Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 16 de 16
Filter
1.
Front Psychol ; 14: 1221661, 2023.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38370107

ABSTRACT

Introduction: Self-harm amongst young people is becoming increasingly prevalent. Understanding, responding to, and supporting young people who self-harm is vital. Friends are typically the first and sometimes the only source of support sought by adolescents who self-harm. Despite their important role as confidants, friends' perspectives and experiences remain poorly understood. Methods: We conducted retrospective qualitative semi-structured interviews, prompted by an adapted version of the Card Sort Task for Self-Harm (CaTS-FF), about the experiences of nine female young adults (18-20 years old) who supported a friend who self-harmed during adolescence. Data were analyzed using thematic analysis. Results: Four themes were developed: (1) "I did not realize my friend was on the road to self-harm": Friends' reactions to self-harm; (2) "That's what friends do": the role of friends; (3) The impact of supporting a friend who self-harms; and (4) "They were quite formative years": reflecting on growth through the experience. Discussion: The present findings highlight the complex experiences of young people supporting a friend who self-harms. Despite being willing to take on the role of a supporter, participants experienced a range of difficult emotions and consequences. The temporal transition running through the four themes reflects the evolving nature of participants' attitudes, knowledge, and friendships. Overall, results highlight the unmet needs of adolescents supporting young people who self-harm, as well as identifying potential pathways to "support the supporters" toward resilience.

2.
Transl Psychiatry ; 12(1): 453, 2022 10 19.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36261422

ABSTRACT

Positive mood amplification is a hallmark of the bipolar disorder spectrum (BPDS). We need better understanding of cognitive mechanisms contributing to such elevated mood. Generation of vivid, emotionally compelling mental imagery is proposed to act as an 'emotional amplifier' in BPDS. We used a positive mental imagery generation paradigm to manipulate affect in a subclinical BPDS-relevant sample reporting high (n = 31) vs. low (n = 30) hypomanic-like experiences on the Mood Disorder Questionnaire (MDQ). Participants were randomized to an 'elated' or 'calm' mental imagery condition, rating their momentary affect four times across the experimental session. We hypothesized greater affect increase in the high (vs. low) MDQ group assigned to the elated (vs. calm) imagery generation condition. We further hypothesized that affect increase in the high MDQ group would be particularly apparent in the types of affect typically associated with (hypo)mania, i.e., suggestive of high activity levels. Mixed model and time-series analysis showed that for the high MDQ group, affect increased steeply and in a sustained manner over time in the 'elated' imagery condition, and more shallowly in 'calm'. The low-MDQ group did not show this amplification effect. Analysis of affect clusters showed high-MDQ mood amplification in the 'elated' imagery condition was most pronounced for active affective states. This experimental model of BPDS-relevant mood amplification shows evidence that positive mental imagery drives changes in affect in the high MDQ group in a targeted manner. Findings inform cognitive mechanisms of mood amplification, and spotlight prevention strategies targeting elated imagery, while potentially retaining calm imagery to preserve adaptive positive emotionality.


Subject(s)
Bipolar Disorder , Mania , Young Adult , Humans , Affect , Emotions , Bipolar Disorder/psychology , Imagery, Psychotherapy , Surveys and Questionnaires
3.
Emotion ; 22(6): 1193-1207, 2022 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33370141

ABSTRACT

The relations between self and others are fluid and constantly changing but exert a profound influence on our identity and emotional experiences. Indeed, human emotions are frequently and intensely social, and the people with whom we interact can alter our momentary mood. But does emotional "contagion" extend over prolonged periods of hours to days, and, if so, how does it propagate through interconnected groups? Answering this question is empirically challenging, because mood similarity in connected individuals can arise through multiple mechanisms (social influence, social selection, and shared external causation), making causal inferences hard to draw. We address this challenge using temporally high-resolution, longitudinal data from 2 independent, bounded social networks during periods of high communal activity and low external contact. Adolescent study participants (N = 79) completed daily mood (n = 4,724) and social interaction (n = 1,775) ratings during residential performance tours of classical music lasting 5 to 7 days. Analyses using statistical network models show that in both networks, adolescent musicians became reciprocally more similar in mood to their interaction partners. The observed contagion effect was greater for negative than for positive mood. That is, although one may catch a friend's bad mood, the friend may feel less negative in the process. These results suggest a mechanism for emotional buffering and the cost of social support. We found no evidence for social selection based on mood. Indeed, participants were remarkably tolerant of their peers' mood fluctuations and showed no evidence of altering their patterns of social interaction accordingly. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).


Subject(s)
Affect , Emotions , Adolescent , Friends , Humans , Social Networking , Social Support
4.
J Affect Disord ; 277: 742-746, 2020 12 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32919295

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Bipolar spectrum disorders (BPSD) are associated with frequent and emotional mental imagery, theorized to play a role in mood instability. However, we lack methods for measuring tendency to experience emotional mental imagery in daily life. The current study developed such a measure and evaluated the hypothesis that a high tendency to experience emotional imagery in daily life would be associated with higher levels of hypomanic-like experiences. METHODS: We conducted two rounds of studies to develop and refine a measure of spontaneous emotional imagery (E-SUIS) using factor analysis. We conducted a third study to test the relationship between E-SUIS score and hypomanic-like experiences. Participants (total N = 554, age 18-25) comprised an unselected community sample. RESULTS: First, factor analysis indicated a unidimensional factor structure and excellent reliability (α=0.87) of our novel measure of spontaneous emotional mental imagery. Second, higher scores on a hypomanic-like experiences scale related to higher use of both spontaneous emotional imagery and spontaneous non-emotional imagery. Spontaneous emotional mental imagery significantly improved the prediction of hypomanic-like experiences over non-emotional mental imagery. LIMITATIONS: Only two mental imagery measures were included. To determine discriminant validity of the E-SUIS requires additional imagery measures or interviews. CONCLUSIONS: Levels of hypomanic-like experiences were related to the tendency to use emotional imagery in daily life. Additionally, spontaneous use of emotional imagery appears to be a better predictor of hypomanic-like experiences in the general population compared to spontaneous use of non-emotional imagery.


Subject(s)
Bipolar Disorder , Imagination , Adolescent , Adult , Emotions , Humans , Imagery, Psychotherapy , Reproducibility of Results , Young Adult
5.
Dev Sci ; 23(3): e12926, 2020 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31762124

ABSTRACT

Aggressive behaviour serves many useful social functions, yet can also have damaging consequences. In line with evidence showing adolescent development in social cognitive abilities, we hypothesized that the use of aggression would become more sophisticated with age. We investigated adolescent aggression towards peers using an experimental, hypothetical aggression paradigm, the hot sauce paradigm, in a school-based social network setting. Participants (N = 162 aged 11-17, 98 male) indicated which strength of imaginary hot sauce they would allocate to each of their classmates. A Social Network Questionnaire quantified participants' perceived dyadic social tie strength with each classmate, and the incidence of mutual or unilateral dyadic real-world aggression (e.g. teasing). Participants allocated weaker hot sauce to peers with whom they reported strong, positive social ties and an absence of self-reported unilateral real-world aggression. With increasing cross-sectional age, there was a decrease in the impact of social tie strength and an increase in the extent to which hot sauce allocation was predicted by self-reported mutual real-world aggression. This pattern of findings is consistent with young (vs. late) adolescent use of experimental, hypothetical Hot Sauce aggression to reflect real-world animosity, while late adolescents' behaviour is more subtle. These findings extend our understanding of the dyadic social context of adolescent aggressive behaviour using a novel experimental aggression paradigm.


Subject(s)
Adolescent Behavior/psychology , Aggression/psychology , Friends/psychology , Peer Group , Adolescent , Adolescent Development , Age Factors , Cross-Sectional Studies , Female , Humans , Interpersonal Relations , Male , Self Report , Students
6.
Cortex ; 105: 104-117, 2018 08.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28912037

ABSTRACT

Vivid emotional mental imagery has been identified across a range of mental disorders. In bipolar spectrum disorders - psychopathologies characterized by mood swings that alternate between depression and mania, and include irritability and mixed affect states - mental imagery has been proposed to drive instability in both 'positive' and 'negative' mood. That is, mental imagery can act as an "emotional amplifier". The current experimental study tested this hypothesis and investigated imagery characteristics associated with mood amplification using a spectrum approach to psychopathology. Young adults (N = 42) with low, medium and high scores on a measure of subclinical features of bipolar disorder (BD), i.e., hypomanic-like experiences such as overly 'positive' mood, excitement and hyperactivity, completed a mental imagery generation training task using positive picture-word cues. Results indicate that (1) mood amplification levels were dependent on self-reported hypomanic-like experiences. In particular, (2) engaging in positive mental imagery led to mood amplification of both positive and negative mood in those participants higher in hypomanic-like experiences. Further, (3) in participants scoring high for hypomanic-like experiences, greater vividness of mental imagery during the experimental task was associated with greater amplification of positive mood. Thus, for individuals with high levels of hypomanic-like experiences, the generation of emotional mental imagery may play a causal role in their mood changes. This finding has implications for understanding mechanisms driving mood amplification in bipolar spectrum disorders, such as targeting imagery vividness in therapeutic interventions.


Subject(s)
Affect/physiology , Bipolar Disorder/psychology , Emotions/physiology , Imagery, Psychotherapy , Adolescent , Adult , Affective Symptoms/psychology , Depression/psychology , Female , Humans , Imagination/physiology , Male , Mood Disorders/psychology , Young Adult
7.
Soc Sci Med ; 196: 47-55, 2018 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29127852

ABSTRACT

Complex human behaviour can only be understood within its social environment. However, disentangling the causal links between individual outcomes and social network position is empirically challenging. We present a research design in a closed real-world setting with high-resolution temporal data to understand this interplay within a fundamental human experience - physical pain. Study participants completed an isolated 3-week hiking expedition in the Arctic Circle during which they were subject to the same variation in environmental conditions and only interacted amongst themselves. Adolescents provided daily ratings of pain and social interaction partners. Using longitudinal network models, we analyze the interplay between social network position and the experience of pain. Specifically, we test whether experiencing pain is linked to decreasing popularity (increasing isolation), whether adolescents prefer to interact with others experiencing similar pain (homophily), and whether participants are increasingly likely to report similar pain as their interaction partners (contagion). We find that reporting pain is associated with decreasing popularity - interestingly, this effect holds for males only. Further exploratory analyses suggest this is at least partly driven by males withdrawing from contact with females when in pain, enhancing our understanding of pain and masculinity. Contrary to recent experimental and clinical studies, we found no evidence of pain homophily or contagion in the expedition group.


Subject(s)
Interpersonal Relations , Pain/psychology , Adolescent , Expeditions , Female , Greenland , Humans , Male , Pain Measurement , Sex Factors
8.
Annu Rev Clin Psychol ; 12: 249-80, 2016.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26772205

ABSTRACT

Mental imagery is an experience like perception in the absence of a percept. It is a ubiquitous feature of human cognition, yet it has been relatively neglected in the etiology, maintenance, and treatment of depression. Imagery abnormalities in depression include an excess of intrusive negative mental imagery; impoverished positive imagery; bias for observer perspective imagery; and overgeneral memory, in which specific imagery is lacking. We consider the contribution of imagery dysfunctions to depressive psychopathology and implications for cognitive behavioral interventions. Treatment advances capitalizing on the representational format of imagery (as opposed to its content) are reviewed, including imagery rescripting, positive imagery generation, and memory specificity training. Consideration of mental imagery can contribute to clinical assessment and imagery-focused psychological therapeutic techniques and promote investigation of underlying mechanisms for treatment innovation. Research into mental imagery in depression is at an early stage. Work that bridges clinical psychology and neuroscience in the investigation of imagery-related mechanisms is recommended.


Subject(s)
Depressive Disorder/physiopathology , Imagination/physiology , Psychotherapy/methods , Depressive Disorder/therapy , Humans
9.
Child Dev ; 86(5): 1489-506, 2015.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26235042

ABSTRACT

Adolescence is characterized as a period of social reorientation toward peer relationships, entailing the emergence of sophisticated social abilities. Two studies (Study 1: N = 42, ages 13-17; Study 2: N = 81, ages 13-16) investigated age group differences in the impact of relationship reciprocation within school-based social networks on an experimental measure of cooperation behavior. Results suggest development between mid- and late adolescence in the extent to which reciprocation of social ties predicted resource allocation. With increasing age group, investment decisions increasingly reflected the degree to which peers reciprocated feelings of friendship. This result may reflect social-cognitive development, which could facilitate the ability to navigate an increasingly complex social world in adolescence and promote positive and enduring relationships into adulthood.


Subject(s)
Adolescent Behavior/psychology , Adolescent Development/physiology , Interpersonal Relations , Peer Group , Social Behavior , Adolescent , Female , Humans , Male , Social Support
10.
J Neuropsychol ; 9(2): 319-29, 2015 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25208525

ABSTRACT

Delayed adjustment tasks have recently been developed to examine working memory (WM) precision, that is, the resolution with which items maintained in memory are recalled. However, despite their emerging use in experimental studies of healthy people, evaluation of patient populations is sparse. We first investigated the validity of adjustment tasks, comparing precision with classical span measures of memory across the lifespan in 114 people. Second, we asked whether precision measures can potentially provide a more sensitive measure of WM than traditional span measures. Specifically, we tested this hypothesis examining WM in a group with early, untreated Parkinson's disease (PD) and its modulation by subsequent treatment on dopaminergic medication. Span measures correlated with precision across the lifespan: in children, young, and elderly participants. However, they failed to detect changes in WM in PD patients, either pre- or post-treatment initiation. By contrast, recall precision was sensitive enough to pick up such changes. PD patients pre-medication were significantly impaired compared to controls, but improved significantly after 3 months of being established on dopaminergic medication. These findings suggest that precision methods might provide a sensitive means to investigate WM and its modulation by interventions in clinical populations.


Subject(s)
Aging , Attention/physiology , Memory Disorders/diagnosis , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Mental Recall/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Child , Female , Humans , Male , Memory Disorders/etiology , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Parkinson Disease/complications , Reproducibility of Results , Young Adult
11.
J Physiol Paris ; 109(1-3): 16-26, 2015.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24747776

ABSTRACT

Apathy is a common but poorly understood condition with a wide societal impact observed in several brain disorders as well as, to some extent, in the normal population. Hence the need for better characterization of the underlying mechanisms. The processes by which individuals decide to attribute physical effort to obtain rewards might be particularly relevant to relate to apathy traits. Here, we designed two paradigms to assess individual differences in physical effort production and effort-based decision-making and their relation to apathy in healthy people. Apathy scores were measured using a modified version of the Lille Apathy Rating Scale, suitable for use in a non-clinical population. In the first study, apathy scores were correlated with the degree to which stake (reward on offer) and difficulty level impacts on physical effort production. Individuals with relatively high apathy traits showed an increased modulation of effort while more motivated individuals generally exerted greater force across different levels of stake. To clarify the underlying mechanisms for this behavior, we designed a second task that allows independent titration of stake and effort levels for which subjects are willing to engage in an effortful response to obtain a reward. Our results suggest that apathy traits in the normal population are related to the way reward subjectively affects the estimation of effort costs, and more particularly manifest as decreased willingness to exert effort when rewards are small, or below threshold. The tasks we introduce here may provide useful tools to further investigate apathy in clinical populations.


Subject(s)
Apathy , Decision Making/physiology , Motivation/physiology , Reward , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Algorithms , Analysis of Variance , Feedback, Psychological , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Statistics as Topic , Young Adult
12.
Horm Behav ; 64(2): 314-22, 2013 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23998674

ABSTRACT

This article is part of a Special Issue "Puberty and Adolescence". There is increasing evidence that puberty plays an important role in the structural and functional brain development seen in adolescence, but little is known of the pubertal influence on changes in functional connectivity. We explored how pubertal indicators (salivary concentrations of testosterone, oestradiol and DHEA; pubertal stage; menarcheal status) relate to functional connectivity between components of a mentalising network identified to be engaged in social emotion processing by our prior work, using psychophysiological interaction (PPI) analysis. Female adolescents aged 11 to 13years were scanned whilst silently reading scenarios designed to evoke either social emotions (guilt and embarrassment) or basic emotions (disgust and fear), of which only social compared to basic emotions require the representation of another person's mental states. Pubertal stage and menarcheal status were used to assign participants to pre/early or mid/late puberty groups. We found increased functional connectivity between the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (DMPFC) and the right posterior superior temporal sulcus (pSTS) and right temporo-parietal junction (TPJ) during social relative to basic emotion processing. Moreover, increasing oestradiol concentrations were associated with increased functional connectivity between the DMPFC and the right TPJ during social relative to basic emotion processing, independent of age. Our analysis of the PPI data by phenotypic pubertal status showed that more advanced puberty stage was associated with enhanced functional connectivity between the DMPFC and the left anterior temporal cortex (ATC) during social relative to basic emotion processing, also independent of age. Our results suggest increased functional maturation of the social brain network with the advancement of puberty in girls.


Subject(s)
Adolescent Development/physiology , Emotions/physiology , Mental Processes/physiology , Nerve Net/physiology , Puberty/psychology , Social Adjustment , Adolescent , Adolescent Behavior/physiology , Brain/physiology , Brain Mapping , Child , Facial Expression , Female , Head Movements/physiology , Humans
13.
Clin Psychol Rev ; 33(1): 1-23, 2013 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23123567

ABSTRACT

Mental imagery is an under-explored field in clinical psychology research but presents a topic of potential interest and relevance across many clinical disorders, including social phobia, schizophrenia, depression, and post-traumatic stress disorder. There is currently a lack of a guiding framework from which clinicians may select the domains or associated measures most likely to be of appropriate use in mental imagery research. We adopt an interdisciplinary approach and present a review of studies across experimental psychology and clinical psychology in order to highlight the key domains and measures most likely to be of relevance. This includes a consideration of methods for experimentally assessing the generation, maintenance, inspection and transformation of mental images; as well as subjective measures of characteristics such as image vividness and clarity. We present a guiding framework in which we propose that cognitive, subjective and clinical aspects of imagery should be explored in future research. The guiding framework aims to assist researchers in the selection of measures for assessing those aspects of mental imagery that are of most relevance to clinical psychology. We propose that a greater understanding of the role of mental imagery in clinical disorders will help drive forward advances in both theory and treatment.


Subject(s)
Depression/psychology , Imagination , Phobic Disorders/psychology , Psychology, Clinical/methods , Schizophrenia , Schizophrenic Psychology , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/psychology , Depression/diagnosis , Humans , Phobic Disorders/diagnosis , Psychometrics , Schizophrenia/diagnosis , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/diagnosis
14.
Dev Sci ; 15(6): 801-11, 2012 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23106734

ABSTRACT

The social brain undergoes developmental change during adolescence, and pubertal hormones are hypothesized to contribute to this development. We used fMRI to explore how pubertal indicators (salivary concentrations of testosterone, oestradiol and DHEA; pubertal stage; menarcheal status) relate to brain activity during a social emotion task. Forty-two females aged 11.1 to 13.7 years underwent fMRI scanning while reading scenarios pertaining either to social emotions, which require the representation of another person's mental states, or to basic emotions, which do not. Pubertal stage and menarcheal status were used to assign girls to early or late puberty groups. Across the entire sample, the contrast between social versus basic emotion resulted in activity within the social brain network, including dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (DMPFC), the posterior superior temporal sulcus, and the anterior temporal cortex (ATC) in both hemispheres. Increased hormone levels (independent of age) were associated with higher left ATC activity during social emotion processing. More advanced age (independent of hormone levels) was associated with lower DMPFC activity during social emotion processing. Our results suggest functionally dissociable effects of pubertal hormones and age on the adolescent social brain.


Subject(s)
Adolescent Development , Emotions/physiology , Interpersonal Relations , Puberty/psychology , Adolescent , Brain Mapping , Child , Dehydroepiandrosterone/analysis , Estradiol/analysis , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Regression Analysis , Saliva/chemistry , Testosterone/analysis
15.
Dev Sci ; 15(4): 528-39, 2012 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22709402

ABSTRACT

Visual working memory (VWM) is the facility to hold in mind visual information for brief periods of time. Developmental studies have suggested an increase during childhood in the maximum number of complete items that can simultaneously be stored in VWM. Here, we exploit a recent theoretical and empirical innovation to investigate instead the precision with which items are stored in VWM, where precision is a continuous measure reflecting VWM resolution. Ninety boys aged 7 to 13 years completed one-item and three-item VWM tasks in which stimuli were coloured bars varying in orientation. On each trial, participants used a rotating dial to reproduce the probed stimulus from memory. Results show linear age-related improvement in recall precision for both one-item and three-item VWM tasks. However, even the youngest age group stored a significant amount of information about all three items on the difficult 3-item VWM task. Importantly, the development of VWM precision was not accounted for by development on a sensorimotor control task. Whereas storage of a single complete item was previously thought to be well within the capacity limitations of the current age range, these results suggest protracted development during childhood and early adolescence in the resolution with which single and multiple items are stored in VWM. Probabilistic modelling of response distribution data suggests that improvement in VWM performance is attributable to a specific decrease in variability of stored feature representations, rather than to a decrease in misbinding or random noise. As such, we highlight a novel, potentially developmentally plausible mechanism that may underlie developmental improvement in VWM performance, independent of any alterations in the maximum number of complete items which can be stored.


Subject(s)
Color Perception/physiology , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Adolescent , Algorithms , Analysis of Variance , Child , Humans , Male , Models, Psychological , Photic Stimulation , Psychomotor Performance/physiology
16.
Front Psychol ; 3: 153, 2012.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22661960

ABSTRACT

Impulsivity is a feature of many brain disorders. Although often defined as the predisposition to act with an inadequate degree of deliberation, forethought, or control, it has proven difficult to measure. This may in part be due to the fact that it is a multifaceted construct, with impulsive decisions potentially arising as a result of a number of underlying mechanisms. Indeed, a "functional" degree of impulsivity may even promote effective behavior in healthy participants in a way that can be advantageous under certain circumstances. Although many tasks have been developed to study impulsivity, few examine decisions made rapidly, for time-sensitive rewards. In the current study we examine behavior in 59 adults on a manual "Traffic Light" task which requires participants to take risks under time pressure, if they are to maximize reward. We show that behavioral variables that index rapid anticipatory responding in this paradigm are correlated with one, specific self-report measure of impulsivity: "lack of premeditation" on the UPPS Impulsive Behavior Scale. Participants who scored more highly on this subscale performed better on the task. Moreover, anticipatory behavior reduced significantly with age (18-79 years), an effect that continued to be upheld after correction for potential age differences in the ability to judge the timing of responses. Based on these findings, we argue that the Traffic Light task provides a parametric method to study one aspect of impulsivity in health and disease: namely, rapid decision-making in pursuit of risky, time-sensitive rewards.

SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...