Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 7 de 7
Filter
Add more filters











Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
Ann Clin Psychiatry ; 30(2): 113-121, 2018 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29697712

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Traumatic experiences can cause ethical conflicts. "Moral injury" (MI) has been used to describe this emotional/cognitive state, and could contribute to the development of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) or block its recovery. We examine the relationship between MI and PTSD, and the impact of religious involvement (RI) on that relationship. METHODS: We conducted a study of 120 veterans enrolled at the Charlie Norwood VA Medical Center in Augusta, Georgia. Standard measures of PTSD symptoms, MI, and RI were administered. Regression models were used to examine correlates of PTSD symptoms and the moderating or mediating effects of RI. RESULTS: A strong relationship was found between MI and PTSD symptoms (r = 0.54, P ≤ .0001), and between MI and RI (r = -.41, P ≤ .0001), but only a weak relationship was found between RI and PTSD symptoms (r = -.17, P = .058). RI did not mediate or moderate the relationship between MI and PTSD symptoms in the overall sample. However, among non-Middle Eastern war theater veterans, a significant buffering effect of religiosity was found. CONCLUSIONS: MI has a strong positive relationship with PTSD symptoms and an inverse relationship with RI. RI did not mediate or moderate the relationship between MI and PTSD in the overall sample, but it moderated this relationship in non-Middle Eastern war theater veterans.


Subject(s)
Religion , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/diagnosis , Veterans/psychology , Female , Hospitals, Veterans , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/psychology , Surveys and Questionnaires , United States , Veterans/statistics & numerical data
2.
Hear Res ; 240(1-2): 112-5, 2008 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18514450

ABSTRACT

The focus of this study was to gauge the influence of intervening interference on an intensity standard held within auditory working memory through measurement of the just noticeable difference (JND) for intensity. Additionally, the use of interaural phase differences and interaural level differences as spatial cues were employed to identify whether these indicators provided a means for release from interference. A series of tones, both with and without spatial cues, were presented to subjects and responses were obtained using the method of constant stimuli. The JND for intensity was measured in a control condition with a silent inter-comparison interval and three conditions containing intervening tones within the temporal gap between the standard and comparison stimuli. The presence of intervening interference produced a significant increase in the intensity difference needed for discrimination. Further, the provision of spatial cues did not result in a significant release from this interference. These results indicate that a release from interference is not obtained when listeners are required to rely entirely on information used for spatial location (i.e., overall intensity differences and interaural phase/intensity differences) without unique information identifying the sound source to aid in retention of relevant information within auditory working memory.


Subject(s)
Auditory Pathways/physiology , Cues , Loudness Perception , Memory , Perceptual Masking , Sound Localization , Acoustic Stimulation , Humans
3.
J Rheumatol ; 32(9): 1699-705, 2005 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16142863

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: Pain and psychological distress are associated with fatigue, and social support may play a buffering role in the adjustment to a chronic disease. Investigations of the relationship between fatigue and disease activity in chronic diseases have provided inconclusive findings. The influence of medications on perceived fatigue remains unclear. We investigated the relationship between pain, depression, fatigue, and disease activity in patients with systemic lupus erythematosus (SLE). METHODS: Participants (n = 127) completed a psychosocial questionnaire during routine clinic visits. Hierarchical multiple regression analysis was conducted to predict the contribution of disease activity, pain, depression (Beck Depression Inventory), and perceived social support to fatigue. RESULTS: Disease activity as measured by SLE Disease Activity Index (SLEDAI) did not significantly predict self-reported levels of fatigue. Medication usage did not predict fatigue levels. Pain and depression were both unique positive predictors of fatigue. Controlling for pain and depression, perceived social support contributed negatively to the variance in fatigue scores, suggesting a buffering effect. This model reliably explained 42% of the variance in fatigue scores. CONCLUSION: Our results emphasize the importance of depression, pain, and perceived social support in predicting reported fatigue levels in patients with SLE. In contrast, disease activity measured by SLEDAI does not appear to account for fatigue in SLE. Understanding the effect of psychosocial factors on fatigue in SLE may improve patient outcomes through psychosocial interventions aimed at reducing pain and increasing coping skills and social support.


Subject(s)
Depressive Disorder/epidemiology , Fatigue/epidemiology , Fatigue/psychology , Lupus Erythematosus, Systemic/epidemiology , Lupus Erythematosus, Systemic/psychology , Age Distribution , Cohort Studies , Comorbidity , Depressive Disorder/diagnosis , Fatigue/diagnosis , Female , Follow-Up Studies , Humans , Incidence , Interpersonal Relations , Lupus Erythematosus, Systemic/diagnosis , Pain Measurement , Predictive Value of Tests , Probability , Risk Assessment , Severity of Illness Index , Sickness Impact Profile , Social Support , Surveys and Questionnaires
4.
J Pain ; 5(9): 521-7, 2004 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15556831

ABSTRACT

UNLABELLED: This investigation examined self-care behaviors for muscle pain because of the prevalence of musculoskeletal pain and the substitution of self-care for formal medical care. In Study 1, university students (N = 187) completed a retrospective questionnaire about self-care for muscle pain. In Study 2, muscle pain was experimentally induced in university students (N = 79) with subsequent measurement of self-care. In both studies, stretching and massaging were the most frequently performed behaviors, and consuming medication was the least frequently performed. In Study 1, the perceived effectiveness of behaviors and level of pain required to perform self-care accounted for 12% to 32% of the variance in behavior frequency. In Study 2, pain ratings and pain during activities were higher among those who performed self-care (ds = .59 to 1.00). These studies indicated that self-care behaviors are performed for both naturally occurring and experimentally induced muscle pain. However, both studies determined that the performance of self-care behaviors did not always correspond with current evidence of treatment effectiveness for muscle injuries. Unique opportunities for future investigations of self-care behavior models and interventions are permitted by muscle pain induction. PERSPECTIVE: Self-care for pain reduction is an understudied behavior. This report describes 2 studies of self-care behaviors for naturally occurring and experimentally induced muscle pain. The most frequent types of self-care behaviors are similar for the types of pain, and the perceived effectiveness of behaviors and pain level influence performance of the behaviors.


Subject(s)
Health Behavior , Muscle, Skeletal/injuries , Pain Management , Pain/psychology , Self Care , Adult , Analgesics/therapeutic use , Female , Humans , Hyperthermia, Induced , Hypothermia, Induced , Male , Massage , Nonprescription Drugs/therapeutic use , Rest
5.
Eur J Pain ; 8(4): 299-305, 2004 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15207510

ABSTRACT

Recent research suggests that the interpretation of maximal endpoints of pain scales vary between sexes. The purposes of this study were to investigate sex differences in (a) maximal endpoints of pain scales and (b) bias, discrimination, and the "better than average effect" for ratings of common pain events. Study participants described and rated the intensity of events that were the "most intense pain imaginable" for the typical woman, typical man, and one's self. Study participants also described and rated the intensity of the "most painful" events they had experienced. Study participants completed the situational pain questionnaire (SPQ), which measured the amount of pain that the typical woman, typical man, or one's self would be expected to experience during thirty common painful events. One hundred and fifteen undergraduate psychology students completed this study. Men and women differed in the categories of events selected for most intense pain imaginable for one's self. There were no significant sex differences for the intensity of most intense self-imagined pain or most painful event experienced. However, women were more likely to report the intensity of their worst self-imagined pain event as 100/100. In addition, only women demonstrated a significant correlation between the intensity of most painful self-experienced event and intensity of most intense self-imagined event. Analyses of the SPQ discrimination scores revealed no sex or version differences. Analyses of the SPQ bias scores showed that both sexes indicated that the typical woman would rate the intensity of common pain events higher than would the typical man. Women rated the intensity of common pain events for themselves lower than for the typical woman, but higher than the typical man, and men rated also rated themselves as lower than the typical women, but the same as the typical man. Thus, there was inconsistent support for the "better than average effect". Future research is needed to determine the clinical relevance of sex differences in pain anchors and gender-related stereotypes for evaluating other people's pain.


Subject(s)
Pain Measurement/methods , Pain Measurement/psychology , Pain/psychology , Sex Characteristics , Surveys and Questionnaires/standards , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Humans , Imagination/physiology , Male , Observer Variation , Reproducibility of Results
6.
Arthritis Rheum ; 51(2): 239-45, 2004 Apr 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15077266

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: To evaluate how a prior affective disorder (major depression or generalized anxiety disorder) affects current fatigue among individuals with rheumatoid arthritis (RA). To determine whether that relationship is mediated by self-efficacy expectations. METHODS: Forty-eight RA patients with a prior affective disorder and 74 without a history of affective disorder completed a mailed questionnaire that included the Multidimensional Assessment of Fatigue and indicators of neuroticism and self efficacy. RESULTS: RA patients with a history of affective disorder reported higher levels of fatigue than those with no previous affective disturbance. Controlling for neuroticism and self efficacy, affective disorder history continued to predict current fatigue. Mediational analyses revealed both direct and indirect effects (via self efficacy) of history of affective disorder on the experience of fatigue in RA. CONCLUSION: History of affective disorder independently predicts higher levels of fatigue in RA patients, and self efficacy plays a mediating role in this relationship.


Subject(s)
Arthritis, Rheumatoid/diagnosis , Arthritis, Rheumatoid/psychology , Fatigue/diagnosis , Mood Disorders/diagnosis , Aged , Anxiety/diagnosis , Anxiety/psychology , Depression/diagnosis , Depression/psychology , Fatigue/psychology , Female , Humans , Male , Medical History Taking , Middle Aged , Models, Psychological , Mood Disorders/psychology , Neurotic Disorders/diagnosis , Neurotic Disorders/psychology , Predictive Value of Tests , Self Efficacy , Surveys and Questionnaires
7.
J Pain ; 4(1): 40-5, 2003 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14622726

ABSTRACT

This study examined (1) the effects of sex-related stereotypes in commonly experienced, potentially painful events and (2) differences in events representing the worst pain sensation imaginable for the typical woman and the typical man. Undergraduates (63 women and 54 men) completed the Situational Pain Questionnaire as the typical woman would and as the typical man would. The participants also answered 2 open-ended questions regarding the worst pain sensation imaginable for the typical woman and for the typical man. Our findings demonstrate that sex-related stereotypes extend to common pain events and that men and women expected that men would report less pain for common pain events than women. This suggests a gender-role related learning history that is relatively consistent for both sexes. The worst pain sensation imaginable was perceived to be different for typical men and women. Both sexes chose injury as the class of events men would find most painful and childbirth and menstrual pain as the class of events women would find most painful. Implications of this finding for common pain scaling approaches are discussed. The results of this study were obtained from a fairly uniform group of undergraduate men and women, which may limit the generalizability of our findings.


Subject(s)
Pain Measurement , Pain Threshold , Stereotyping , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Gender Identity , Humans , Male , Sex Factors
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL