Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 14 de 14
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
Drug Alcohol Depend ; 227: 108946, 2021 10 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34392051

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: The Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development ™ Study (ABCD Study®) is an open-science, multi-site, prospective, longitudinal study following over 11,800 9- and 10-year-old youth into early adulthood. The ABCD Study aims to prospectively examine the impact of substance use (SU) on neurocognitive and health outcomes. Although SU initiation typically occurs during teen years, relatively little is known about patterns of SU in children younger than 12. METHODS: This study aims to report the detailed ABCD Study® SU patterns at baseline (n = 11,875) in order to inform the greater scientific community about cohort's early SU. Along with a detailed description of SU, we ran mixed effects regression models to examine the association between early caffeine and alcohol sipping with demographic factors, externalizing symptoms and parental history of alcohol and substance use disorders (AUD/SUD). PRIMARY RESULTS: At baseline, the majority of youth had used caffeine (67.6 %) and 22.5 % reported sipping alcohol (22.5 %). There was little to no reported use of other drug categories (0.2 % full alcohol drink, 0.7 % used nicotine, <0.1 % used any other drug of abuse). Analyses revealed that total caffeine use and early alcohol sipping were associated with demographic variables (p's<.05), externalizing symptoms (caffeine p = 0002; sipping p = .0003), and parental history of AUD (sipping p = .03). CONCLUSIONS: ABCD Study participants aged 9-10 years old reported caffeine use and alcohol sipping experimentation, but very rare other SU. Variables linked with early childhood alcohol sipping and caffeine use should be examined as contributing factors in future longitudinal analyses examining escalating trajectories of SU in the ABCD Study cohort.


Subject(s)
Substance-Related Disorders , Adolescent , Adult , Brain , Child , Child, Preschool , Cognition , Humans , Longitudinal Studies , Prospective Studies , Substance-Related Disorders/epidemiology
2.
Vision Res ; 41(25-26): 3295-305, 2001.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11718774

ABSTRACT

This paper summarizes recent experiments conducted by the authors - experiments that studied the behavioral characteristics of large gaze shifts and the neural bases of coordinated movements of the eyes and head.


Subject(s)
Eye Movements/physiology , Head Movements/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Visual Cortex/physiology , Animals , Electric Stimulation/methods , Macaca mulatta , Superior Colliculi/physiology
3.
Biol Cybern ; 84(6): 453-62, 2001 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11417057

ABSTRACT

Advances in understanding the neural control of saccades (visual orienting movements made when the head is prevented from moving) stem largely from early modeling efforts which provided a framework for developing and testing hypotheses about the relationships between neural activity and observed behaviors. When the head is free to move, visual orienting is often accomplished with coordinated movements of the eyes and head. A recent description of the temporal progression (i.e., kinematics) of these movements led to the hypothesis that eye and head control signals interact. This hypothesis is now formalized as a control systems model which accounts for existing data and makes explicit predictions about the neural control of orienting gaze shifts.


Subject(s)
Fixation, Ocular/physiology , Head Movements/physiology , Models, Neurological , Saccades/physiology , Animals , Biomechanical Phenomena , Computer Simulation , Fovea Centralis/physiology , Haplorhini , Humans , Neck/physiology , Reflex/physiology , Reflex, Vestibulo-Ocular/physiology , Visual Fields/physiology
4.
Exp Brain Res ; 131(1): 22-32, 2000 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10759168

ABSTRACT

When the head is restrained, saccades are characterized by lawful relationships between movement amplitude, peak velocity, and duration. In addition, the spatiotemporal progression of saccades (i.e., movement kinematics) is predictable if saccade amplitude and direction are known. However, when the head is free to move, changes in the direction of the line of sight (gaze shifts) often involve saccades associated with simultaneous head movements. The metrics (duration, amplitude, peak velocity) and kinematics of saccades occurring in conjunction with head movements cannot be predicted on the basis of saccade amplitude and direction alone. For example, when the head is unrestrained, velocity profiles of 35 degree eye movements can be symmetrical and might have peaks approximately 600 degrees/s. But, 35 degrees eye movements can also have peak velocities of approximately 300 degrees/s and have velocity profiles with two pronounced peaks: an initial peak followed by a reduction and subsequent increase in velocity. Saccade amplitude and direction are insufficient to predict the shape of the velocity profile. However, as illustrated in this report, if the amplitude of the concurrent head movement is taken into account, saccade kinematics are predictable even during gaze shifts with large head components. The data presented here are indicative of an interaction between eye and head motor systems in which head movement commands alter the execution of concurrent saccades.


Subject(s)
Head Movements/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Saccades/physiology , Animals , Biomechanical Phenomena , Macaca mulatta , Reaction Time/physiology , Reflex, Vestibulo-Ocular/physiology , Restraint, Physical
5.
J Neurophysiol ; 82(5): 2808-11, 1999 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10561447

ABSTRACT

Saccadic eye movements result from high-frequency bursts of activity in ocular motoneurons. This phasic activity originates in premotor burst neurons. When the head is restrained, the number of action potentials in the bursts of burst neurons and motoneurons increases linearly with eye movement amplitude. However, when the head is unrestrained, the number of action potentials now increase as a function of the change in the direction of the line of sight during eye movements of relatively similar amplitudes. These data suggest an apparent uncoupling of premotor neuron and motoneuron activity from the resultant eye movement.


Subject(s)
Abducens Nerve/physiology , Brain Stem/physiology , Motor Neurons/physiology , Neurons/physiology , Saccades/physiology , Action Potentials , Animals , Conditioning, Operant , Fixation, Ocular , Haplorhini , Head Movements , Immobilization , Photic Stimulation
6.
J Neurophysiol ; 78(3): 1669-90, 1997 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9310452

ABSTRACT

When the head is free to move, microstimulation of the primate superior colliculus (SC) evokes coordinated movements of the eyes and head. The similarity between these stimulation-induced movements and visually guided movements indicates that the SC of the primate is involved in redirecting the line of sight (gaze). To determine how movement commands are represented by individual collicular neurons, we recorded the activity of single cells in the deeper layers of the superior colliculus of the rhesus monkey during coordinated eye-head gaze shifts. Two alternative hypotheses were tested. The "separate channel" hypothesis states that two displacement commands are generated by the SC: one signal specifying the amplitude and direction of eye movements and a second signal specifying the amplitude and direction of head movements. Alternatively, a single gaze displacement command could be generated by the SC ("gaze displacement" hypothesis). The activity of collicular neurons was examined during three behavioral dissociations of gaze, eye, and head movement amplitude and direction (metrics). Subsets of trials were selected in which the amplitude and direction of either gaze shifts or eye movements or head movements were relatively constant but the metrics of the other two varied over wide ranges. Under these conditions, the separate channel and gaze displacement hypotheses make differential predictions about the patterns of SC activity. We tested these differential predictions by comparing observed patterns with predicted patterns of neuronal activity. We obtained data consistent with the predictions of the gaze displacement hypothesis. The predictions of the separate channel hypothesis were not confirmed. Thus microstimulation data, single-unit recording data, and behavioral data are all consistent with the gaze displacement hypothesis of collicular function--the hypothesis that a gaze displacement signal is derived from the locus of activity within the motor map of the SC and subsequently is decomposed into separate eye and head displacement signals downstream from the colliculus.


Subject(s)
Fixation, Ocular/physiology , Superior Colliculi/physiology , Animals , Conditioning, Operant/physiology , Data Interpretation, Statistical , Electric Stimulation , Eye Movements/physiology , Female , Head Movements/physiology , Macaca mulatta , Memory/physiology , Motor Neurons/physiology , Superior Colliculi/cytology , Visual Fields/physiology
7.
J Neurophysiol ; 77(5): 2328-48, 1997 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9163361

ABSTRACT

We analyzed gaze shifts made by trained rhesus monkeys with completely unrestrained heads during performance of a delayed gaze shift task. Subjects made horizontal, vertical, and oblique gaze shifts to visual targets. We found that coordinated eye-head movements are characterized by a set of lawful relationships, and that the initial position of the eyes in the orbits and the direction of the gaze shift are two factors that influence these relationships. Head movements did not contribute to the change in gaze position during small gaze shifts (<20 degrees) directed along the horizontal meridian, when the eyes were initially centered in the orbits. For larger gaze shifts (25-90 degrees), the head contribution to the gaze shift increased linearly with increasing gaze shift amplitude, and eye movement amplitude saturated at an asymptotic amplitude of approximately 35 degrees. When the eyes began deviated in the orbits contralateral to the direction of the ensuing gaze shift, the head contributed less and the eyes more to amplitude-matched gaze shifts. The relative timing of eye and head movements was altered by initial eye position; head latency relative to gaze onset increased as the eyes began in more contralateral initial positions. The direction of the gaze shift also affected the relative amplitudes of eye and head movements; as gaze shifts were made in progressively more vertical directions, eye amplitude increased and head contribution declined systematically. Eye velocity was a saturating function of gaze amplitude for movements without a head contribution (gaze amplitude <20 degrees). As head contribution increased with increasing gaze amplitude (20-60 degrees), peak eye velocity declined by >200 degrees/s and head velocity increased by 100 degrees/s. For constant-amplitude eye movements (approximately 30 degrees), eye velocity declined as the velocity of the concurrent head movement increased. On the basis of these relationships, it is possible to accurately predict gaze amplitude, the amplitudes of the eye and head components of the gaze shift, and gaze, eye, and head velocities, durations and latencies if the two-dimensional displacement of the target and the initial position of the eyes in the orbits are known. These data indicate that signals related to the initial positions of the eyes in the orbits and the direction of the gaze shift influence separate eye and head movement commands. The hypothesis that this divergence of eye and head commands occurs downstream from the superior colliculus is supported by recent electrical stimulation and single-unit recording data.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Head Movements/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Saccades/physiology , Animals , Brain Mapping , Female , Macaca mulatta , Orientation/physiology , Superior Colliculi/physiology , Visual Pathways/physiology
8.
J Neurophysiol ; 76(5): 3360-81, 1996 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8930279

ABSTRACT

1. Microstimulation is used to investigate how activity in the superior colliculus (SC) contributes to determining the properties of primate saccadic eye movements. The site of collicular stimulation, the duration of the stimulation train, and the frequency of the stimulation train are each varied to examine the relative contributions of the locus, duration, and level of collicular activity to determining saccade amplitude, direction, duration, and velocity. 2. For any given site of stimulation, a relationship between movement amplitude and train duration can be demonstrated. Movement amplitude is a monotonically increasing, but saturating, function of increasing train duration. The size of the largest movement is dictated by the site of stimulation. Within the range over which amplitude can be modulated, movement offset is linked to the offset of the stimulation train. As a result, each decrement or increment in train duration produces a corresponding decrement or increment in movement duration. 3. The peak velocity of an evoked movement is influenced by the frequency of stimulation; a higher frequency of stimulation produces a movement of higher velocity. 4. The effects of train duration and frequency can be traded to produce movements that have comparable amplitudes but different dynamic characteristics; high-velocity movements of short duration and low-velocity movements of long duration can be produced by stimulating with high-frequency, short-duration, and low-frequency, long-duration trains, respectively. Across stimulation frequencies, the amplitude of an evoked movement is best related to the total number of pulses in the stimulation train. 5. Because it is possible to compensate for reduced velocity by increasing the duration of the stimulation train, the same site-specific maximum amplitude can be attained with different frequencies of stimulation. 6. Small, but significant, changes in movement direction occur as a result of varying train duration or train frequency. 7. The latency to movement onset (i.e., interval from stimulation onset to movement onset) depends upon the frequency of stimulation. A higher frequency of stimulation produces a movement of shorter latency. 8. These data demonstrate that both the site of stimulation and the parameters of stimulation contribute to determining the properties of a movement evoked from the primate SC. In doing so, they contradict the results of early microstimulation studies that suggest that the properties of eye movements evoked from the primate SC are determined solely by the site of stimulation. The findings conflict with the traditional view of collicular function that suggests that the collicular motor representation is purely anatomic. Rather, these data support a revised view whereby the locus, duration, and level of collicular activity contribute to determining the properties of a primate saccadic eye movement. According to this view, independent information relating to desired displacement and saccade velocity are extracted from the spatiotemporal profile of collicular activity.


Subject(s)
Behavior, Animal/physiology , Eye Movements/physiology , Saccades/physiology , Superior Colliculi/physiology , Animals , Electric Stimulation , Macaca mulatta
9.
J Neurophysiol ; 76(2): 927-52, 1996 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8871209

ABSTRACT

1. We electrically stimulated the intermediate and deep layers of the superior colliculus (SC) in two rhesus macaques free to move their heads both vertically and horizontally (head unrestrained). Stimulation of the primate SC can elicit high-velocity, combined, eye-head gaze shifts that are similar to visually guided gaze shifts of comparable amplitude and direction. The amplitude of gaze shifts produced by collicular stimulation depends on the site of stimulation and on the parameters of stimulation (frequency, current, and duration of the stimulation train). 2. The maximal amplitude gaze shifts, produced by electrical stimulation at 56 sites in the SC of two rhesus monkeys, ranged in amplitude from approximately 7 to approximately 80 deg. Because the head was unrestrained, stimulation-induced gaze shifts often included movements of the head. Head movements produced at the 56 stimulation sites ranged in amplitude from 0 to approximately 70 deg. 3. The relationships between peak velocity and amplitude and between duration and amplitude of stimulation-induced head movements and gaze shifts were comparable with the relationships observed during visually guided gaze shifts. The relative contributions of the eyes and head to visually guided and stimulation-induced gaze shifts were also similar. 4. As was true for visually guided gaze shifts, the head contribution to stimulation-induced gaze shifts depended on the position of the eyes relative to the head at the onset of stimulation. When the eyes were deviated in the direction of the ensuing gaze shift, the head contribution increased and the latency to head movement onset was decreased. 5. We systematically altered the duration of stimulation trains (10-400 ms) while stimulation frequency and current remained constant. Increases in stimulation duration systematically increased the amplitude of the evoked gaze shift until a site specific maximal amplitude was reached. Further increases in stimulation duration did not increase gaze amplitude. There was a high correlation between the end of the stimulation train and the end of the evoked gaze shift for movements smaller than the site-specific maximal amplitude. 6. Unlike the effects of stimulation duration on gaze amplitude, the amplitude and duration of evoked head movements did not saturate for the range of durations tested (10-400 ms), but continued to increase linearly with increases in stimulation duration. 7. The frequency of stimulation was systematically varied (range: 63-1,000 Hz) while other stimulation parameters remained constant. The velocity of evoked gaze shifts was related to the frequency of stimulation; higher stimulation frequencies resulted in higher peak velocities. The maximal, site-specific amplitude was independent of stimulation frequency. 8. When stimulating a single collicular site using identical stimulation parameters, the amplitude and direction of stimulation-induced gaze shifts, initiated from different initial positions, were relatively constant. In contrast, the amplitude and direction of the eye component of these fixed vector gaze shifts depended upon the initial position of the eyes in the orbits; the endpoints of the eye movements converged on an orbital region, or "goal," that depended on the site of collicular stimulation. 9. When identical stimulation parameters were used and when the eyes were centered initially in the orbits, the gaze shifts produced by caudal collicular stimulation when the head was restrained were typically smaller than those evoked from the same site when the head was unrestrained. This attenuation occurred because stimulation drove the eyes to approximately the same orbital position when the head was restrained or unrestrained. Thus movements produced when the head was restrained were reduced in amplitude by approximately the amount that the head would have contributed if free to move. 10. When the head was restrained, only the eye component of the intended gaze shift


Subject(s)
Eye Movements/physiology , Head Movements/physiology , Superior Colliculi/physiology , Animals , Electric Stimulation , Fixation, Ocular/physiology , Macaca mulatta , Restraint, Physical
10.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 86(4): 1318-32, 1989 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2808907

ABSTRACT

The ability of the echolocating bat, Eptesicus fuscus, to detect a sonar target is affected by the presence of other targets along the same axis at slightly different ranges. If echoes from one target arrive at about the same delay as echoes from another target, clutter interference occurs and one set of echoes masks the other. Although the bat's sonar emissions and the echoes themselves are 2 to 5 ms long, echoes (of approximately equal sensation levels--around 15 dB SL) only interfere with each other if they arrive within 200 to 400 microseconds of the same arrival time. This figure is an estimate of the integration time of the bat's sonar receiver for echoes. The fine structure of the clutter-interference data reflects the reinforcement and cancellation of echoes according to their time separation. When clutter interference first occurs, the waveforms of test and cluttering echoes already overlap for much of their duration. The masking effect underlying clutter interference appears specifically due to overlap, not between raw echo waveforms, but between the patterns of mechanical excitation created when echoes pass through bandpass filters equivalent to auditory-nerve tuning curves. While the time scale of clutter interference is substantially shorter than the duration of echo waveforms, it still is much longer than the eventual width of a target's range-axis image expressed in terms of echo delay.


Subject(s)
Chiroptera/physiology , Echolocation/physiology , Animals , Orientation
11.
J Neurobiol ; 20(7): 619-34, 1989 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2794996

ABSTRACT

The electric organ discharge (EOD) of several species of mormyrid fishes within the genus Brienomyrus is sexually dimorphic during the breeding season: the duration of the male's EOD is much longer than the duration of the female's (for a review see Hopkins, 1986). The mormyrid used here, Brienomyrus sp., exhibits similar alterations in the duration of the triphasic EOD after treatment with testosterone, as do other members of this genus (for reviews see Bass, 1986a,b). In this experiment, animals were intraperitoneally implanted with pellets of either 11-ketotestosterone or 17 a-methyltestosterone, and the time course of the changes in the duration of each of the three phases of the EOD were quantified. Additionally, the time course of changes in the morphology of the electric organ, after testosterone treatment, was also quantified using electron microscopic techniques. The results suggest that the change in the duration of the first phase of the EOD is due exclusively to the change in the thickness of the electrocyte body: this is consistent with a model proposed by Bennett and Grundfest (1961) for the electrogenesis of a triphasic EOD. Changes in the duration of the second and third phases of the EOD are highly correlated with the changes in the surface area of the posterior and anterior faces of the electrocyte, respectively. The results support the hypothesis that gonadal steroid hormone-induced changes in the EOD are due to structural changes in the electrocyte's membranes, and that all of the observed changes in the discharge of this system can be explained by the action of steroid hormones on the peripheral target cells (electrocytes).


Subject(s)
Electric Fish/physiology , Electric Organ/physiology , Testosterone/pharmacology , Action Potentials/drug effects , Animals , Electric Organ/cytology , Electric Organ/drug effects , Female , Male , Methyltestosterone/pharmacology , Testosterone/analogs & derivatives
12.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 84(6): 2081-91, 1988 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3265711

ABSTRACT

Responses of individual eighth-nerve fibers in the bullfrog (Rana catesbeiana) were measured to tone bursts at best frequency against a background of continuous, broadband masking noise. These data were used to calculate critical masking ratios to describe the fibers' responses to tones embedded in noise. In the frequency response range of the amphibian papilla (100-1000 Hz), critical ratios increase with tone frequency. Critical ratios of basilar papilla fibers (1000-2000 Hz) are generally higher than those of amphibian papilla fibers. Critical ratios are also significantly related to fiber threshold such that fibers with high thresholds, regardless of their best frequencies, have higher critical ratios and are thus less selective to signals embedded in noise. Critical ratios based on neural responses show a somewhat different frequency-dependent trend than do critical ratios based on psychophysical data presented previously for this species [A. M. Simmons, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 83, 1087-1092 (1988a)]. In addition, these neural critical ratios do not appear to be level independent, as are psychophysical critical ratios. The data suggest that frequency selectivity of hearing in the bullfrog as measured behaviorally is probably not mediated solely by spectral filtering in the auditory periphery.


Subject(s)
Perceptual Masking/physiology , Pitch Discrimination/physiology , Rana catesbeiana/physiology , Vestibulocochlear Nerve/physiology , Animals , Auditory Threshold/physiology , Nerve Fibers/physiology , Psychoacoustics
14.
Eur J Pharmacol ; 82(3-4): 131-5, 1982 Aug 27.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-6290234

ABSTRACT

We investigated the effect of GABA, muscimol and THIP on the K+ -stimulated and spontaneous release of [3H]GABA from neuron-enriched cell cultures of the rat cerebellum. Each agonist produced significant reductions in evoked [3H]GABA without causing marked changes in spontaneous release. The agonist-induced inhibition of K+ -stimulated [3H]GABA release was reversed by the GABA antagonists bicuculline and picrotoxin. It is suggested that GABAergic neurons in cerebellar cell cultures possess GABA receptors which are involved in the regulation of evoked transmitter release.


Subject(s)
Cerebellum/metabolism , Neurons/metabolism , gamma-Aminobutyric Acid/metabolism , Animals , Bicuculline/pharmacology , Cells, Cultured , Fluspirilene/pharmacology , Isoxazoles/pharmacology , Muscimol/pharmacology , Potassium/pharmacology , Rats , Receptors, Cell Surface/metabolism , Receptors, GABA-A
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...