Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 25
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
Neurology ; 75(19): 1717-25, 2010 Nov 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21060094

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: To establish the diagnostic accuracy of the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) when screening externally validated cognition in Parkinson disease (PD), by comparison with a PD-focused test (Scales for Outcomes in Parkinson disease-Cognition [SCOPA-COG]) and the standardized Mini-Mental State Examination (S-MMSE) as benchmarks. METHODS: A convenience sample of 114 patients with idiopathic PD and 47 healthy controls was examined in a movement disorders center. The 21 patients with dementia (PD-D) were diagnosed using Movement Disorders Society criteria, externally validated by detailed independent functional and neuropsychological tests. The 21 patients with mild cognitive impairment (PD-MCI) scored 1.5 SD or more below normative data in at least 2 measures in 1 of 4 cognitive domains. Other patients had normal cognition (PD-N). RESULTS: Primary outcomes using receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curve analyses showed that all 3 mental status tests produced excellent discrimination of PD-D from patients without dementia (area under the curve [AUC], 87%-91%) and PD-MCI from PD-N patients (AUC, 78%-90%), but the MoCA was generally better suited across both assessments. The optimal MoCA screening cutoffs were <21/30 for PD-D (sensitivity 81%; specificity 95%; negative predictive value [NPV] 92%) and <26/30 for PD-MCI (sensitivity 90%; specificity 75%; NPV 95%). Further support that the MoCA is at least equivalent to the SCOPA-COG, and superior to the S-MMSE, came from the simultaneous classification of the 3 PD patient groups (volumes under a 3-dimensional ROC surface, chance = 17%: MoCA 79%, confidence interval [CI] 70%-89%; SCOPA-COG 74%, CI 62%-86%; MMSE-Sevens item 56%, CI 44%-68%; MMSE-World item 62%, CI 50%-73%). CONCLUSIONS: The MoCA is a suitably accurate, brief test when screening all levels of cognition in PD.


Subject(s)
Cognition Disorders/diagnosis , Cognition Disorders/psychology , Parkinson Disease/diagnosis , Parkinson Disease/psychology , Psychiatric Status Rating Scales , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Cognition Disorders/complications , Female , Humans , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests/standards , New Zealand , Parkinson Disease/complications , Psychiatric Status Rating Scales/standards , ROC Curve
2.
Neurocase ; 15(4): 311-7, 2009 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19370480

ABSTRACT

Monocular patching might improve perceptual-attentional, not motor-intentional deficits in a patient with chronic post-stroke left spatial neglect. Performing a line-cancellation task, his omission errors were associated with a perceptual-attentional 'where' deficit, while perseverative errors were associated with 'aiming' motor-intentional bias. Contralesional patching had no effect on the omissions (p = .871), whereas ipsilesional patching reduced left-sided omissions compared with the unpatched condition (p = .016). Neither patching condition altered perseverative errors. Further research is needed to examine whether targeting treatments to spatial neglect symptoms (omissions, perseveration) results in improved outcomes.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Cognition Disorders/therapy , Perceptual Disorders/therapy , Prostheses and Implants , Vision, Monocular/physiology , Adaptation, Physiological/physiology , Cognition Disorders/physiopathology , Cognition Disorders/psychology , Eye Protective Devices , Functional Laterality/physiology , Humans , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Orientation , Perceptual Disorders/diagnosis , Perceptual Disorders/physiopathology , Perceptual Disorders/psychology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Sensory Deprivation/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Spatial Behavior/physiology , Visual Fields/physiology
3.
Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat ; 3(6): 955-8, 2007 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19300633

ABSTRACT

We performed an analysis of prospectively-acquired cross sectional data on 106 Parkinson disease (PD) patients who underwent comprehensive neuropsychological testing and the Unified Parkinson Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS) motor scale. A significant correlation between the UPDRS motor and neuropsychological tests in all cognitive domains except for general intelligence and visuo-spatial function was seen. In this study, cognitive decline within this PD cohort correlated with motor impairment but not disease duration. Our findings suggest that overall cognitive impairment (except visuospatial dysfunction) may track motor progression in PD more than duration of disease. Longitudinal studies are needed to confirm our results.

4.
Neurology ; 67(7): 1285-7, 2006 Oct 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17030769

ABSTRACT

We evaluated the productions of an artist with frontotemporal lobar degeneration from before dementia onset until she was fully symptomatic. We noted an improvement of technique that might be related to sparing and disinhibition of the right posterior neocortex. There was a reduction of closure (completeness of the painting), possibly induced by impersistence and a decrease in evocative impact that might be explained by frontal and anterotemporal-limbic dysfunction.


Subject(s)
Creativity , Dementia , Paintings/classification , Task Performance and Analysis , Aged , Female , Humans
5.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry ; 77(6): 793-5, 2006 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16705204

ABSTRACT

To find out if patients with right hemisphere strokes (RHD) demonstrate a form of progressive or incremental limb hypometria (ILH), 11 RHD patients and eight matched controls were tested by having them draw a series of horizontal lines while blindfolded. Unlike controls, six RHD patients displayed an incremental decrease in the length of lines they drew, a sign of ILH. ILH might be a common source of disability and hinder rehabilitation efforts.


Subject(s)
Motor Skills Disorders/etiology , Stroke/complications , Aged , Arm/physiology , Case-Control Studies , Female , Functional Laterality , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Posture
7.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry ; 72(3): 403-5, 2002 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11861708

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: It remains unclear from lesion studies whether the four signs of the Gerstmann syndrome (finger agnosia, acalculia, agraphia, and right-left confusion) cluster because the neuronal nets that mediate these activities have anatomical proximity, or because these four functions share a common network. If there is a common network, with degeneration, as may occur in Alzheimer's disease, each of the signs associated with Gerstmann's syndrome should correlate with the other three signs more closely than they correlate with other cognitive deficits. METHODS: Thirty eight patients with probable Alzheimer's disease were included in a retrospective analysis of neuropsychological functions. RESULTS: The four Gerstmann's syndrome signs did not cluster together. Finger naming and calculations were not significantly correlated. Right-left knowledge and calculations also did not correlate. CONCLUSIONS: The four cognitive functions impaired in Gerstmann's syndrome do not share a common neuronal network, and their co-occurrence with dominant parietal lobe injuries may be related to the anatomical proximity of the different networks mediating these functions.


Subject(s)
Alzheimer Disease/diagnosis , Gerstmann Syndrome/diagnosis , Neuropsychological Tests , Adult , Aged , Alzheimer Disease/physiopathology , Anomia/diagnosis , Anomia/physiopathology , Diagnosis, Differential , Dominance, Cerebral/physiology , Female , Gerstmann Syndrome/physiopathology , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Nerve Net/physiopathology , Parietal Lobe/physiopathology , Problem Solving
8.
Neurology ; 57(1): 147-9, 2001 Jul 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11445648

ABSTRACT

The authors studied four patients with spatial neglect, using a task in which lines contain an off-centered bisection mark and a task in which the right and left segments of these bisected lines are presented independently and sequentially. In the prebisected line task, subjects reported the position of the bisection. In the segments task, subjects compared the length of the segments. Accuracy was greater with the sequential presentation of line segments, suggesting that an extinction-like phenomenon plays a role in line bisection bias.


Subject(s)
Memory , Perceptual Disorders/psychology , Aged , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Psychological Tests , Space Perception
9.
Arch Phys Med Rehabil ; 82(4): 516-8, 2001 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11295013

ABSTRACT

To determine whether monocular patching influences the performance of a patient with primarily sensory-attentional bias on the line bisection task, we present a case study of a 49-year-old woman who had right cortical infarction affecting temporal, parietal, and occipital regions. She had primarily sensory-attentional bias when performing the line bisection task on a video apparatus. In hospital, she was tested with monocular eye patching of the left or the right eye or unpatched. Paradoxically, the right-eye patching significantly worsened and the left patch significantly improved performance. The eye may have some input to the ipsilateral as well as the contralateral superior colliculus. Alternatively, the patch-a novel tactile stimulus-may induce orienting to its side via noncollicular mechanisms. When using a monocular patch for any reason, clinicians should be aware that increased spatial bias may occur.


Subject(s)
Perceptual Disorders/rehabilitation , Stroke/physiopathology , Vision Disorders/rehabilitation , Analysis of Variance , Attention , Bandages , Eye Movements , Female , Humans , Middle Aged , Perceptual Disorders/physiopathology , Sensory Deprivation , Vision Disorders/physiopathology
10.
Neurology ; 56(6): 724-9, 2001 Mar 27.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11274305

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: To learn if Parkinson's disease (PD) is associated with a restricted attentional "floodlight." BACKGROUND: Different visual tasks may have different attentional requirements. Focused attention may be needed for some tasks; other tasks demand spatially distributed attention. Neglect after right cortical injury and dopamine depletion may limit the area over which attention can be spread. Although subjects with PD have dopamine depletion and can perform poorly on tests of visuospatial function, it is unclear if their attentional floodlight is restricted. METHODS: Eleven subjects with PD and 11 control subjects viewed different-sized letters on five printed stimulus sheets, 43 x 56 cm. On each sheet, four different large letters (14 cm2) were composed of four different medium-sized letters (2.5 cm2), which in turn were composed of four different small letters (0.4 cm2). Stimulus sheets were presented at 30- and 75-cm viewing distances. Subjects named "all the letters they could see." RESULTS: Subjects with PD named small- and medium-sized letters comparably to control subjects, but PD subjects named fewer large letters than control subjects (control = 65.68%, PD = 24.55%; group-by-letter-size interaction, p < 0.05). Subjects with PD who had undergone stereotactic pallidotomy named more letters than prepallidotomy PD subjects (p = 0.05). CONCLUSIONS: PD may affect the patient's ability to perceive large spatial configurations. As global configurations in subjects may be perceived preferentially over local patterns, it is possible that DA depletion induces an aberrant perceptual-attentional bias, such that patients have a narrowed attentional floodlight.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Parkinson Disease/physiopathology , Space Perception/physiology , Aged , Analysis of Variance , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged
11.
Neurology ; 56(2): 159-65, 2001 Jan 23.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11160949

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: To learn how PD influences verbal description of emotional events. BACKGROUND: Individuals with PD exhibit emotional processing deficits. Emotional experience likely involves several dimensions (e.g., valence, arousal, motor activation) subserved by a distributed modular network involving cortical, limbic, basal ganglia, diencephalic, and mesencephalic regions. Although the neurodegeneration in PD likely affects components in this network, little is known about how PD influences emotional processing. Because PD is associated with activation deficits, one could predict that the discourse of emotional experiences involving high activation would be reduced in patients with PD compared to control subjects. Alternatively, because patients with PD exhibit paradoxical sensitivity to externally evoked motor activation (kinesia paradoxica), it is possible that emotional stimuli may facilitate verbal emotional expression more so in patients with PD than in control subjects. METHODS: The authors measured verbal descriptions of personal emotional experiences in subjects with PD and normal controls. RESULTS: Compared with control subjects, individuals with PD showed a relative increase in the number of words spoken and in discourse duration when talking about emotional experiences that are usually associated with high levels of arousal and motor activation. Although the authors did not measure arousal or activation, prior research has shown that, when asked to recall an emotional experience, people will often re-experience the emotion previously experienced during that episode. CONCLUSIONS: Recalling emotional episodes induces verbal kinesia paradoxica in patients with PD. Although recall of these emotional episodes may have been associated with increased arousal and activation, the mechanism underlying emotional verbal kinesia paradoxica is unclear.


Subject(s)
Emotions/physiology , Parkinson Disease/psychology , Speech/physiology , Aged , Female , Humans , Male , Parkinson Disease/physiopathology , Psychiatric Status Rating Scales
12.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 97(15): 8734-7, 2000 Jul 18.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10900024

ABSTRACT

Individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) have impaired ability to use context, which may manifest as alterations of relatedness within the semantic network. However, impairment in context use may be more difficult to detect in high-functioning adults with ASD. To test context use in this population, we examined the influence of context on memory by using the "false memory" test. In the false memory task, lists of words were presented to high-functioning subjects with ASD and matched controls. Each list consists of words highly related to an index word not on the list. Subjects are then given a recognition test. Positive responses to the index words represent false memories. We found that individuals with ASD are able to discriminate false memory items from true items significantly better than are control subjects. Memory in patients with ASD may be more accurate than in normal individuals under certain conditions. These results also suggest that semantic representations comprise a less distributed network in high-functioning adults with ASD. Furthermore, these results may be related to the unusually high memory capacities found in some individuals with ASD. Research directed at defining the range of tasks performed superiorly by high-functioning individuals with ASD will be important for optimal vocational rehabilitation.


Subject(s)
Autistic Disorder/psychology , Discrimination Learning , Memory , Adult , Discriminant Analysis , Female , Humans , Male , Paired-Associate Learning , Photic Stimulation , Verbal Learning , Word Association Tests
13.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc ; 6(4): 455-9, 2000 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10902414

ABSTRACT

Whereas the ventral cortical visual stream is important in object recognition, the dorsal stream is specialized for spatial localization. In humans there are also right and left hemisphere asymmetries in visual processing: the left hemisphere being more important in object recognition and the right in specifying spatial locations. Based on these dorsal-ventral and right-left where-what dichotomies, one would expect that the dorsal right hemisphere systems would be most activated during spatial localization tasks, and this activation may induce a leftward spatial bias in lower space. To determine if visual stimuli in upper and lower body space evoke different hemispheric activation, we had 12 normal participants bisect horizontal lines above and below eye level. Participants erred leftward in lower body space relative to upper body space (M = 1.3345 mm and 0.4225 mm, respectively; p = .011). In upper body space, bisection errors did not differ from zero, but in lower body space, errors tended to deviate leftward (M = 1.3345 mm, differs from null hypotheses at p = .0755). Our results are consistent with dorsal stream/right hemisphere activation when performing a spatial localization task in lower versus upper body space.


Subject(s)
Space Perception/physiology , Adult , Attention/physiology , Female , Functional Laterality/physiology , Humans , Male , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Sex Characteristics
14.
Neurology ; 54(6): 1258-64, 2000 Mar 28.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10746595

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: To learn how pAD (probable Alzheimer's disease), PD+ ("Parkinson's Plus" syndrome), and control subjects remember internally generated material under different conditions. BACKGROUND: "Self-discovered," or internally generated knowledge, prized by educators and therapists, can bring about considerable behavioral change. Both parietal-temporal-limbic (pAD) and frontal-subcortical dementia (e.g. PD+) cause dysmemory, but may cause different internal-external memory bias. pAD subjects, confusing internal and external information (confabulation) and reporting internal information during memory testing (intrusions), may be biased to remember internal material. PD+ subjects, impaired at generative tests, may be externally biased. METHODS: Ten pAD, 5 PD+, and 10 control subjects generated words in a category without instruction to remember (INR), and took a list-learning test of incidental memory for internally and externally generated words. To test how INR influences memory, subjects then generated and attempted to recall four more words. RESULTS: All three subject groups remembered more internally generated than externally provided words without INR. Recall versus recognition of internally generated words differed by group, with PD+ subjects showing greatest improvement with recognition. The pAD subjects performed worse with INR than without INR, had the most intrusion errors, and, rather than demonstrating a release from proactive inhibition, recalled fewer words outside the category. Groups differed in overall recall/recognition improvement (p = 0.015). CONCLUSIONS: Aged subjects preferentially retained internally generated material. However, among demented subjects, memory for internally generated words was influenced by the testing method used. PD+ subjects have poor internal recall, but excellent internal recognition. In pAD, memory for internally generated words may exceed external memory, but only when subjects are not explicitly trying to remember.


Subject(s)
Alzheimer Disease/psychology , Dementia/psychology , Memory/physiology , Neuropsychological Tests , Parkinson Disease/psychology , Aged , Alzheimer Disease/physiopathology , Dementia/physiopathology , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Parkinson Disease/physiopathology
15.
Neuropsychologia ; 38(6): 757-67, 2000.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10689051

ABSTRACT

Visual-spatial deficits are often associated with Parkinson's Disease (PD). Recent theories suggest that frontal-basal ganglionic dysfunction affects cognition in PD. Although this hypothesis does not entirely explain spatial deficits in PD, the inappropriate utilization of cues associated with executive dysfunction may induce spatial deficits. Alternatively, the vestibular system is also involved in spatial cognition, and vestibular dysfunction may affect visual-spatial ability in PD. To test these hypotheses, we administered the Water Jar Test, while perturbing vestibulo-proprioceptive input. Non-demented PD patients were significantly less accurate than controls in judging horizontal, and appeared to inappropriately utilize cues. No group effect was found for head tilt. These findings suggest the visual-spatial difficulties seen in PD are related to executive dysfunction that is associated with a disruption of the frontal-basal ganglionic and frontal-parietal systems.


Subject(s)
Cognition Disorders/diagnosis , Cognition Disorders/etiology , Parkinson Disease/complications , Parkinson Disease/physiopathology , Proprioception/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Vestibule, Labyrinth/physiopathology , Basal Ganglia/physiopathology , Cues , Female , Frontal Lobe/physiopathology , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Parietal Lobe/physiopathology
16.
Neuropsychologia ; 38(6): 778-84, 2000.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10689053

ABSTRACT

Studies of animals and humans with focal brain damage suggest that attention in near and far extrapersonal space may be mediated by anatomically separate systems. Thalamic lesions have been associated with spatial neglect, but whether asymmetric attention specific to near or far space occur after thalamic damage has not been explored. It is also unclear if thalamic injury can induce contralesional defective response inhibition. We tested a woman with a left thalamic infarction who reported that, when driving, she had a tendency to veer towards people or objects on the right side of the road. Our patient and four controls performed a line bisection task with a laser pointer in near and far extrapersonal space. The experimenter marked each bisection either from the right of the presented line (right-distractor, RD) or the left (left-distractor, LD). RD and LD trials were pseudo-randomized. Our patient performed similarly to controls (mean -0.7 mm, controls -0.6 mm) on the line bisection task in near space. In far space she erred significantly rightward compared to her performance in near space (p<0.001). Controls performed similarly in near and far space. The experimenter position did not affect our patient's performance on near line bisections, nor did controls demonstrate a distractor effect for the near condition. In the far condition, however, our patient showed a significant distractor effect (LD -3.3 mm, RD 35.3 mm, p<0.001). Controls also demonstrated a distractor effect in the far condition (LD -6.4 mm, RD 0.7 mm, p<0.01), though of much smaller magnitude. Our results suggest that frontal-thalamic systems regulating visual attention may be disrupted by thalamic infarction. Such damage may produce an attentional grasp specific to far extrapersonal space.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Stroke/pathology , Thalamic Diseases/pathology , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Middle Aged , Random Allocation , Stroke/physiopathology , Thalamic Diseases/physiopathology , Thalamus/pathology
17.
Cortex ; 36(5): 623-47, 2000 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11195911

ABSTRACT

The relationship between autonomic-visceral arousal and emotional experience is unclear. The attribution or cognitive-arousal theory of emotional experience posits that emotional experience is dependent on both visceral-autonomic nervous system feedback and the cognitive interpretation of the stimulus that induced this visceral activation. The finding that false cardiac feedback can alter emotional experience suggests that it may be the conscious perception that one is aroused, together with the cognitive interpretation of the stimulus that are important in developing emotional experience. Because the right hemisphere appears to play a special role in modulating arousal and interpreting emotional stimuli, it is possible that right hemisphere damage may interfere with developing the computations needed for emotional experience. To test this hypothesis we exposed men, both neurologically intact and those with right and left hemisphere lesions, to emotionally provocative pictures that were paired with false cardiac feedback, and examined the effects of this false feedback on their ratings of attractiveness of these pictures and their cardiac reactivity to this information. Subjects with left hemisphere damage, but not right hemisphere damage, showed significant changes in their emotional rating whereas control subjects showed marginal reactivity in their emotional ratings. Subjects with left hemisphere damage also showed significant changes in their cardiac reactivity. This finding is consistent with prior reports that indicate, when compared to right hemisphere damaged patients and normal controls, patients with left hemisphere lesions have an increased visceral-autonomic response to stimuli. These findings further provide support for the postulate that it is the cognitive interpretation of perceived physiological arousal together with the cognitive interpretation of the stimulus that is important in the development of emotional judgment and experience. These results do not support the approach-left hemisphere/avoidance-right hemisphere dichotomy, but instead suggest that left hemisphere damage increases reactivity to false feedback, and that the intact right hemisphere function integrates the cognitive interpretation of the emotional information and perceived arousal that lead to that emotional judgment. That these subjects showed no consistent relationship between their measures of cardiac reactivity and their ratings of attractiveness detracts from the James-Lange and attribution theories. These subjects also showed no consistent relationship between their knowledge of affective physiological reactivity and their ratings of attractiveness, or between their knowledge of physiological reactivity and actual measures of cardiac reactivity, suggesting that other neuropsychological factors are involved in making an emotional judgment.


Subject(s)
Cerebral Infarction/physiopathology , Cerebral Infarction/psychology , Emotions , Heart Rate , Aged , Auditory Perception , Discrimination, Psychological , Esthetics , Feedback , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Photic Stimulation/methods , Reference Values
18.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10223259

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Several studies have demonstrated that patients with right hemisphere damage, when compared with left-hemisphere damaged controls, are impaired at comprehending emotional prosody. Critics of these studies, however, note that selection may have been biased because left-hemisphere-damaged subjects had good verbal comprehension. OBJECTIVE: To learn whether a subject with a large left hemisphere stroke and global aphasia could comprehend emotional prosody in spoken material. METHOD: The authors formally tested speech and language with the Western Aphasia Battery and comprehension of emotional prosody and emotional facial expression with the Florida Affect Battery. RESULTS: The patient could not perform verbally mediated tests but demonstrated spared ability to match emotional prosody to emotional facial expressions under a variety of conditions. CONCLUSIONS: These observations further support the idea that verbal and emotional communication systems are independent and mediated by different hemispheres.


Subject(s)
Affect , Aphasia/etiology , Cerebral Infarction/complications , Facial Expression , Speech Perception , Adult , Aphasia/diagnosis , Cerebral Infarction/diagnosis , Cerebral Infarction/physiopathology , Dominance, Cerebral , Female , Functional Laterality , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Neuropsychological Tests , Visual Perception
19.
Arch Phys Med Rehabil ; 80(5): 600-3, 1999 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10326927

ABSTRACT

Studies in animals and humans report dopamine agonists can improve neglect. Because dopamine deficit reduces intention to act, it has been suspected the dopamine agonist bromocriptine would improve deficient hemispatial intention. Thus, the effect of bromocriptine on line bisection was examined in a patient with neglect and failure of the action-intention system. The 58-year-old patient had left-sided neglect from a right cerebral infarction involving both cortical and subcortical (striatal) structures. It was determined that neglect on a line bisection task was attributable to a motor-intentional bias by testing under congruous and incongruous video monitoring. Testing sessions were held before starting bromocriptine, on 20 mg/d, and after stopping bromocriptine. The patient's ipsilesional bias increased on bromocriptine, and improved when bromocriptine was stopped. Bromocriptine may worsen neglect if putamenal receptors are damaged. Dopamine agonists may activate the normal hemisphere, increasing an intentional bias. Clinicians using dopaminergic pharmacotherapy should assess patients for this possible adverse effect.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Brain/drug effects , Bromocriptine/adverse effects , Cerebral Infarction/physiopathology , Cerebral Infarction/rehabilitation , Dopamine Agonists/adverse effects , Motor Skills/physiology , Movement Disorders/physiopathology , Attention/drug effects , Brain/pathology , Bromocriptine/therapeutic use , Cerebral Infarction/complications , Cerebral Infarction/diagnosis , Dopamine Agonists/therapeutic use , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Motor Skills/drug effects , Neuropsychological Tests , Videotape Recording
20.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry ; 66(6): 776-8, 1999 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10329754

ABSTRACT

The effects of dopamine on developmental stuttering was studied in a 44 year old man with developmental stuttering and Parkinson's disease during three levodopa "on" periods and three "off" periods. When compared with the "off" periods, during the "on"' periods he demonstrated an increase of speech dysfluencies. These findings lend support to the dopamine hypothesis of developmental stuttering.


Subject(s)
Levodopa/therapeutic use , Parkinson Disease/drug therapy , Stuttering/drug therapy , Adult , Humans , Male
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL