Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 24
Filter
2.
Sleep Med ; 112: 246-255, 2023 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37925851

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVES: There are racial and ethnic disparities in sleep duration, with members of historically marginalized groups typically reporting shorter sleep than White Americans. This study examines subjective social status (SSS) as a moderator, and variation in ideal sleep norms as a mediator, of differences in sleep duration between racial/ethnic groups. METHODS: Asian, Black, Hispanic, and non-Hispanic White respondents in an online survey reported their typical weeknight and weekend-night sleep duration, along with estimates of ideal sleep duration norms. Objective and subjective indicators of socioeconomic status were also assessed. A conditional process analysis was used to examine whether racial or ethnic differences in sleep duration were a) moderated by SSS and b) mediated by ideal sleep duration norms. RESULTS: Racial/ethnic disparities in sleep duration varied by group. Hispanic participants reported shorter weeknight sleep than White participants. In Asian and Black participants, shorter weeknight sleep relative to White participants was only observed at medium (Black) or high (Black and Asian) levels of SSS. Shorter norms for ideal sleep duration partially mediated differences in sleep duration between Black and White adults, but not the other racial/ethnic groups. There was no evidence of moderated mediation. Neither income nor education moderated racial/ethnic disparities in sleep duration. CONCLUSIONS: Racial and ethnic disparities in sleep duration may partially depend on SSS. Continued research into moderators and mediators of racial/ethnic differences in sleep duration is warranted.


Subject(s)
Health Status Disparities , Racial Groups , Sleep , Adult , Humans , Hispanic or Latino , Self Report , Social Class , Black or African American
3.
Cogn Emot ; 37(3): 453-465, 2023.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36794332

ABSTRACT

Repetitive negative thinking (RNT) describes a recursive, unproductive pattern of thought that is commonly observed in individuals who experience anxiety and depression. Past research on RNT has primarily relied on self-report, which fails to capture the potential mechanisms that underlie the persistence of maladaptive thought. We investigated whether RNT may be maintained by a negatively biased semantic network. The present study used a modified free association task to assess state RNT. Following the presentation of a valenced (positive, neutral, negative) cue word, participants generated a series of free associates, which allowed for the dynamic progression of responses. State RNT was conceptualised as the length of consecutive, negatively valenced free associates (i.e. chains). Participants also completed two self-report measures that assessed trait RNT and trait negative affect. Within a structural equation model, negative (but not positive or neutral) response chain length positively predicted trait RNT and negative affect, and this was only the case for positive (but not negative or neutral) cue words. These results suggest that RNT tendencies may be reflected in semantic retrieval and can be assessed without self-report.


Subject(s)
Pessimism , Humans , Anxiety , Anxiety Disorders , Self Report , Attention , Surveys and Questionnaires
4.
Psychol Aging ; 37(5): 557-574, 2022 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35604697

ABSTRACT

Changes in brain connectivity patterns as a function of age have been recently proposed to underlie differences in cognitive abilities between young and older adults. These shifts track patterns of increased functional coupling between the executive control network (ECN)-a network of prefrontal and parietal areas that is broadly implicated in externally directed attention and cognitive control-and default mode network (DMN) regions-most commonly associated with internally directed cognitive activity. Although age-related changes in ECN-DMN coupling are well characterized, the contributions of the salience network are less clear. Furthermore, given the salience network's crucial role in arbitrating ECN-DMN functional connectivity, it is important to understand its contribution throughout the adult lifespan. Here, we used the data from a large cohort (N = 547) of participants from the Cambridge Centre for Ageing and Neuroscience (Cam-CAN) database (18-88 years old) to investigate first whether resting-state ECN-DMN functional connectivity predicts age. We further examined how connectivity between ECN, DMN, and salience network regions impacts the hypothesized age-related increased connectivity between ECN and DMN areas. Multiple regression analyses revealed that connectivity between dorsolateral and ventromedial prefrontal cortex and parietal regions, including the precuneus, accounted for a significant portion of age variability and that the inclusion of connectivity between orbitofrontal insula, anterior insula, and anterior cingulate regions of the salience network improved the models' explanatory power. Additional age cohort analyses further highlighted that these relationships vary across the lifespan. We discuss how these findings expand on our current understanding of the variations in large-scale intrinsic network connectivity as a function of healthy aging. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).


Subject(s)
Aging , Brain , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Brain/diagnostic imaging , Brain Mapping , Humans , Longevity , Neural Pathways
5.
Sleep Health ; 8(2): 216-224, 2022 04.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34840105

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVES: Understanding the association between sleep-related beliefs and behaviors may be useful in improving sleep health in the general population. This study examines false beliefs about sleep and their associations with self-reported sleep and related behaviors. METHODS: Respondents in an online survey indicated the degree to which they agreed with 20 statements previously identified as sleep myths by experts in the field. A total sleep myths score was calculated for each participant, with higher scores reflecting greater false beliefs. Sociodemographic factors, behaviors, and knowledge related to sleep were also assessed. RESULTS: Total sample size was 1120 adults residing in the United States (51.5% female; M age = 47.22). Overall, belief in sleep myths was relatively common, with 10 of 20 false statements endorsed by at least 50% of the sample. Sleep myth scores varied by sociodemographic factors, including age, gender, socioeconomic indicators, and region of residence. Higher sleep myth scores were associated with greater inconsistency in bedtimes (odds ratio: 1.07 [1.04-1.09]), more frequent napping (odds ratio: 1.11 [1.09-1.14]), more in-bed activities (ß = 0.35, p < .001), engaging in behaviors incompatible with sleep hygiene recommendations (ß = 0.24, p < .001), and perceiving fewer consequences of insufficient sleep (ß = -0.13, p < .001). Those endorsing more myths reported shorter sleep on non-worknights (ß = -0.09, p = .01) but not on worknights. CONCLUSIONS: Belief in sleep myths is related to sleep health behavior and may be a modifiable target for intervention.


Subject(s)
Sleep Hygiene , Sleep , Adult , Female , Health Behavior , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Surveys and Questionnaires , United States/epidemiology
6.
Cogn Res Princ Implic ; 6(1): 76, 2021 11 27.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34837587

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: The term "continued influence effect" (CIE) refers to the phenomenon that discredited and obsolete information continues to affect behavior and beliefs. The practical relevance of this work is particularly apparent as we confront fake news everyday. Thus, an important question becomes, how can we mitigate the continued influence of misinformation? Decades of research have identified several factors that contribute to the CIE reduction, but few have reported successful elimination. Across three studies, we evaluated the relative contribution of three factors (i.e., targeting the misinformation, providing an alternative explanation, and relative importance of the misinformation content) to the reduction of the CIE. RESULTS: Across three studies and two different CIE measures, we found that alternative provision consistently resulted in CIE reduction. Furthermore, under certain conditions, the combination of alternative inclusion and direct targeting of misinformation in the correction statement resulted in successful elimination of the CIE, such that individuals who encountered that type of correction behaved similarly to baseline participants who never encountered the (mis)information. In contrast, under one CIE measure, participants who received correction statements that failed to include those elements referenced the (mis)information as frequently as baseline participants who never encountered a correction. Finally, we delineated several component processes involved in misinformation outdating and found that the extent of outdating success varied as a function of the causality of misinformation. CONCLUSIONS: The damaging effects of fake news are undeniable, and the negative consequences are exacerbated in the digital age. Our results contribute to our understanding of how fake news persists and how we may begin to mitigate their effects.


Subject(s)
Communication , Humans
7.
Behav Brain Res ; 401: 113056, 2021 03 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33290756

ABSTRACT

Increased generalization between fear-inducing stimuli (e.g., looking over the edge of a tall building) and perceptually-similar neutral stimuli (e.g., an aerial photograph) is observed in all subtypes of anxiety disorders, leading to avoidance behaviors that feed forward from the feared stimulus to other, seemingly unrelated stimuli. However, recent research suggests a much more nuanced relationship between generalization, discrimination, and behavior. This study seeks to extend current understanding by using a mnemonic discrimination task to explore the relationship between risk for anxiety and differences in mnemonic discrimination abilities. Participants self-reported trait anxiety and behavioral inhibition (a temperamental construct linked to risk for anxiety), and also completed a memory task. After incidental encoding of color photographs of neutral everyday objects, participants performed a surprise recognition task, where they categorized each test image as "old" (identical to a previously viewed image), "similar" (new but perceptually-similar to a studied image, with half the images being highly similar and the other half being less similar to the studied images), or "new" (new and perceptually-dissimilar to studied images). We found that those with high behavioral inhibition are more successful at discriminating between previously seen "old" items from highly similar items. In contrast, those with high trait anxiousness are less successful at the same kind of discrimination. Interestingly, these relationships were not apparent in low similarity items. Our data suggest that behavioral inhibition and trait anxiety may be associated with unique aspects of individual differences in mnemonic discrimination abilities.


Subject(s)
Anxiety/physiopathology , Discrimination Learning/physiology , Individuality , Inhibition, Psychological , Recognition, Psychology/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Disease Susceptibility , Female , Humans , Male , Young Adult
9.
Aging Clin Exp Res ; 32(11): 2339-2347, 2020 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31898168

ABSTRACT

Executive functions (EF) are a grouping of cognitive abilities essential for daily life. Previous research has shown that physical activity (PA) may in fact preserve EF in older adults, but the link between sedentary behavior (SB) and cognitive ability has been less explored. The purpose of this study was to examine the relationship between SB and cognition (executive function and memory) in older adults. Seventy five older adults (74.6 ± 9 years) self-reported their sedentary time (ST) and PA, as well as EF ability (paper-based measure of EF). Participants also completed several performance-based measures of EF and a memory task. Older adults who were less sedentary had superior EF and memory (e.g., Stroop time was significantly faster in less sedentary adults (34.7 s ± 1.9) compared to more sedentary adults (39.6 s ± 1.8), p = .02). Regression analysis showed that total ST was associated with several measures of EF after adjusting for age, and physical activity (e.g., Stroop time ß = .005 (.002, .009). Less cognitively demanding SB (TV viewing and napping) was associated with worse performance on most EF and in the memory task. Performing a hobby was also associated with lower levels of EF and memory. For example, the building times for the Lego task were positively related to napping (r2 = .34), watching TV (r2 = .27), and performing a hobby (r2 = .46). Associations of ST with cognitive abilities were more pronounced in older adults who engaged in less PA. These results suggest that SB may play an important role in cognitive abilities of older adults. Longitudinal studies using performance-based assessments of EF are needed. Lara Coelho and Kayla Hauck contributed equally to the manuscript.


Subject(s)
Cognition , Sedentary Behavior , Aged , Executive Function , Exercise , Humans , Memory
10.
Cogn Neuropsychol ; 37(1-2): 97-141, 2020.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31739752

ABSTRACT

Neuropsychological studies from the past century have associated damage to the ventromedial frontal lobes (VMF) with impairments in a variety of domains, including memory, executive function, emotion, social cognition, and valuation. A central question in the literature is whether these seemingly distinct functions are subserved by different sub-regions within the VMF, or whether VMF supports a broader cognitive process that is crucial to these varied domains. In this comprehensive review of the neuropsychological literature from the last two decades, we present a qualitative synthesis of 184 papers that have examined the psychological impairments that result from VMF damage. We discuss these findings in the context of several theoretical frameworks and advocate for the view that VMF is critical for the formation and representation of schema and cognitive maps.


Subject(s)
Frontal Lobe/physiopathology , Neuropsychological Tests/standards , Female , Humans , Male
11.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28728468

ABSTRACT

The observation that older adults show enhanced cognition for emotionally positive information has been labeled the positivity effect (Reed, Chan, & Mikels, 2014). According to the Socioemotional Selectivity Theory (SST, Carstensen, 1991), a prominent lifespan development theory, cognition is strongly influenced by motivational goals, and these goals are impacted by subjective time perspective. Although the positivity effect is most commonly observed in older adults, as age usually co-varies with time perspective, the SST posits that time perspective, not age, is the key explanatory factor of positivity. We examined the effects of these predictors on positivity in an episodic memory task in younger and older adults and found that age, not time perspective, was a key predictor of memory positivity. Our results add to the growing literature that challenge the notion that time perspective is the driving force behind age-related differences in emotional processing and functioning.


Subject(s)
Aging/psychology , Emotions , Memory , Time Perception , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Humans , Middle Aged , Models, Neurological , Perception , Visual Perception , Young Adult
12.
Cognition ; 129(3): 637-51, 2013 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24103774

ABSTRACT

What do perceptually bistable figures, sentences vulnerable to misinterpretation and the Stroop task have in common? Although seemingly disparate, they all contain elements of conflict or ambiguity. Consequently, in order to monitor a fluctuating percept, reinterpret sentence meaning, or say "blue" when the word RED is printed in blue ink, individuals must regulate attention and engage cognitive control. According to the Conflict Monitoring Theory (Botvinick, Braver, Barch, Carter, & Cohen, 2001), the detection of conflict automatically triggers cognitive control mechanisms, which can enhance resolution of subsequent conflict, namely, "conflict adaptation." If adaptation reflects the recruitment of domain-general processes, then conflict detection in one domain should facilitate conflict resolution in an entirely different domain. We report two novel findings: (i) significant conflict adaptation from a syntactic to a non-syntactic domain and (ii) from a perceptual to a verbal domain, providing strong evidence that adaptation is mediated by domain-general cognitive control.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Psychological/physiology , Conflict, Psychological , Executive Function/physiology , Functional Neuroimaging/methods , Stroop Test , Adult , Functional Neuroimaging/instrumentation , Humans , Language , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Young Adult
13.
Mem Cognit ; 39(5): 778-90, 2011 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21286899

ABSTRACT

Studies of implicit memory for novel associations have focused primarily on verbal materials and have highlighted the contribution of conceptually unitized representations to such priming. Using pictorial stimuli in a perceptual identification task, we examined whether new association priming can occur at a purely perceptual level. By manipulating the spatial contiguity of stimuli, we also evaluated whether such priming requires the creation of perceptually unitized representations. Finally, we examined the status of such priming in aging. In Experiment 1, we found that spatial contiguity of stimuli is not necessary for novel pictorial association priming to emerge, although such contiguity does enhance the magnitude of associative priming. In Experiment 2, we found that new association priming is age invariant, regardless of spatial contiguity. In Experiment 3, we provide additional evidence that pictorial association priming is perceptually based. These findings expand the scope and delineate the conditions of novel association priming and inform theories about the nature of implicit memory for new associations.


Subject(s)
Aging/psychology , Association Learning , Cues , Memory, Short-Term , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Adult , Aged , Concept Formation , Discrimination Learning , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Orientation , Practice, Psychological , Semantics , Young Adult
14.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc ; 16(6): 1006-17, 2010 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20630119

ABSTRACT

Several prominent models of confabulation characterize the syndrome as a failure in controlled aspects of memory retrieval, such as pre-retrieval cue specification and post-retrieval monitoring. These models have been generated primarily in the context of studies of autobiographical memory retrieval. Less research has focused on the existence and mechanisms of semantic confabulation. We examined whether confabulation extends to the semantic domain, and if so, whether it could be understood as a monitoring failure. We focus on post-retrieval monitoring by using a verification task that minimizes cue specification demands. We used the semantic illusion paradigm that elicits erroneous endorsement of misleading statements (e.g., "Two animals of each kind were brought onto the Ark by Moses before the great flood") even in controls, despite their knowing the correct answer (e.g., Noah). Monitoring demands were manipulated by varying semantic overlap between target and foils, ranging from high semantic overlap to unrelated. We found that semantic overlap modulated the magnitude of semantic illusion in all groups. Compared to controls, both confabulators and non-confabulators had greater difficulty monitoring semantically related foils; however, elevated endorsement of unrelated foils was unique to confabulators. We interpret our findings in the context of a two-process model of post-retrieval monitoring.


Subject(s)
Confusion/psychology , Illusions/psychology , Memory Disorders/psychology , Semantics , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Brain Mapping , Confusion/complications , Confusion/diagnostic imaging , Female , Follow-Up Studies , Humans , Illusions/physiology , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Male , Memory Disorders/complications , Memory Disorders/diagnostic imaging , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Tomography, X-Ray Computed/methods
15.
Cogn Neuropsychol ; 26(6): 527-67, 2009 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20183014

ABSTRACT

Patients with focal lesions to the left inferior frontal gyrus (LIFG; BA 44/45) exhibit difficulty with language production and comprehension tasks, although the nature of their impairments has been somewhat difficult to characterize. No reported cases suggest that these patients are Broca's aphasics in the classic agrammatic sense. Recent case studies, however, do reveal a consistent pattern of deficit regarding their general cognitive processes: they are reliably impaired on tasks in which conflicting representations must be resolved by implementing top-down cognitive control (e.g., Stroop; memory tasks involving proactive interference). In the present study, we ask whether the language production and comprehension impairments displayed by a patient with circumscribed LIFG damage can best be understood within a general conflict resolution deficit account. We focus on one patient in particular--patient I.G.--and discuss the implications for language processing abilities as a consequence of a general cognitive control disorder. We compared I.G. and other frontal patients to age-matched control participants across four experiments. Experiment 1 tested participants' general conflict resolution abilities within a modified working memory paradigm in an attempt to replicate prior case study findings. We then tested language production abilities on tasks of picture naming (Experiment 2) and verbal fluency (Experiment 3), tasks that generated conflict at the semantic and/or conceptual levels. Experiment 4 tested participants' sentence processing and comprehension abilities using both online (eye movement) and offline measures. In this task, participants carried out spoken instructions containing a syntactic ambiguity, in which early interpretation commitments had to be overridden in order to recover an alternative, intended analysis of sentence meaning. Comparisons of I.G.'s performance with frontal and healthy control participants supported the following claim: I.G. suffers from a general conflict resolution impairment, which affects his ability to produce and comprehend language under specific conditions--namely, when semantic, conceptual, and/or syntactic representations compete and must be resolved.


Subject(s)
Language Disorders/diagnosis , Language Disorders/physiopathology , Memory Disorders/diagnosis , Memory Disorders/physiopathology , Prefrontal Cortex/pathology , Prefrontal Cortex/physiopathology , Functional Laterality/physiology , Humans , Language Disorders/epidemiology , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Memory Disorders/epidemiology , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Severity of Illness Index
16.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 21(5): 938-44, 2009 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18702596

ABSTRACT

We evaluated whether prior semantic knowledge would enhance episodic learning in amnesia. Subjects studied prices that are either congruent or incongruent with prior price knowledge for grocery and household items and then performed a forced-choice recognition test for the studied prices. Consistent with a previous report, healthy controls' performance was enhanced by price knowledge congruency; however, only a subset of amnesic patients experienced the same benefit. Whereas patients with relatively intact semantic systems, as measured by an anatomical measure (i.e., lesion involvement of anterior and lateral temporal lobes), experienced a significant congruency benefit, patients with compromised semantic systems did not experience a congruency benefit. Our findings suggest that when prior knowledge structures are intact, they can support acquisition of new episodic information by providing frameworks into which such information can be incorporated.


Subject(s)
Amnesia/pathology , Concept Formation/physiology , Knowledge , Semantics , Temporal Lobe/physiopathology , Verbal Learning/physiology , Adult , Aged , Amnesia/physiopathology , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Middle Aged , Temporal Lobe/pathology , Tomography, X-Ray Computed/methods
17.
Neuropsychologia ; 45(11): 2589-97, 2007 Jun 18.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17433382

ABSTRACT

In this study, we examined the role of the hippocampus in relational memory by comparing item recognition performance in amnesic patients with medial temporal lobe (MTL) damage and their matched controls. Specifically, we investigated the contribution of associative memory to item recognition using a cued recognition paradigm. Control subjects studied cue-target pairs once, whereas amnesic patients studied cue-target pairs six times. Following study, subjects made recognition judgments about targets that were presented either alone (no cue), with the originally presented cue (same cue), or with a cue that had been presented with a different target (recombined cue). Controls had higher recognition scores in the same cue than in the recombined cue condition, indicating that they benefited from the associative information provided by the same cue. By contrast, amnesic patients did not. This was true even for a subgroup of patients whose recognition performance in the no cue condition was matched to that of the controls. These data provide further support for the idea that the hippocampus plays a critical role in relational memory, even when associative information need not be retrieved intentionally.


Subject(s)
Amnesia/physiopathology , Association Learning/physiology , Hippocampus/physiology , Recognition, Psychology/physiology , Temporal Lobe/physiology , Adult , Aged , Amnesia/pathology , Case-Control Studies , Cues , Female , Hippocampus/physiopathology , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Middle Aged , Neural Pathways/physiology , Neural Pathways/physiopathology , Neuropsychological Tests , Organ Size , Reference Values , Temporal Lobe/pathology , Temporal Lobe/physiopathology
18.
Cogn Behav Neurol ; 19(4): 225-36, 2006 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17159621

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: We explored the constituents of the graphemic buffer in a patient with acquired dysgraphia and tested the hypothesis that the graphemic buffer is composed of 2 dissociable components: letter selection and letter assembly. BACKGROUND: Research on dysgraphia has established the graphemic buffer as a component of the spelling mechanism, and the buffer is considered a short-term memory store that is critical for letter production. However, little is known about the components within the buffer. METHOD: We devised 2 spelling tasks that rely differentially on letter selection and letter assembly. In the selection task, our patient produced the letters that composed a target word, but she did not have to provide serial position information. In the assembly task, B.H. was given all the letters of a target word and was asked to spell the word by arranging the letters in the proper serial order. RESULTS: Compared to spelling to dictation, our patient did not benefit from being given letter identity information (ie, assembly task), but her performance improved significantly when position information was available (ie, selection task). CONCLUSIONS: Based on these data, and the comparison of her performance with another dysgraphic patient, we propose that the graphemic buffer engages in both letter selection and letter assembly.


Subject(s)
Agraphia/physiopathology , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Verbal Behavior , Verbal Learning , Aged , Agraphia/psychology , Female , Humans , Neuropsychological Tests , Psycholinguistics
19.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 18(2): 267-77, 2006 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16494686

ABSTRACT

A number of theories about the evolution of language posit a close (and perhaps causal) relationship between tool use and speech. Consistent with this idea, neuroimaging studies have found that tool knowledge retrieval activates not only a region of the left premotor cortex involved in hand action, but also an adjacent region that is typically described as a language center. We examined whether this pattern of activation is best described as the result of a single process, related to both action and language, or the result of two independent processes. We identified two distinct neural components that jointly contribute to this response: a posterior region centered in the premotor cortex, which responds to motor knowledge retrieval, and an anterior region centered in the left frontal operculum, which responds to lexical competition. Crucial to the interpretation of the premotor response, individual variation in motor experience was highly correlated with the magnitude of the response in the premotor cortex, but not in the prefrontal cortex.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping , Frontal Lobe/physiology , Functional Laterality/physiology , Mental Recall/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Semantics , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Frontal Lobe/blood supply , Humans , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted/methods , Knowledge , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Male , Motor Cortex/physiology , Oxygen/blood , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Photic Stimulation/methods , Prefrontal Cortex/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology
20.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 17(12): 1855-70, 2005 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16356324

ABSTRACT

Retrieval of conceptual information from action pictures causes greater activation than from object pictures bilaterally in human motion areas (MT/MST) and nearby temporal regions. By contrast, retrieval of conceptual information from action words causes greater activation in left middle and superior temporal gyri, anterior and dorsal to the MT/MST. We performed two fMRI experiments to replicate and extend these findings regarding action words. In the first experiment, subjects performed conceptual judgments of action and object words under conditions that stressed visual semantic information. Under these conditions, action words again activated posterior temporal regions close to, but not identical with, the MT/MST. In the second experiment, we included conceptual judgments of manipulable object words in addition to judgments of action and animal words. Both action and manipulable object judgments caused greater activity than animal judgments in the posterior middle temporal gyrus. Both of these experiments support the hypothesis that middle temporal gyrus activation is related to accessing conceptual information about motion attributes, rather than alternative accounts on the basis of lexical or grammatical factors. Furthermore, these experiments provide additional support for the notion of a concrete to abstract gradient of motion representations with the lateral occipito-temporal cortex, extending anterior and dorsal from the MT/MST towards the peri-sylvian cortex.


Subject(s)
Temporal Lobe/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Brain Mapping , Female , Humans , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Memory/physiology , Motion Perception/physiology , Occipital Lobe/physiology , Photic Stimulation , Psycholinguistics , Psychomotor Performance/physiology
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...