Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 6 de 6
Filter
Add more filters











Database
Publication year range
1.
Psychol Sci ; 24(6): 1037-43, 2013 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23625879

ABSTRACT

All humans share a universal, evolutionarily ancient approximate number system (ANS) that estimates and combines the numbers of objects in sets with ratio-limited precision. Interindividual variability in the acuity of the ANS correlates with mathematical achievement, but the causes of this correlation have never been established. We acquired psychophysical measures of ANS acuity in child and adult members of an indigene group in the Amazon, the Mundurucú, who have a very restricted numerical lexicon and highly variable access to mathematics education. By comparing Mundurucú subjects with and without access to schooling, we found that education significantly enhances the acuity with which sets of concrete objects are estimated. These results indicate that culture and education have an important effect on basic number perception. We hypothesize that symbolic and nonsymbolic numerical thinking mutually enhance one another over the course of mathematics instruction.


Subject(s)
Mathematical Concepts , Mathematics/education , Thinking , Adolescent , Adult , Brazil/ethnology , Child , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Young Adult
2.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 108(24): 9782-7, 2011 Jun 14.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21606377

ABSTRACT

Kant argued that Euclidean geometry is synthesized on the basis of an a priori intuition of space. This proposal inspired much behavioral research probing whether spatial navigation in humans and animals conforms to the predictions of Euclidean geometry. However, Euclidean geometry also includes concepts that transcend the perceptible, such as objects that are infinitely small or infinitely large, or statements of necessity and impossibility. We tested the hypothesis that certain aspects of nonperceptible Euclidian geometry map onto intuitions of space that are present in all humans, even in the absence of formal mathematical education. Our tests probed intuitions of points, lines, and surfaces in participants from an indigene group in the Amazon, the Mundurucu, as well as adults and age-matched children controls from the United States and France and younger US children without education in geometry. The responses of Mundurucu adults and children converged with that of mathematically educated adults and children and revealed an intuitive understanding of essential properties of Euclidean geometry. For instance, on a surface described to them as perfectly planar, the Mundurucu's estimations of the internal angles of triangles added up to ~180 degrees, and when asked explicitly, they stated that there exists one single parallel line to any given line through a given point. These intuitions were also partially in place in the group of younger US participants. We conclude that, during childhood, humans develop geometrical intuitions that spontaneously accord with the principles of Euclidean geometry, even in the absence of training in mathematics.


Subject(s)
Indians, South American/psychology , Intuition , Surveys and Questionnaires , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Brazil , Child , Female , Geography , Humans , Male , Mathematics , Middle Aged , Task Performance and Analysis , Young Adult
3.
Science ; 320(5880): 1217-20, 2008 May 30.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18511690

ABSTRACT

The mapping of numbers onto space is fundamental to measurement and to mathematics. Is this mapping a cultural invention or a universal intuition shared by all humans regardless of culture and education? We probed number-space mappings in the Mundurucu, an Amazonian indigene group with a reduced numerical lexicon and little or no formal education. At all ages, the Mundurucu mapped symbolic and nonsymbolic numbers onto a logarithmic scale, whereas Western adults used linear mapping with small or symbolic numbers and logarithmic mapping when numbers were presented nonsymbolically under conditions that discouraged counting. This indicates that the mapping of numbers onto space is a universal intuition and that this initial intuition of number is logarithmic. The concept of a linear number line appears to be a cultural invention that fails to develop in the absence of formal education.


Subject(s)
Cultural Evolution , Indians, South American , Intuition , Mathematics , Adolescent , Adult , Anthropology, Cultural , Brazil , Child , Educational Status , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged
5.
Science ; 311(5759): 381-4, 2006 Jan 20.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16424341

ABSTRACT

Does geometry constitute a core set of intuitions present in all humans, regardless of their language or schooling? We used two nonverbal tests to probe the conceptual primitives of geometry in the Mundurukú, an isolated Amazonian indigene group. Mundurukú children and adults spontaneously made use of basic geometric concepts such as points, lines, parallelism, or right angles to detect intruders in simple pictures, and they used distance, angle, and sense relationships in geometrical maps to locate hidden objects. Our results provide evidence for geometrical intuitions in the absence of schooling, experience with graphic symbols or maps, or a rich language of geometrical terms.


Subject(s)
Comprehension , Indians, South American/psychology , Knowledge , Mathematics , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Analysis of Variance , Brazil , Child , Child, Preschool , Culture , Female , Humans , Language , Male , Maps as Topic , Middle Aged
6.
Science ; 306(5695): 499-503, 2004 Oct 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15486303

ABSTRACT

Is calculation possible without language? Or is the human ability for arithmetic dependent on the language faculty? To clarify the relation between language and arithmetic, we studied numerical cognition in speakers of Mundurukú, an Amazonian language with a very small lexicon of number words. Although the Mundurukú lack words for numbers beyond 5, they are able to compare and add large approximate numbers that are far beyond their naming range. However, they fail in exact arithmetic with numbers larger than 4 or 5. Our results imply a distinction between a nonverbal system of number approximation and a language-based counting system for exact number and arithmetic.


Subject(s)
Cognition , Indians, South American , Language , Mathematics , Vocabulary , Adult , Brazil , Child , Culture , Humans
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL