Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 41
Filter
1.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; : 17470218241238737, 2024 Mar 21.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38426458

ABSTRACT

The current study explores the role of attention in location memory for animals and objects. Participants completed an incidental learning task where they rated animals and objects with regard to either their ease of collection to win a scavenger hunt (Experiments 1a and b) or their distance from the centre of the computer screen (Experiment 2). The images of animals and objects were pseudo-randomly positioned on the screen in both experiments. After completing the incidental learning task (and a reverse counting distractor task), participants were then given a surprise location memory recall task. In the location memory recall task, items were shown in the centre of the screen and participants used the mouse to indicate the position the item had been shown during the incidental encoding task. The results of both experiments show that location memory for objects was more accurate than for animals. While we cannot definitively identify the mechanism responsible for the difference in the location memory of objects and animals, we propose that differences in the influence of object-based attention at encoding affect location memory when tested at recall.

2.
Vision (Basel) ; 7(3)2023 Aug 22.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37756129

ABSTRACT

The visual inspection of scenes is disrupted when participants are forced to begin inspection away from the centre of an image. The present study explored the effect of the starting point on the visual inspection of paintings. Eye movements were recorded while participants viewed paintings for later identification in a yes/no discrimination task. The viewing of each painting was preceded by the presentation of a pseudo-randomly positioned Navon figure. Participants were instructed using a cue to attend to either the local or global level of the Navon figure. Each painting was split into regions of interest (ROIs) defined by face, theme, and context to allow the analysis of eye movements. These data were directly compared with a subset of those initially reported in our previous study in which the same experiment was run but without the inclusion of the Navon figure. The inclusion of the Navon task lowered the discrimination accuracy in the yes/no discrimination task. More importantly, eye movements to the paintings were disrupted across the entire period over which they were viewed and not just in the period following the offset of the Navon figure. The results show the sensitivity of eye movements to the conditions present at the beginning of viewing. The results have implications for the viewing of paintings (and other images) in the real world, where the starting conditions for inspection cannot be controlled.

3.
Acta Psychol (Amst) ; 210: 103173, 2020 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32905872

ABSTRACT

In a typical visual search task, participants search for single targets amongst displays containing non-overlapping objects that are presented on a single depth plane. Recent work has begun to examine displays containing overlapping objects that are presented on different depth planes to one another. It has been found that searching displays containing depth improves response accuracy by making participants more likely to fixate targets and to identify targets after fixating them. Here we extended this previous research by seeking first of all to replicate the previous pattern of results, and then to determine whether extensive training using depth in search transfers to two-dimensional displays. We provided participants with sixteen sessions of training with displays containing transparent overlapping objects presented in depth, and found a similar pattern of results to our previous study. We also found evidence that some performance improvements from the depth training transferred to search of two-dimensional displays that did not contain depth. Further examinations revealed that participants learn to search more exhaustively (i.e., search for longer) in displays containing depth. We conclude that depth does influence search performance but the influences depend very much on the stimuli and the degree of overlap within them.


Subject(s)
Attention , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Humans , Learning
4.
PLoS One ; 14(12): e0225819, 2019.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31851679

ABSTRACT

Participants' eye movements (EMs) and EEG signal were simultaneously recorded to examine foveal and parafoveal processing during sentence reading. All the words in the sentence were manipulated for inter-word spacing (intact spaces vs. spaces replaced by a random letter) and parafoveal preview (identical preview vs. random letter string preview). We observed disruption for unspaced text and invalid preview conditions in both EMs and fixation-related potentials (FRPs). Unspaced and invalid preview conditions received longer reading times than spaced and valid preview conditions. In addition, the FRP data showed that unspaced previews disrupted reading in earlier time windows of analysis, compared to string preview conditions. Moreover, the effect of parafoveal preview was greater for spaced relative to unspaced conditions, in both EMs and FRPs. These findings replicate well-established preview effects, provide novel insight into the neural correlates of reading with and without inter-word spacing and suggest that spatial selection precedes lexical processing.


Subject(s)
Fixation, Ocular/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Reading , Adolescent , Adult , Attention , Female , Healthy Volunteers , Humans , Macula Lutea/physiology , Male , Time Factors , Young Adult
5.
Vision (Basel) ; 3(2)2019 Jun 04.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31735825

ABSTRACT

There has been an increasing drive to understand failures in searches for weapons and explosives in X-ray baggage screening. Tracking eye movements during the search has produced new insights into the guidance of attention during the search, and the identification of targets once they are fixated. Here, we review the eye-movement literature that has emerged on this front over the last fifteen years, including a discussion of the problems that real-world searchers face when trying to detect targets that could do serious harm to people and infrastructure.

6.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 45(7): 911-935, 2019 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30985178

ABSTRACT

Searching for two targets produces a dual-target cost compared with single-target search, with reduced attentional guidance toward targets (Stroud, Menneer, Cave, & Donnelly, 2012). We explore the effect of holding a color in working memory (WM) on guidance in single-target search. In Experiments 1 and 2, participants searched for a T of a specific color while holding one of the following in WM: a color patch, a letter, a dot pattern, or an oriented bar. Only when holding a color in WM was guidance in single-target search affected as strongly as it is in dual-target search. In Experiment 3, the target changed color from trial to trial. A color in WM reduced guidance, but not to the extent of dual-target search. However, search and WM error rates were high, suggesting interference and incomplete engagement with the combined task. We conclude that the guidance cost in dual-target search is not solely due to attentional capture by the WM-color, because the WM-color can be effectively separated from search color, with little confusion between the two. However, WM load does cause substantial interference in guidance when both tasks involve color. These results illustrate the complex interactions between WM and attentional guidance. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved).


Subject(s)
Attention , Memory, Short-Term , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Adolescent , Adult , Color , Female , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation , Young Adult
7.
J Exp Psychol Gen ; 148(3): 453-474, 2019 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30335444

ABSTRACT

Participants' eye movements and electroencephalogram (EEG) signal were recorded as they read sentences displayed according to the gaze-contingent boundary paradigm. Two target words in each sentence were manipulated for lexical frequency (high vs. low frequency) and parafoveal preview of each target word (identical vs. string of random letters vs. string of Xs). Eye movement data revealed visual parafoveal-on-foveal (PoF) effects, as well as foveal visual and orthographic preview effects and word frequency effects. Fixation-related potentials (FRPs) showed visual and orthographic PoF effects as well as foveal visual and orthographic preview effects. Our results replicated the early preview positivity effect (Dimigen, Kliegl, & Sommer, 2012) in the X-string preview condition, and revealed different neural correlates associated with a preview comprised of a string of random letters relative to a string of Xs. The former effects seem likely to reflect difficulty associated with the integration of parafoveal and foveal information, as well as feature overlap, while the latter reflect inhibition, and potentially disruption, to processing underlying reading. Interestingly, and consistent with Kretzschmar, Schlesewsky, and Staub (2015), no frequency effect was reflected in the FRP measures. The findings provide insight into the neural correlates of parafoveal processing and written word recognition in reading and demonstrate the value of utilizing ecologically valid paradigms to study well established phenomena that occur as text is read naturally. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved).


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Eye Movements/physiology , Fovea Centralis/physiology , Reading , Adolescent , Adult , Electroencephalography , Female , Fixation, Ocular/physiology , Humans , Male , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Young Adult
8.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 81(2): 377-406, 2019 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30402735

ABSTRACT

For some real-world color searches, the target colors are not precisely known, and any item within a range of color values should be attended. Thus, a target representation that captures multiple similar colors would be advantageous. If such a multicolor search is possible, then search for two targets (e.g., Stroud, Menneer, Cave, and Donnelly, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38(1): 113-122, 2012) might be guided by a target representation that included the target colors as well as the continuum of colors that fall between the targets within a contiguous region in color space. Results from Stroud, Menneer, Cave, and Donnelly, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38(1): 113-122, (2012) suggest otherwise, however. The current set of experiments show that guidance for a set of colors that are all from a single region of color space can be reasonably effective if targets are depicted as specific discrete colors. Specifically, Experiments 1-3 demonstrate that a search can be guided by four and even eight colors given the appropriate conditions. However, Experiment 5 gives evidence that guidance is sometimes sensitive to how informative the target preview is to search. Experiments 6 and 7 show that a stimulus showing a continuous range of target colors is not translated into a search target representation. Thus, search can be guided by multiple discrete colors that are from a single region in color space, but this approach was not adopted in a search for two targets with intervening distractor colors.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Color Perception/physiology , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Color , Eye Movements/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Reaction Time , Young Adult
9.
J Exp Psychol Appl ; 24(4): 564-577, 2018 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29389162

ABSTRACT

Many real-world tasks now involve monitoring visual representations of data that change dynamically over time. Monitoring dynamically changing displays for the onset of targets can be done in two ways: detecting targets directly, post-onset, or predicting their onset from the prior state of distractors. In the present study, participants' eye movements were measured as they monitored arrays of 108 colored squares whose colors changed systematically over time. Across three experiments, the data show that participants detected the onset of targets both directly and predictively. Experiments 1 and 2 showed that predictive detection was only possible when supported by sequential color changes that followed a scale ordered in color space. Experiment 3 included measures of individual differences in working memory capacity (WMC) and anxious affect and a manipulation of target prevalence in the search task. It found that predictive monitoring for targets, and decisions about target onsets, were influenced by interactions between individual differences in verbal and spatial WMC and intolerance of uncertainty, a characteristic that reflects worry about uncertain future events. The results have implications for the selection of individuals tasked with monitoring dynamic visual displays for target onsets. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2018 APA, all rights reserved).


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Eye Movements/physiology , Individuality , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Uncertainty , Adult , Anxiety/psychology , Color Perception/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation , Reaction Time/physiology , Young Adult
10.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; : 1-29, 2017 Aug 31.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28856981

ABSTRACT

Previous research shows that visual search for two different targets is less efficient than search for a single target. Stroud, Menneer, Cave and Donnelly (2012) concluded that two target colours are represented separately based on modeling the fixation patterns. Although those analyses provide evidence for two separate target representations, they do not show whether participants search simultaneously for both targets, or first search for one target and then the other. Some studies suggest that multiple target representations are simultaneously active, while others indicate that search can be voluntarily simultaneous, or switching, or a mixture of both. Stroud et al.'s participants were not explicitly instructed to use any particular strategy. These data were revisited to determine which strategy was employed. Each fixated item was categorised according to whether its colour was more similar to one target or the other. Once an item similar to one target is fixated, the next fixated item is more likely to be similar to that target than the other, showing that at a given moment during search, one target is generally favoured. However, the search for one target is not completed before search for the other begins. Instead, there are often short runs of one or two fixations to distractors similar to one target, with each run followed by a switch to the other target. Thus, the results suggest that one target is more highly weighted than the other at any given time, but not to the extent that search is purely successive.

11.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 43(8): 1504-1519, 2017 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28368160

ABSTRACT

The efficiency of visual search for one (single-target) and either of two (dual-target) unfamiliar faces was explored to understand the manifestations of capacity and guidance limitations in face search. The visual similarity of distractor faces to target faces was manipulated using morphing (Experiments 1 and 2) and multidimensional scaling (Experiment 3). A dual-target cost was found in all experiments, evidenced by slower and less accurate search in dual- than single-target conditions. The dual-target cost was unequal across the targets, with performance being maintained on one target and reduced on the other, which we label "preferred" and "non-preferred" respectively. We calculated the capacity for each target face and show reduced capacity for representing the non-preferred target face. However, results show that the capacity for the non-preferred target can be increased when the dual-target condition is conducted after participants complete the single-target conditions. Analyses of eye movements revealed evidence for weak guidance of fixations in single-target search, and when searching for the preferred target in dual-target search. Overall, the experiments show dual-target search for faces is capacity- and guidance-limited, leading to superior search for 1 face over the other in dual-target search. However, learning faces individually may improve capacity with the second face. (PsycINFO Database Record


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Facial Recognition/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Adult , Eye Movement Measurements , Humans , Male , Young Adult
12.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 43(8): 1532-1549, 2017 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28383964

ABSTRACT

Standard models of visual search have focused upon asking participants to search for a single target in displays where the objects do not overlap one another, and where the objects are presented on a single depth plane. This stands in contrast to many everyday visual searches wherein variations in overlap and depth are the norm, rather than the exception. Here, we addressed whether presenting overlapping objects on different depths planes to one another can improve search performance. Across 4 different experiments using different stimulus types (opaque polygons, transparent polygons, opaque real-world objects, and transparent X-ray images), we found that depth was primarily beneficial when the displays were transparent, and this benefit arose in terms of an increase in response accuracy. Although the benefit to search performance only appeared in some cases, across all stimulus types, we found evidence of marked shifts in eye-movement behavior. Our results have important implications for current models and theories of visual search, which have not yet provided detailed accounts of the effects that overlap and depth have on guidance and object identification processes. Moreover, our results show that the presence of depth information could aid real-world searches of complex, overlapping displays. (PsycINFO Database Record


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Depth Perception/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Adult , Eye Movement Measurements , Humans , Young Adult
13.
Cogn Res Princ Implic ; 2(1): 14, 2017.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28275707

ABSTRACT

A number of real-world search tasks (i.e. police search, detection of improvised explosive devices (IEDs)) require searchers to search exhaustively across open ground. In the present study, we simulated this problem by asking individuals (Experiments 1a and 1b) and dyads (Experiment 2) to search for coin targets pseudo-randomly located in a bounded area of open grassland terrain. In Experiment 1a, accuracy, search time, and the route used to search an area were measured. Participants tended to use an 'S'-shaped pattern with a common width of search lane. Increased accuracy was associated with slower, but also variable, search speed, though only when participants moved along the length (as opposed to across the width) of the search area. Experiment 1b varied the number of targets available within the bounded search area and in doing so varied target prevalence and density. The results confirmed that the route taken in Experiment 1a generalizes across variations in target prevalence/density. In Experiment 2, accuracy, search time, and the search strategy used by dyads was measured. While dyads were more accurate than individuals, dyads that opted to conduct two independent searches were more accurate than those who opted to split the search space. The implications of these results for individuals and dyads when searching for targets in open space are discussed.

14.
Behav Brain Sci ; 40: e147, 2017 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29342609

ABSTRACT

The Functional Visual Field (FVF) offers explanatory power. To us, it relates to existing literature on the flexibility of attentional focus in visual search and reading (Eriksen & St. James 1986; McConkie & Rayner 1975). The target article promotes reflection on existing findings. Here we consider the FVF as a mechanism in the Prevalence Effect (PE) in visual search.


Subject(s)
Attention , Visual Fields , Prevalence
15.
Exp Psychol ; 63(4): 215-236, 2016 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27750517

ABSTRACT

Previous work showed that prior experience with discriminations requiring configural solutions (e.g., biconditional discrimination) confers an advantage for the learning of new configural discriminations (e.g., negative patterning) in comparison to prior experience with elemental discriminations. This effect is well established but its mechanism is not well understood. In the studies described below we assessed whether the saliences of configural and element cues were affected by prior training. We observed positive transfer to a new configural discrimination after configural pre-training but we were unable to find evidence for changes in cue salience using a signal-detection task. Our results confirm previous work by demonstrating experience-dependent flexibility in cue processing but they also suggest that this flexibility occurs at a point in the stimulus processing pipeline later than 1-2 s after the presentation of stimulus inputs. (138 words).


Subject(s)
Discrimination Learning/physiology , Association Learning , Cues , Female , Humans , Male , Signal Detection, Psychological , Transfer, Psychology , Young Adult
16.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 22(2): 445-52, 2015 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25102929

ABSTRACT

The Thatcher illusion (Thompson in Perception, 9, 483-484, 1980) is often explained as resulting from recognising a distortion of configural information when 'Thatcherised' faces are upright but not when inverted. However, recent behavioural studies suggest that there is an absence of perceptual configurality in upright Thatcherised faces (Donnelly et al. in Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 74, 1475-1487, 2012) and both perceptual and decisional sources of configurality in behavioural tasks with Thatcherised stimuli (Mestry, Menneer et al. in Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 456, 2012). To examine sources linked to the behavioural experience of the illusion, we studied inversion and Thatcherisation of faces (comparing across conditions in which no features, the eyes, the mouth, or both features were Thatcherised) on a set of event-related potential (ERP) components. Effects of inversion were found at the N170, P2 and P3b. Effects of eye condition were restricted to the N170 generated in the right hemisphere. Critically, an interaction of orientation and eye Thatcherisation was found for the P3b amplitude. Results from an individual with acquired prosopagnosia who can discriminate Thatcherised from typical faces but cannot categorise them or perceive the illusion (Mestry, Donnelly et al. in Neuropsychologia, 50, 3410-3418, 2012) only differed from typical participants at the P3b component. Findings suggest the P3b links most directly to the experience of the illusion. Overall, the study showed evidence consistent with both perceptual and decisional sources and the need to consider both in relation to configurality.


Subject(s)
Electroencephalography , Evoked Potentials, Visual/physiology , Facial Recognition/physiology , Optical Illusions/physiology , Orientation/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Perceptual Distortion/physiology , Attention/physiology , Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Dominance, Cerebral/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Prosopagnosia/physiopathology , Psychophysics , Young Adult
17.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 77(1): 150-9, 2015 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25214305

ABSTRACT

Previous research has shown that during visual search tasks target prevalence (the proportion of trials in which a target appears) influences both the probability that a target will be detected, and the speed at which participants will quit searching and provide an 'absent' response. When prevalence is low (e.g., target presented on 2 % of trials), participants are less likely to detect the target than when prevalence is higher (e.g., 50 % of trials). In the present set of experiments, we examined perceptual failures to detect low prevalence targets in visual search. We used a relative prevalence search task in order to be able to present an overall 50 % target prevalence and thereby prevent the results being accounted for by early quitting behavior. Participants searched for two targets, one of which appeared on 45 % of trials and another that appeared on 5 % of trials, leaving overall target prevalence at 50 %. In the first experiment, participants searched for two dissimilar targets; in the second experiment, participants searched for two similar targets. Overall, the results supported the notion that a reduction in prevalence primarily influenced perceptual failures of identification, rather than of selection. Together, these experiments add to a growing body of research exploring how and why observers fail to detect low prevalence targets, especially in real-world tasks in which some targets are more likely to appear than others.


Subject(s)
Recognition, Psychology/physiology , Cues , Eye Movements/physiology , Fixation, Ocular/physiology , Form Perception/physiology , Humans , Perceptual Masking/physiology , Probability , Reaction Time/physiology , Young Adult
18.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 22(2): 469-75, 2015 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25023956

ABSTRACT

The proportion of trials on which a target is presented (referred to as the target prevalence) during visual search influences the probability that the target will be detected. As prevalence increases, participants become biased toward reporting that the target is present. This bias results in an increase in detection rates for the target, coupled with an increased likelihood of making a false alarm. Previous work has demonstrated that, as prevalence increases, participants spend an increasing period of time searching on target-absent trials. The goal of the present study was to determine the information processing during the additional time spent searching on target-absent trials as prevalence increased. We recorded participants' eye movement behavior as they were engaged in low-prevalence (25% target-present trials), medium-prevalence (50%), or high-prevalence (75%) search. Increased prevalence primarily influenced search by increasing the time spent examining objects in the display, rather than by increasing the proportion of objects examined in each display. In addition, the additional time spent examining objects in high-prevalence target-absent trials was the result of revisiting objects. We discuss the implications of these results in relation to current models of search as well as ongoing efforts to alleviate the prevalence effect.


Subject(s)
Attention , Mental Processes , Motion Perception , Orientation , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Adolescent , Adult , Discrimination, Psychological , Eye Movements , Female , Humans , Male , Prevalence , Probability , Reaction Time , Young Adult
19.
Clin Psychol Rev ; 34(1): 1-13, 2014 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24286750

ABSTRACT

Theoretical frameworks of anxiety propose that attentional biases to threat-related stimuli cause or maintain anxious states. The current paper draws on theoretical frameworks and key empirical studies to outline the distinctive attentional processes highlighted as being important in understanding anxiety. We develop a conceptual framework to make a distinction between two attentional biases: selective attention to threat and hypervigilance for threat. We suggest that these biases each have a different purpose and can account for the typical patterns of facilitated and impaired attention evident in anxious individuals. The framework is novel in its specification of the eye movement behavior associated with these attentional biases. We highlight that selective attention involves narrowing overt attention onto threat to ensure that these stimuli receive processing priority, leading to rapid engagement with task-relevant threat and delayed disengagement from task-irrelevant threat. We show that hypervigilance operates in the presence and absence of threat and involves monitoring for potential dangers via attentional broadening or excessive scanning of the environment with numerous eye movements, leading to improved threat detection and increased distraction from task-irrelevant threat. We conclude that future research could usefully employ eye movement measures to more clearly understand the diverse roles of attention in anxiety.


Subject(s)
Anxiety/psychology , Attention/physiology , Eye Movements/physiology , Fear/psychology , Anxiety/physiopathology , Fear/physiology , Humans , Reaction Time/physiology
20.
Otol Neurotol ; 34(7): 1291-8, 2013 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23921933

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: Rehabilitation of hearing is complicated in patients with profound bilateral hearing loss in the presence of sporadic vestibular schwannoma (VS) or neurofibromatosis 2 (NF2), especially if the tumor does not need to be removed. We present the outcome of patients who have had a cochlear implant in the tumor affected ear without removal of the primary tumor. DESIGN: This is a retrospective multicentre study investigating outcomes of cochlear implantation in profoundly deaf patients with vestibular schwannoma in the implanted ear. MATERIALS AND METHODS: Out of 11 implanted patients, 5 required no treatment for their tumor, whereas 6 had previously undergone radiotherapy. Nine patients experienced NF2, and 2 had unilateral VS in the only hearing ear. Postoperative hearing was assessed with open and closed set speech discrimination, including City University of New York (CUNY) in noise and Bamford, Kowal and Bench (BKB) sentence scores. RESULTS: Patients with untreated lesions experienced marked improvement in their BKB and CUNY scores in the implanted ear and were daily cochlear implant users. The improvement was less consistent in the patients who had radiotherapy where only 1 patient attained open set speech discrimination. CONCLUSION: Patients with unilateral VS (sporadic or those affected with NF2) whose tumor status was stable, benefited from cochlear implantation in their tumor-affected ear. Patients who had radiotherapy also benefited from CI, but their outcomes were variable.


Subject(s)
Cochlear Implants , Deafness/etiology , Deafness/rehabilitation , Neuroma, Acoustic/complications , Adolescent , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Audiometry, Pure-Tone , Data Interpretation, Statistical , Female , Functional Laterality/physiology , Hearing , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Neurofibromatosis 2/complications , Neuroma, Acoustic/pathology , Neuroma, Acoustic/therapy , Patient Care Team , Retrospective Studies , Speech Discrimination Tests , Treatment Outcome , Young Adult
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...