Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 14 de 14
Filter
Add more filters











Publication year range
1.
PLoS One ; 18(12): e0292953, 2023.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38055650

ABSTRACT

In three studies, we tested whether hierarchical preferences could explain differences in punishment recommendations for sexual harassment. Building on research that suggests punishment is used to regulate social hierarchies, we argue that individuals who are motivated to maintain existing hierarchies will treat male perpetrators of sexual harassment with greater leniency, especially when judging perpetrators of high social status. Conversely, we predict that egalitarians-who are motivated to reduce group-based hierarchies-will judge male perpetrators more harshly, especially those of high social status. Given competing theories in the existing literature, we make no predictions about how perpetrator status will affect punishment recommendations overall. Supporting our hypotheses, we found that individuals high on gender system justification and social dominance orientation recommended more lenient punishments to perpetrators. Moreover, an integrative data analysis uncovered an interaction between social dominance orientation and perpetrator status. This interaction was primarily driven by egalitarians, who provided more lenient punishment recommendations to low status perpetrators when compared to high status perpetrators. Contrary to our predictions, we did not find strong evidence that individuals high on social dominance orientation provided harsher judgements to low status perpetrators. Nor did we find strong evidence for a main effect of perpetrator status on punishment recommendations. Taken together, these findings suggest that people punish sexual harassment to bolster or attenuate power structures. This is particularly true of egalitarians, whose emphasis on social equality leads them to judge high status perpetrators of sexual harassment with particular severity.


Subject(s)
Sexual Harassment , Humans , Male , Hierarchy, Social , Attitude , Gender Identity , Social Dominance
2.
Sci Rep ; 13(1): 5266, 2023 03 31.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37002316

ABSTRACT

Online misogyny has become a fixture in female politicians' lives. Backlash theory suggests that it may represent a threat response prompted by female politicians' counterstereotypical, power-seeking behaviors. We investigated this hypothesis by analyzing Twitter references to Hillary Clinton before, during, and after her presidential campaign. We collected a corpus of over 9 million tweets from 2014 to 2018 that referred to Hillary Clinton, and employed an interrupted time series analysis on the relative frequency of misogynistic language within the corpus. Prior to 2015, the level of misogyny associated with Clinton decreased over time, but this trend reversed when she announced her presidential campaign. During the campaign, misogyny steadily increased and only plateaued after the election, when the threat of her electoral success had subsided. These findings are consistent with the notion that online misogyny towards female political nominees is a form of backlash prompted by their ambition for power in the political arena.


Subject(s)
Social Media , Humans , Female , Politics , Language , Administrative Personnel , Interrupted Time Series Analysis
3.
Behav Brain Sci ; 45: e94, 2022 05 13.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35550026

ABSTRACT

Cesario argues that experimental studies of bias tell us little about why group disparities exist. We argue that Cesario's alternative approach implicitly frames understanding of group disparities as a false binary between "bias" and "group differences." This, we suggest, will contribute little to our understanding of the complex dynamics that produce group disparities, and risks inappropriately rationalizing them.


Subject(s)
Bias , Humans
4.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34872970

ABSTRACT

Investigations of sex differences in the human brain take place on politically sensitive terrain. While some scholars express concern that gendered biases and stereotypes remain embedded in scientific research, others are alarmed about the politicization of science. To help better understand these debates, this review sets out three kinds of conflicts that can arise in the neuroscience of sex differences: academic freedom versus gender equality; frameworks, background assumptions, and dominant methodologies; and inductive risk and social values. The boundaries between fair criticism and politicization are explored for each kind of conflict, pointing to ways in which the academic community can facilitate fair criticism while protecting against politicization.


Subject(s)
Brain , Sex Characteristics , Female , Humans , Male
5.
Lancet ; 393(10171): 515-516, 2019 02 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30739677
6.
PLoS One ; 13(7): e0200921, 2018.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30040839

ABSTRACT

It has been argued that gender essentialism impedes progress towards greater gender equality. Here we present a new gender essentialism scale (GES), and validate it in two large nationally representative samples from Denmark and Australia. In both samples the GES was highly reliable and predicted lack of support for sex-role egalitarianism and support for gender discrimination, as well as perceived fairness of gender-based treatment in the Australian sample, independently of two established predictors (i.e., social dominance orientation and conservative political orientation). In addition, gender essentialism assessed by the GES moderated some manifestations of the backlash effect: high essentialists were more likely to respond negatively towards a power-seeking female political candidate relative to a male candidate. Given the implications for possible workplace interventions, further work could usefully explore whether gender essentialism moderates other well-established forms of gender bias.


Subject(s)
Gender Identity , Public Opinion , Socioeconomic Factors , Female , Humans , Male , Politics , Sexism
7.
Trends Cogn Sci ; 21(9): 666-673, 2017 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28821346

ABSTRACT

Common understanding of human sex-linked behaviors is that proximal mechanisms of genetic and hormonal sex, ultimately shaped by the differential reproductive challenges of ancestral males and females, act on the brain to transfer sex-linked predispositions across generations. Here, we extend the debate on the role of nature and nurture in the development of traits in the lifetime of an individual, to their role in the cross-generation transfer of traits. Advances in evolutionary theory that posit the environment as a source of trans-generational stability, and new understanding of sex effects on the brain, suggest that the cross-generation stability of sex-linked patterns of behavior are sometimes better explained in terms of inherited socioenvironmental conditions, with biological sex fostering intrageneration variability.


Subject(s)
Biological Evolution , Sexual Behavior , Social Environment , Female , Humans , Male
8.
Sci Am ; 317(3): 32-37, 2017 Aug 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28813396
10.
Science ; 346(6212): 915-6, 2014 Nov 21.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25414288
11.
Front Hum Neurosci ; 8: 650, 2014.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25221493

ABSTRACT

Neuroimaging (NI) technologies are having increasing impact in the study of complex cognitive and social processes. In this emerging field of social cognitive neuroscience, a central goal should be to increase the understanding of the interaction between the neurobiology of the individual and the environment in which humans develop and function. The study of sex/gender is often a focus for NI research, and may be motivated by a desire to better understand general developmental principles, mental health problems that show female-male disparities, and gendered differences in society. In order to ensure the maximum possible contribution of NI research to these goals, we draw attention to four key principles-overlap, mosaicism, contingency and entanglement-that have emerged from sex/gender research and that should inform NI research design, analysis and interpretation. We discuss the implications of these principles in the form of constructive guidelines and suggestions for researchers, editors, reviewers and science communicators.

12.
Trends Cogn Sci ; 17(11): 550-1, 2013 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24176517

ABSTRACT

Why is popular understanding of female­male differences still based on rigid models of development, even though contemporary developmental sciences emphasize plasticity? Is it because the science of sex differences still works from the same rigid models?


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Neuronal Plasticity/physiology , Sex Characteristics , Female , Humans , Male
13.
Cogn Neuropsychiatry ; 12(1): 46-77, 2007 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17162446

ABSTRACT

INTRODUCTION: There is substantial evidence that patients with delusions exhibit a reasoning bias--known as the "jumping to conclusions" (JTC) bias--which leads them to accept hypotheses as correct on the basis of less evidence than controls. We address three questions concerning the JTC bias that require clarification. Firstly, what is the best measure of the JTC bias? Second, is the JTC bias correlated specifically with delusions, or only with the symptomatology of schizophrenia? And third, is the bias enhanced by emotionally salient material? METHODS: To address these questions, we conducted a series of meta-analyses of studies that used the Beads task to compare the probabilistic reasoning styles of individuals with and without delusions. RESULTS: We found that only one of four measures of the JTC bias--"draws to decision"--reached significance. The JTC bias exhibited by delusional subjects-as measured by draws to decision--did not appear to be solely an epiphenomenal effect of schizophrenic symptomatology, and was not amplified by emotionally salient material. CONCLUSIONS: A tendency to gather less evidence in the Beads task is reliably associated with the presence of delusional symptomatology. In contrast, certainty on the task, and responses to contradictory evidence, do not discriminate well between those with and without delusions. The implications for the underlying basis of the JTC bias, and its role in the formation and maintenance of delusions, are discussed.


Subject(s)
Bias , Decision Making/physiology , Delusions/psychology , Emotions , Imagination/physiology , Delusions/etiology , Humans , Mental Disorders/complications
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL