Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 42
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
Nat Commun ; 15(1): 5348, 2024 Jun 24.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38914556

ABSTRACT

Quantum fluctuations in low-dimensional systems and near quantum phase transitions have significant influences on material properties. Yet, it is difficult to experimentally gauge the strength and importance of quantum fluctuations. Here we provide a resonant inelastic x-ray scattering study of magnon excitations in Mott insulating cuprates. From the thin film of SrCuO2, single- and bi-magnon dispersions are derived. Using an effective Heisenberg Hamiltonian generated from the Hubbard model, we show that the single-magnon dispersion is only described satisfactorily when including significant quantum corrections stemming from magnon-magnon interactions. Comparative results on La2CuO4 indicate that quantum fluctuations are much stronger in SrCuO2 suggesting closer proximity to a magnetic quantum critical point. Monte Carlo calculations reveal that other magnetic orders may compete with the antiferromagnetic Néel order as the ground state. Our results indicate that SrCuO2-due to strong quantum fluctuations-is a unique starting point for the exploration of novel magnetic ground states.

2.
PLoS One ; 18(6): e0288038, 2023.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37384780

ABSTRACT

People respond faster to smaller numbers in their left space and to larger numbers in their right space. Here we argue that movements in space contribute to the formation of spatial-numerical associations (SNAs). We studied the impact of continuous isometric forces along the horizontal or vertical cardinal axes on SNAs while participants performed random number production and arithmetic verification tasks. Our results suggest that such isometric directional force do not suffice to induce SNAs.

3.
Acta Psychol (Amst) ; 231: 103791, 2022 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36370674

ABSTRACT

In numerical processing, the functional role of Spatial-Numerical Associations (SNAs, such as the association of smaller numbers with left space and larger numbers with right space, the Mental Number Line hypothesis) is debated. Most studies demonstrate SNAs with lateralized responses, and there is little evidence that SNAs appear when no response is required. We recorded passive holding grip forces in no-go trials during number processing. In Experiment 1, participants performed a surface numerical decision task ("Is it a number or a letter?"). In Experiment 2, we used a deeper semantic task ("Is this number larger or smaller than five?"). Despite instruction to keep their grip force constant, participants' spontaneous grip force changed in both experiments: Smaller numbers led to larger force increase in the left than in the right hand in the numerical decision task (500-700 ms after stimulus onset). In the semantic task, smaller numbers again led to larger force increase in the left hand, and larger numbers increased the right-hand holding force. This effect appeared earlier (180 ms) and lasted longer (until 580 ms after stimulus onset). This is the first demonstration of SNAs with passive holding force. Our result suggests that (1) explicit motor response is not a prerequisite for SNAs to appear, and (2) the timing and strength of SNAs are task-dependent. (216 words).


Subject(s)
Hand Strength , Space Perception , Humans , Space Perception/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Semantics , Hand
4.
Sci Rep ; 12(1): 12777, 2022 07 27.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35896701

ABSTRACT

Peripersonal space is the space surrounding our body, where multisensory integration of stimuli and action execution take place. The size of peripersonal space is flexible and subject to change by various personal and situational factors. The dynamic representation of our peripersonal space modulates our spatial behaviors towards other individuals. During the COVID-19 pandemic, this spatial behavior was modified by two further factors: social distancing and wearing a face mask. Evidence from offline and online studies on the impact of a face mask on pro-social behavior is mixed. In an attempt to clarify the role of face masks as pro-social or anti-social signals, 235 observers participated in the present online study. They watched pictures of two models standing at three different distances from each other (50, 90 and 150 cm), who were either wearing a face mask or not and were either interacting by initiating a hand shake or just standing still. The observers' task was to classify the model by gender. Our results show that observers react fastest, and therefore show least avoidance, for the shortest distances (50 and 90 cm) but only when models wear a face mask and do not interact. Thus, our results document both pro- and anti-social consequences of face masks as a result of the complex interplay between social distancing and interactive behavior. Practical implications of these findings are discussed.


Subject(s)
COVID-19 , COVID-19/epidemiology , COVID-19/prevention & control , Humans , Masks , Pandemics/prevention & control , Personal Space , Physical Distancing
5.
Phys Rev Lett ; 128(20): 207002, 2022 May 20.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35657867

ABSTRACT

In the cuprates, high-temperature superconductivity, spin-density-wave order, and charge-density-wave (CDW) order are intertwined, and symmetry determination is challenging due to domain formation. We investigated the CDW in the prototypical cuprate La_{1.88}Sr_{0.12}CuO_{4} via x-ray diffraction employing uniaxial pressure as a domain-selective stimulus to establish the unidirectional nature of the CDW unambiguously. A fivefold enhancement of the CDW amplitude is found when homogeneous superconductivity is partially suppressed by magnetic field. This field-induced state provides an ideal search environment for a putative pair-density-wave state.

6.
Nat Commun ; 13(1): 1795, 2022 Apr 04.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35379813

ABSTRACT

Static stripe order is detrimental to superconductivity. Yet, it has been proposed that transverse stripe fluctuations may enhance the inter-stripe Josephson coupling and thus promote superconductivity. Direct experimental studies of stripe dynamics, however, remain difficult. From a strong-coupling perspective, transverse stripe fluctuations are realized in the form of dynamic "kinks"-sideways shifting stripe sections. Here, we show how modest uniaxial pressure tuning reorganizes directional kink alignment. Our starting point is La1.88Sr0.12CuO4 where transverse kink ordering results in a rotation of stripe order away from the crystal axis. Application of mild uniaxial pressure changes the ordering pattern and pins the stripe order to the crystal axis. This reordering occurs at a much weaker pressure than that to detwin the stripe domains and suggests a rather weak transverse stripe stiffness. Weak spatial stiffness and transverse quantum fluctuations are likely key prerequisites for stripes to coexist with superconductivity.

7.
Sci Adv ; 6(13): eaax9480, 2020 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32258393

ABSTRACT

Van der Waals materials offer unprecedented control of electronic properties via stacking of different types of two-dimensional materials. A fascinating frontier, largely unexplored, is the stacking of strongly correlated phases of matter. We study 4Hb-TaS2, which naturally realizes an alternating stacking of 1T-TaS2 and 1H-TaS2 structures. The former is a well-known Mott insulator, which has recently been proposed to host a gapless spin-liquid ground state. The latter is a superconductor known to also host a competing charge density wave state. This raises the question of how these two components affect each other when stacked together. We find a superconductor with a T c of 2.7 Kelvin and anomalous properties, of which the most notable one is a signature of time-reversal symmetry breaking, abruptly appearing at the superconducting transition. This observation is consistent with a chiral superconducting state.

8.
Phys Rev Lett ; 120(21): 216801, 2018 May 25.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29883178

ABSTRACT

The topological phases of periodically driven, or Floquet systems, rely on a perfectly periodic modulation of system parameters in time. Even the smallest deviation from periodicity leads to decoherence, causing the boundary (end) states to leak into the system's bulk. Here, we show that in one dimension this decay of topologically protected end states depends fundamentally on the nature of the bulk states: a dispersive bulk results in an exponential decay, while a localized bulk slows the decay down to a diffusive process. The localization can be due to disorder, which remarkably counteracts decoherence even when it breaks the symmetry responsible for the topological protection. We derive this result analytically, using a novel, discrete-time Floquet-Lindblad formalism and confirm our findings with the help of numerical simulations. Our results are particularly relevant for experiments, where disorder can be tailored to protect Floquet topological phases from decoherence.

9.
Neuropsychologia ; 82: 74-83, 2016 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26721762

ABSTRACT

Apraxia typically results from left-hemispheric (LH), but also from right-hemispheric (RH) stroke, and often impairs gesture imitation. Especially in LH stroke, it is important to differentiate apraxia-induced gesture imitation deficits from those due to co-morbid aphasia and associated semantic deficits, possibly influencing the imitation of meaningful (MF) gestures. To explore this issue, we first investigated if the 10 supposedly meaningless (ML) gestures of a widely used finger imitation test really carry no meaning, or if the test also contains MF gestures, by asking healthy subjects (n=45) to classify these gestures as MF or ML. Most healthy subjects (98%) classified three of the 10 gestures as clearly MF. Only two gestures were considered predominantly ML. We next assessed how imitation in stroke patients (255 LH, 113 RH stroke) is influenced by gesture meaning and how aphasia influences imitation of LH stroke patients (n=208). All patients and especially patients with imitation deficits (17% of LH, 27% of RH stroke patients) imitated MF gestures significantly better than ML gestures. Importantly, meaningfulness-scores of all 10 gestures significantly predicted imitation scores of patients with imitation deficits. Furthermore, especially in LH stroke patients with imitation deficits, the severity of aphasia significantly influenced the imitation of MF, but not ML gestures. Our findings in a large patient cohort support current cognitive models of imitation and strongly suggest that ML gestures are particularly sensitive to detect imitation deficits while minimising confounding effects of aphasia which affect the imitation of MF gestures in LH stroke patients.


Subject(s)
Apraxias/psychology , Gestures , Imitative Behavior , Stroke/complications , Aged , Apraxias/etiology , Female , Fingers , Humans , Male , Middle Aged
10.
Nature ; 511(7510): 449-51, 2014 Jul 24.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25056062

ABSTRACT

Magnetic devices are a leading contender for the implementation of memory and logic technologies that are non-volatile, that can scale to high density and high speed, and that do not wear out. However, widespread application of magnetic memory and logic devices will require the development of efficient mechanisms for reorienting their magnetization using the least possible current and power. There has been considerable recent progress in this effort; in particular, it has been discovered that spin-orbit interactions in heavy-metal/ferromagnet bilayers can produce strong current-driven torques on the magnetic layer, via the spin Hall effect in the heavy metal or the Rashba-Edelstein effect in the ferromagnet. In the search for materials to provide even more efficient spin-orbit-induced torques, some proposals have suggested topological insulators, which possess a surface state in which the effects of spin-orbit coupling are maximal in the sense that an electron's spin orientation is fixed relative to its propagation direction. Here we report experiments showing that charge current flowing in-plane in a thin film of the topological insulator bismuth selenide (Bi2Se3) at room temperature can indeed exert a strong spin-transfer torque on an adjacent ferromagnetic permalloy (Ni81Fe19) thin film, with a direction consistent with that expected from the topological surface state. We find that the strength of the torque per unit charge current density in Bi2Se3 is greater than for any source of spin-transfer torque measured so far, even for non-ideal topological insulator films in which the surface states coexist with bulk conduction. Our data suggest that topological insulators could enable very efficient electrical manipulation of magnetic materials at room temperature, for memory and logic applications.

11.
Acta Psychol (Amst) ; 131(3): 209-20, 2009 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19545853

ABSTRACT

Together with magnitude representations, knowledge about multiplicativity and parity contributes to numerical problem solving. In the present study, we used eye tracking to document how and when multiplicativity and parity are recruited in the number bisection task. Fourteen healthy adults evaluated whether the central number of a triplet (e.g., 21_24_27) corresponds to the arithmetic integer mean of the interval defined by the two outer numbers. We observed multiplicativity to specifically affect gaze duration on numbers, indicating that the information of multiplicative relatedness is activated at early processing stages. In contrast, parity only affected total reading time, suggesting involvement in later processing stages. We conclude that different representational features of numbers are available and integrated at different processing stages within the same task and outline a processing model for these temporal dynamics of numerical cognition.


Subject(s)
Eye Movements , Fixation, Ocular , Mathematics , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Problem Solving , Adolescent , Adult , Attention , Color Perception , Female , Humans , Male , Psychomotor Performance , Reaction Time , Young Adult
12.
Neurology ; 57(5): 822-6, 2001 Sep 11.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11552011

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Response speed in parity judgments is faster with the left hand for small numbers (e.g., 1 or 2) and faster with the right hand for larger numbers (e.g., 8 or 9). This effect suggests that number processing can induce systematic spatial biases in bisection tasks. METHOD: Neurologically healthy participants bisected visually presented stimuli with a pencil. Stimuli were long strings of uniform digits (experiment 1) or lines with single digit flankers (experiment 2). RESULTS: Bisection performance was biased to the left of center for strings made of digits 1 or 2 and to the right of center for strings made of digits 8 or 9. Line bisection was biased toward the flanker representing the larger magnitude, regardless of its position. CONCLUSIONS: These results extend previous findings and support the notion of an automatic association of number magnitudes with spatial response codes. The effect may be useful for an assessment of semantic number processing in special populations.


Subject(s)
Photic Stimulation/methods , Psychomotor Performance , Spatial Behavior , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Spatial Behavior/physiology
13.
J Am Coll Nutr ; 20(1): 81-6, 2001 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11293472

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: This study was conducted to determine whether IGF-I concentrations are low in nonambulant profoundly retarded adults and to identify associated nutritional factors. METHODS: Serum IGF-I, albumin, pre-albumin, creatinine, zinc (Zn) and plasma amino acids were measured before and after a four-week 25% increase in formula in 25 individuals, divided into those fed by day (Group A) or by night (Group B). RESULTS: Circulating IGF-I was low in nine of the 22 subjects (40.9%) included in the analysis. Mean IGF-I increased 10.4% (p=0.004). Despite high intakes of essential amino acids and Zn, initial mean plasma tryptophan and phenylalanine were low, and serum Zn was low in 40.9% of subjects. Plasma tryptophan was low at both samplings and correlated with circulating IGF-I concentrations (p=0.02) at the beginning of the study. Serum IGF-I and Zn also correlated (p=0.02) initially. CONCLUSIONS: IGF-I is commonly low in this population and is associated with low plasma amino acid and Zn concentrations, despite high intakes of these nutrients. The causes and clinical implications of these abnormalities need further study.


Subject(s)
Amino Acids/blood , Enteral Nutrition , Insulin-Like Growth Factor I/analysis , Intellectual Disability/blood , Zinc/blood , Adult , Amino Acids/administration & dosage , Circadian Rhythm , Creatinine/blood , Female , Humans , Insulin-Like Growth Factor I/drug effects , Intellectual Disability/metabolism , Male , Nutritional Status , Prealbumin/analysis , Serum Albumin/analysis , Zinc/administration & dosage
14.
Exp Brain Res ; 136(4): 507-13, 2001 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11291731

ABSTRACT

There is a debate about how task-irrelevant visual distractors influence motor responses. Inconsistent findings in previous studies may reflect the use of different spatial layouts. In this study, participants pointed to randomly lateralized targets under full viewing conditions, either with or without a single distractor present. Distractor location, size, and spacing from the target were systematically manipulated while target size and movement amplitude remained constant. Larger distractors facilitated reaction times and slowed movement times. Distant distractors facilitated only reaction times. Movement endpoints were biased away from distractors. The single distractor also modulated perceived target size. These results are discussed in the context of current theories of the role of visual perception for action control.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Movement/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Adult , Humans , Photic Stimulation , Random Allocation , Reaction Time/physiology , Touch/physiology
15.
Neuropsychologia ; 39(6): 586-96, 2001.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11257284

ABSTRACT

To assess attentional, perceptual and motor contributions to pseudoneglect, the landmark task requires pointing to the subjectively shorter end of accurately pre-transected horizontal lines. We presented irrelevant flankers 317, 567 or 967 ms prior to the line transector on a touch screen and recorded reaction times, movement times, landing points and uncertainty of pointing decisions. Flankers were visible for 167 ms to exclude perceptual biases and lines disappeared upon response initiation to prevent subsequent corrections. Healthy participants showed pseudoneglect and a response bias toward the flanker. This finding is interpreted as evidence for inhibition of return and casts doubt on an attentional interpretation of previously reported flanker effects.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Motion Perception/physiology , Visual Fields/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Humans , Inhibition, Psychological , Male , Reaction Time , Surveys and Questionnaires
16.
Brain Cogn ; 45(2): 143-54, 2001 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11237363

ABSTRACT

The Corsi Blocks task (CBT) measures spatial memory span, but methodological differences across previous studies prevent a clear appreciation of perceptual, memory, and motor contributions to spatial span. CBT performance in a standard condition was compared to performance with longer encoding intervals (3 vs 1 s), longer memory intervals (9 vs 1 s), or fewer response alternatives (all nine vs only the relevant positions). All experiments also contrasted ascending vs descending order of item difficulty and recorded response-initiation times. Performance improved with longer encoding and maintenance intervals and with fewer response alternatives. Item order had no reliable effects. Response-initiation times were a useful additional measure of spatial span. Implications of these findings for research using the CBT are discussed.


Subject(s)
Memory/physiology , Neuropsychological Tests , Space Perception/physiology , Humans , Movement/physiology , Reaction Time
17.
Exp Brain Res ; 136(2): 224-30, 2001 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11206284

ABSTRACT

A recent report that hand transport was sensitive to a size-contrast illusion (SCI) implied that the distinction between visual processing for perception versus action might only affect visual information obtained late during reaching. In this study, the presence of a perceptual SCI did not affect reaction time, movement time, or movement amplitude. However, both perception and action became sensitive to the SCI with memory-based responses. It is concluded that the distinction between visual processing for perception versus action does extend to hand transport. Immediate action is entirely based on veridical visuo-motor representations, whereas even slightly delayed actions begin to reflect distorted perceptual representations.


Subject(s)
Hand/physiology , Illusions/physiology , Movement/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Brain/physiology , Hand/innervation , Humans , Neuropsychological Tests , Photic Stimulation
18.
Am J Clin Nutr ; 72(3): 784-9, 2000 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10966900

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: In addition to increasing stool weight, supplements of psyllium seed husk produce stools that are slick and gelatinous. OBJECTIVE: Our purpose was to test the hypothesis that a gel-forming fraction of psyllium escapes microbial fermentation and is responsible for the characteristics that enhance laxation. DESIGN: Fifteen healthy adults consumed controlled diets for two 7-d periods, one of which included 8.8 g dietary fiber provided by 15 g/d of a psyllium seed husk preparation. All stools were collected and evaluated and diet was monitored throughout. RESULTS: Psyllium significantly increased the apparent viscosity of an aqueous stool extract, stool moisture, and wet and dry stool weights. A very viscous fraction, not present in low-fiber stool and containing predominantly 2 sugars that are also found in abundance in psyllium husk, was isolated from psyllium stool. CONCLUSIONS: In contrast with other viscous fibers that are fermented completely in the colon, a component of psyllium is not fermented. This gel provided lubrication that facilitated propulsion of colon contents and produced a stool that was bulkier and more moist than were stools resulting with use of comparable amounts of other bowel-regulating fiber sources.


Subject(s)
Cathartics/pharmacology , Psyllium/pharmacology , Adolescent , Adult , Carbohydrates/analysis , Dietary Fiber/pharmacology , Feces/chemistry , Female , Fermentation , Gels , Humans , Male , Viscosity , Water/analysis
19.
Perception ; 29(3): 337-54, 2000.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10889943

ABSTRACT

Normal readers were asked to mark the middle of visually presented words. They made systematic errors toward the left, indicating an overestimation of the length of the beginning of a word. The number of characters determines the size of this error. The bias extended to pseudowords, letter strings, and symbols, but not to blocks, dashes, and lines. Finally, the bias was sensitive to typographical errors but not to colour cuing. These findings suggest that special cognitive operations determine the perceived spatial extent of words. Implications for our understanding of perceptual and cognitive processes in reading are discussed.


Subject(s)
Optical Illusions , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Reading , Adult , Attention , Cues , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Photic Stimulation/methods
20.
Q J Exp Psychol A ; 52(3): 649-77, 1999 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10504902

ABSTRACT

Allocation of visuo-spatial attention during dynamic viewing was investigated with a dual task. Primary tasks (reading, scanning, searching) all required sequential left-to-right eye movements. An additional speeded manual response was made to a visual probe that appeared early or late after the onset of a randomly determined fixation (25 or 170 msec probe delay). The probe appeared to the left, directly above, or to the right of the currently fixated character (-10, -5, 0, +5, or +10 characters probe eccentricity). Faster probe detection near the location of the forthcoming eye fixation was found in the search task, but not during reading or scanning. Fixation times increased and saccade lengths decreased as a consequence of probing in all three tasks. Fixations were, however, less prolonged when the probe appeared in the right than when it appeared in the left hemifield, and saccades were greatest when the probe appeared at +10 characters. The results extend the notion of goal-directed attention shifts to free viewing and highlight the impact of task-specific processing requirements.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Saccades/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Fixation, Ocular/physiology , Humans , Male , Reaction Time , Reading , Space Perception/physiology
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...