neuropsychological Tests , Perceptual Disorders/classification , Perceptual Disorders/etiology , Speech Perception/physiology , Temporal Lobe/physiopathology , Temporal Lobe/surgery
20.
J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci ; 51(6): P356-63, 1996 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8931623

ABSTRACT

The purpose of this study was to assess whether older adults can inhibit irrelevant auditory information while performing a span task. In Experiment 1, young and older participants recalled seven visually presented digits while subject to three types of irrelevant noise; white noise (nonverbal), and familiar and nonfamiliar language (verbal). A baseline measure was obtained in silence. The effect of each noise was also assessed under articulatory suppression (AS). In Experiment 2, the number of digits to recall was adjusted to participants' individual span. Results show a clear irrelevant speech effect (ISE), that is, recall declines in the presence of familiar and nonfamiliar verbal noise, but is unaffected by white noise. AS negates ISE. Most important, ISE does not interact with age: older adults have no more difficulty inhibiting irrelevant auditory information than do young adults. These results are discussed relative to current theories of inhibition in aging.


Subject(s)
Aging/physiology , Memory/physiology , Mental Recall , Speech/physiology , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...