Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 13 de 13
Filter
Add more filters











Publication year range
1.
Front Psychol ; 15: 1323700, 2024.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38328385

ABSTRACT

Traditionally, language processing has been thought of in terms of complete processing of the input. In contrast to this, Ferreira and colleagues put forth the idea of good enough processing. The proposal was that during everyday processing, ambiguities remain unresolved, we rely on heuristics instead of full analyses, and we carry out deep processing only if we need to for the task at hand. This idea has gathered substantial traction since its conception. In the current work, I review the papers that have tested the three key claims of good enough processing: ambiguities remain unresolved and underspecified, we use heuristics to parse sentences, and deep processing is only carried out if required by the task. I find mixed evidence for these claims and conclude with an appeal to further refinement of the claims and predictions of the theory.

2.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; : 17470218231219971, 2024 Jan 21.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38044368

ABSTRACT

Earlier work has explored spoken word production during irrelevant background speech such as intelligible and unintelligible word lists. The present study compared how different types of irrelevant background speech (word lists vs. sentences) influenced spoken word production relative to a quiet control condition, and whether the influence depended on the intelligibility of the background speech. Experiment 1 presented native Dutch speakers with Chinese word lists and sentences. Experiment 2 presented a similar group with Dutch word lists and sentences. In both experiments, the lexical selection demands in speech production were manipulated by varying name agreement (high vs. low) of the to-be-named pictures. Results showed that background speech, regardless of its intelligibility, disrupted spoken word production relative to a quiet condition, but no effects of word lists versus sentences in either language were found. Moreover, the disruption by intelligible background speech compared with the quiet condition was eliminated when planning low name agreement pictures. These findings suggest that any speech, even unintelligible speech, interferes with production, which implies that the disruption of spoken word production is mainly phonological in nature. The disruption by intelligible background speech can be reduced or eliminated via top-down attentional engagement.

3.
Sci Rep ; 13(1): 20037, 2023 11 16.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37973908

ABSTRACT

When encountering people, their faces are usually paired with their voices. We know that if the face looks familiar, and the voice is high-pitched, the first impression will be positive and trustworthy. But, how do we integrate these two multisensory physical attributes? Here, we explore 1) the automaticity of audiovisual integration in shaping first impressions of trustworthiness, and 2) the relative contribution of each modality in the final judgment. We find that, even though participants can focus their attention on one modality to judge trustworthiness, they fail to completely filter out the other modality for both faces (Experiment 1a) and voices (Experiment 1b). When asked to judge the person as a whole, people rely more on voices (Experiment 2) or faces (Experiment 3). We link this change to the distinctiveness of each cue in the stimulus set rather than a general property of the modality. Overall, we find that people weigh faces and voices automatically based on cue saliency when forming trustworthiness impressions.


Subject(s)
Cues , Voice , Humans , Attention , Facial Expression , Physical Examination , Trust
4.
Anim Cogn ; 25(5): 1109-1131, 2022 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36018473

ABSTRACT

Dolphins gain information through echolocation, a publicly accessible sensory system in which dolphins produce clicks and process returning echoes, thereby both investigating and contributing to auditory scenes. How their knowledge of these scenes contributes to their echoic information-seeking is unclear. Here, we investigate their top-down cognitive processes in an echoic matching-to-sample task in which targets and auditory scenes vary in their decipherability and shift from being completely unfamiliar to familiar. A blind-folded adult male dolphin investigated a target sample positioned in front of a hydrophone to allow recording of clicks, a measure of information-seeking and effort; the dolphin received fish for choosing an object identical to the sample from 3 alternatives. We presented 20 three-object sets, unfamiliar in the first five 18-trial sessions with each set. Performance accuracy and click counts varied widely across sets. Click counts of the four lowest-performance-accuracy/low-discriminability sets (X = 41%) and the four highest-performance-accuracy/high-discriminability sets (X = 91%) were similar at the first sessions' starts and then decreased for both kinds of scenes, although the decrease was substantially greater for low-discriminability sets. In four challenging-but-doable sets, number of clicks remained relatively steady across the 5 sessions. Reduced echoic effort with low-discriminability sets was not due to overall motivation: the differential relationship between click number and object-set discriminability was maintained when difficult and easy trials were interleaved and when objects from originally difficult scenes were grouped with more discriminable objects. These data suggest that dolphins calibrate their echoic information-seeking effort based on their knowledge and expectations of auditory scenes.


Subject(s)
Bottle-Nosed Dolphin , Dolphins , Echolocation , Male , Animals , Information Seeking Behavior
5.
Front Psychol ; 13: 892822, 2022.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35664165

ABSTRACT

The cognate effect refers to translation equivalents with similar form between languages-i.e., cognates, such as "band" (English) and "banda" (Spanish)-being processed faster than words with dissimilar forms-such as, "cloud" and "nube." Substantive literature supports this claim, but is mostly based on orthographic similarity and tested in the visual modality. In a previous study, we found an inhibitory orthographic similarity effect in the auditory modality-i.e., greater orthographic similarity led to slower response times and reduced accuracy. The aim of the present study is to explain this effect. In doing so, we explore the role of the speaker's accent in auditory word recognition and whether native accents lead to a mismatch between the participants' phonological representation and the stimulus. Participants carried out a lexical decision task and a typing task in which they spelled out the word they heard. Words were produced by two speakers: one with a native English accent (Standard American) and the other with a non-native accent matching that of the participants (native Spanish speaker from Spain). We manipulated orthographic and phonological similarity orthogonally and found that accent did have some effect on both response time and accuracy as well as modulating the effects of similarity. Overall, the non-native accent improved performance, but it did not fully explain why high orthographic similarity items show an inhibitory effect in the auditory modality. Theoretical implications and future directions are discussed.

6.
Sci Rep ; 11(1): 12812, 2021 06 17.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34140594

ABSTRACT

Language perception studies on bilinguals often show that words that share form and meaning across languages (cognates) are easier to process than words that share only meaning. This facilitatory phenomenon is known as the cognate effect. Most previous studies have shown this effect visually, whereas the auditory modality as well as the interplay between type of similarity and modality remain largely unexplored. In this study, highly proficient late Spanish-English bilinguals carried out a lexical decision task in their second language, both visually and auditorily. Words had high or low phonological and orthographic similarity, fully crossed. We also included orthographically identical words (perfect cognates). Our results suggest that similarity in the same modality (i.e., orthographic similarity in the visual modality and phonological similarity in the auditory modality) leads to improved signal detection, whereas similarity across modalities hinders it. We provide support for the idea that perfect cognates are a special category within cognates. Results suggest a need for a conceptual and practical separation between types of similarity in cognate studies. The theoretical implication is that the representations of items are active in both modalities of the non-target language during language processing, which needs to be incorporated to our current processing models.

7.
PLoS One ; 15(10): e0240252, 2020.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33027296

ABSTRACT

Learning new content and vocabulary in a foreign language can be particularly difficult. Yet, there are educational programs that require people to study in a language they are not native speakers of. For this reason, it is important to understand how these learning processes work and possibly differ from native language learning, as well as to develop strategies to ease this process. The current study takes advantage of emotionality-operationally defined as positive valence and high arousal-to improve memory. In two experiments, the present paper addresses whether participants have more difficulty learning the names of objects they have never seen before in their foreign language and whether embedding them in a positive semantic context can help make learning easier. With this in mind, we had participants (with a minimum of a B2 level of English) in two experiments (43 participants in Experiment 1 and 54 in Experiment 2) read descriptions of made-up objects-either positive or neutral and either in their native or a foreign language. The effects of language varied with the difficulty of the task and measure used. In both cases, learning the words in a positive context improved learning. Importantly, the effect of emotionality was not modulated by language, suggesting that the effects of emotionality are independent of language and could potentially be a useful tool for improving foreign language vocabulary learning.


Subject(s)
Emotions , Learning , Photic Stimulation , Vocabulary , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Young Adult
8.
Sci Rep ; 10(1): 13967, 2020 08 18.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32811966

ABSTRACT

Vocabulary learning occurs throughout the lifespan, often implicitly. For foreign language learners, this is particularly challenging as they must acquire a large number of new words with little exposure. In the present study, we explore the effects of contextual diversity-namely, the number of texts a word appears in-on native and foreign language word learning. Participants read several texts that had novel pseudowords replacing high-frequency words. The total number of encounters with the novel words was held constant, but they appeared in 1, 2, 4, or 8 texts. In addition, some participants read the texts in Spanish (their native language) and others in English (their foreign language). We found that increasing contextual diversity improved recall and recognition of the word, as well as the ability to match the word with its meaning while keeping comprehension unimpaired. Using a foreign language only affected performance in the matching task, where participants had to quickly identify the meaning of the word. Results are discussed in the greater context of the word learning and foreign language literature as well as their importance as a teaching tool.

9.
Psicológica (Valencia. Internet) ; 41(1): 21-38, ene. 2020. ilus, tab, graf
Article in English | IBECS | ID: ibc-193628

ABSTRACT

The current study focuses on how different scales with varying demands can affect our subjective assessments. We carried out 2 experiments in which we asked participants to rate how happy or sad morphed images of faces looked. The two extremes were the original happy and original sad faces with 4 morphs in between. We manipulated language of the task-namely, half of the participants carried it out in their native language, Spanish, and the other half in their foreign language, English-and type of scale. Within type of scale, we compared verbal and brightness scales. We found that, while language did not have an effect on the assessment, type of scale did. The brightness scale led to overall higher ratings, i.e., assessing all faces as somewhat happier. This provides a limitation on the foreign language effect, as well as evidence for the influence of the cognitive demands of a scale on emotionality assessments


El estudio actual se centra en cómo escalas diferentes con demandas cognitivas variadas pueden afectar nuestras evaluaciones subjetivas. Se realizaron dos experimentos en los que se les pidió a los participantes que evaluaran cuán felices o tristes les resultaban las expresiones de algunas caras. Los dos extremos eran las caras tristes y felices originales, con cuatro variaciones en el medio. Manipulamos el idioma de la tarea, de tal manera que la mitad de los participantes realizaron el estudio en su idioma nativo (español) y la otra mitad en su idioma extranjero (inglés), y también variamos el tipo de escala. Comparamos dos tipos de escalas de valoración: verbales y de brillo (gris). Encontramos que, si bien la lengua no tuvo un efecto en la evaluación, el tipo de escala sí lo tuvo: la escala de brillo llevó a calificaciones más altas en general. Es decir, los participantes evaluaron todas las caras como algo más felices con la escala de brillo. Esto ofrece una limitación al impacto de los efectos de lenguas extranjeras, proporcionando evidencia sobre la influencia que tienen las demandas cognitivas de la escala en las evaluaciones de emocionalidad


Subject(s)
Humans , Female , Adult , Perception/physiology , Emotions/physiology , Happiness , Bereavement/psychology , Smiling/physiology , Nonverbal Communication/physiology , Smiling/psychology , Nonverbal Communication/psychology
10.
Acta Psychol (Amst) ; 186: 63-70, 2018 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29704743

ABSTRACT

The information we obtain from how speakers sound-for example their accent-affects how we interpret the messages they convey. A clear example is foreign accented speech, where reduced intelligibility and speaker's social categorization (out-group member) affect memory and the credibility of the message (e.g., less trustworthiness). In the present study, we go one step further and ask whether evaluations of messages are also affected by regional accents-accents from a different region than the listener. In the current study, we report results from three experiments on immediate memory recognition and immediate credibility assessments as well as the illusory truth effect. These revealed no differences between messages conveyed in local-from the same region as the participant-and regional accents-from native speakers of a different country than the participants. Our results suggest that when the accent of a speaker has high intelligibility, social categorization by accent does not seem to negatively affect how we treat the speakers' messages.


Subject(s)
Cross-Cultural Comparison , Language , Memory/physiology , Phonetics , Speech Perception/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Cognition/physiology , Cuba/ethnology , Female , Humans , Male , South America/ethnology , Spain/ethnology , Speech/physiology , Young Adult
11.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28194485

ABSTRACT

Manatees live in shallow, frequently turbid waters. The sensory means by which they navigate in these conditions are unknown. Poor visual acuity, lack of echolocation, and modest chemosensation suggest that other modalities play an important role. Rich innervation of sensory hairs that cover the entire body and enlarged somatosensory areas of the brain suggest that tactile senses are good candidates. Previous tests of detection of underwater vibratory stimuli indicated that they use passive movement of the hairs to detect particle displacements in the vicinity of a micron or less for frequencies from 10 to 150 Hz. In the current study, hydrodynamic stimuli were created by a sinusoidally oscillating sphere that generated a dipole field at frequencies from 5 to 150 Hz. Go/no-go tests of manatee postcranial mechanoreception of hydrodynamic stimuli indicated excellent sensitivity but about an order of magnitude less than the facial region. When the vibrissae were trimmed, detection thresholds were elevated, suggesting that the vibrissae were an important means by which detection occurred. Manatees were also highly accurate in two-choice directional discrimination: greater than 90% correct at all frequencies tested. We hypothesize that manatees utilize vibrissae as a three-dimensional array to detect and localize low-frequency hydrodynamic stimuli.


Subject(s)
Hydrodynamics , Touch/physiology , Trichechus manatus/physiology , Vibration , Vibrissae/physiology , Animals , Florida , Male
12.
J Neuropathol Exp Neurol ; 73(4): 345-61, 2014 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24607965

ABSTRACT

There is increasing evidence that long-lasting morphologic and functional consequences can be present in the human visual system after repetitive mild traumatic brain injury (r-mTBI). The exact location and extent of the damage in this condition are not well understood. Using a recently developed mouse model of r-mTBI, we assessed the effects on the retina and optic nerve using histology and immunohistochemistry, electroretinography (ERG), and spectral-domain optical coherence tomography (SD-OCT) at 10 and 13 weeks after injury. Control mice received repetitive anesthesia alone (r-sham). We observed decreased optic nerve diameters and increased cellularity and areas of demyelination in optic nerves in r-mTBI versus r-sham mice. There were concomitant areas of decreased cellularity in the retinal ganglion cell layer and approximately 67% decrease in brain-specific homeobox/POU domain protein 3A-positive retinal ganglion cells in retinal flat mounts. Furthermore, SD-OCT demonstrated a detectable thinning of the inner retina; ERG demonstrated a decrease in the amplitude of the photopic negative response without any change in a- or b-wave amplitude or timing. Thus, the ERG and SD-OCT data correlated well with changes detected by morphometric, histologic, and immunohistochemical methods, thereby supporting the use of these noninvasive methods in the assessment of visual function and morphology in clinical cases of mTBI.


Subject(s)
Brain Injuries/complications , Optic Nerve Diseases/etiology , Optic Nerve Diseases/pathology , Retinal Diseases/etiology , Retinal Diseases/pathology , Animals , Calcium-Binding Proteins/metabolism , Cell Count , Disease Models, Animal , Electroretinography , Female , Glial Fibrillary Acidic Protein/metabolism , Male , Mice , Mice, Inbred C57BL , Microfilament Proteins/metabolism , Neurons/metabolism , Neurons/pathology , Time Factors , Tomography, Optical Coherence , Transcription Factor Brn-3A/metabolism
13.
Neuroreport ; 24(14): 813-7, 2013 Oct 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23903459

ABSTRACT

The current study examined the interaction of fearful, angry, happy, and neutral expressions with left, straight, and right eye gaze directions. Human participants viewed faces consisting of various expression and eye gaze combinations while event-related potential (ERP) data were collected. The results showed that angry expressions modulated the mean amplitude of the P1, whereas fearful and happy expressions modulated the mean amplitude of the N170. No influence of eye gaze on mean amplitudes for the P1 and N170 emerged. Fearful, angry, and happy expressions began to interact with eye gaze to influence mean amplitudes in the time window of 200-400 ms. The results suggest early processing of expression influence ERPs independent of eye gaze, whereas expression and gaze interact to influence later ERPs.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Facial Expression , Fear , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Adult , Eye , Fear/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation/methods , Reaction Time/physiology , Young Adult
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL