Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 51
Filter
1.
Sleep ; 47(3)2024 Mar 11.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38219041

ABSTRACT

STUDY OBJECTIVES: To investigate whether neurobehavioral impairments are exacerbated during successive cycles of sleep restriction and recovery in young adults, and whether a variable short sleep schedule can mitigate these impairments relative to a stable one. METHODS: Fifty-two healthy young adults (25 males, aged: 21-28) were randomly assigned to the stable short sleep group, the variable short sleep group, or the control group in this laboratory-based study. They underwent two baseline nights of 8-hour time-in-bed (TIB), followed by two cycles of "weekday" sleep opportunity manipulation and "weekend" recovery (8-hour TIB). During each manipulation period, the stable short sleep and the control groups received 6- and 8-hour TIBs each night respectively, while the variable short sleep group received 8-hour, 4-hour, 8-hour, 4-hour, and 6-hour TIBs from the first to the fifth night. Neurobehavioral functions were assessed five times each day. RESULTS: The stable short sleep group showed faster vigilance deterioration in the second week of sleep restriction as compared to the first. This effect was not observed in the variable short sleep group. Subjective alertness and practice-based improvement in processing speed were attenuated in both short sleep groups. CONCLUSIONS: In young adults, more variable short sleep schedules incorporating days of prophylactic or recovery sleep might mitigate compounding vigilance deficits resulting from recurrent cycles of sleep restriction. However, processing speed and subjective sleepiness were still impaired in both short sleep schedules. Getting sufficient sleep consistently is the only way to ensure optimal neurobehavioral functioning. CLINICAL TRIAL: Performance, Mood, and Brain and Metabolic Functions During Different Sleep Schedules (STAVAR), https://www.clinicaltrials.gov/study/NCT04731662, NCT04731662.


Subject(s)
Sleep Deprivation , Sleep Duration , Adult , Humans , Male , Young Adult , Polysomnography , Sleep , Sleep Deprivation/complications , Time Factors , Wakefulness , Female
2.
Behav Sleep Med ; 21(6): 774-786, 2023 11 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36594607

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVES: To quantify school-age children's sleep and parent-associated factors on weekdays and weekends in Singapore, and investigate school-related and parent-related factors associated with short sleep. METHODS: In an online survey, 251 parents with a child aged 7-12 y in Singapore reported their child's sleep duration and school start time. Parent-related factors including sleep hygiene (e.g., parent-set bedtime), sleep priority (the amount of sleep respondents allowed their children to trade for other activities), and both parents' sleep durations, were also reported. RESULTS: The prevalence of short sleep among the children was 64.5% on weekdays and 19.5% on weekends. Children's sleep duration increased from 8.42 h on weekdays to 9.45 h on weekends (p < .001). Relative to weekdays, on weekends, parents showed similar increases in sleep durations (p < .001), imposed poorer sleep hygiene on their children (reduced likelihood of setting bedtimes and increased pre-bedtime electronic device use; p < .001), and allowed their children to trade more sleep for interacting with family and friends, social media, gaming, and TV / videos (p < .001). Shorter sleep duration in children was significantly associated with earlier school start time (B = 0.80, p = .02) and poorer sleep hygiene on weekdays (B = 0.16, p < .001), but lower sleep priority (B = 0.05, p = .002) and shorter parental sleep duration on weekends (maternal: B = 0.18, p < .001, paternal: B = 0.17, p = .002). CONCLUSIONS: Delaying school start times may be effective in increasing school-age children's sleep duration on weekdays, while family-based interventions designed to enhance sleep hygiene, priority of sleep over other activities, and parents' sleep durations can benefit children's sleep duration on both weekdays and weekends.


Subject(s)
Sleep Duration , Sleep Wake Disorders , Humans , Child , Sleep , Time Factors , Parents , Surveys and Questionnaires
3.
J Adolesc Health ; 72(3): 460-470, 2023 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36528521

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: Adherence to 24-hour movement guidelines of ≥60 minutes of physical activity, ≤2 hours of screen time, and 9-11 hours of sleep has been shown to benefit cognitive, physical, and psychosocial health in children and young adolescents aged 5-13 years. However, these findings have mostly been based on cross-sectional studies or relatively small samples and the associations between adherence to guidelines and brain structure remain to be evaluated. METHODS: Data from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development℠ (ABCD) study of 10,574 early adolescents aged 9-14 years from September 2016 to January 2021 were used to examine whether adherence to 24-hour movement guidelines benefits cognition (general cognitive ability, executive function, and learning/memory assessed by the National Institutes of Health Toolbox neurocognitive battery), body mass index, psychosocial health (internalizing, externalizing, and total problems from the parent-reported Child Behavior Checklist), and magnetic resonance imaging-derived brain morphometric measures at baseline (T1), ∼2 years later (T2), and longitudinally from T1 to T2 (T2-T1). Multivariable linear mixed models were used, with adjustments for sociodemographic confounders. Time elapsed and T1 outcome measures were also controlled for in longitudinal models. RESULTS: Better cognitive scores, fewer behavioral problems, lower adiposity levels, and greater gray matter volumes were observed in those who met both sleep and screen time recommendations compared to those who met none. Longitudinal follow-up further supports these findings; participants who met both recommendations at T1 and T2 evidenced better outcome measures than those who met none. DISCUSSION: These findings support consideration of integrated rather than isolated movement recommendations across the day in early adolescence for better cognitive, physical and psychosocial health. Although the associations between physical activity and health indicators were less consistent in this study, the significant findings from sleep and screen time demonstrate the importance of considering movement recommendations in an integrated rather than isolated manner for adolescent health. It is recommended that movement behaviors be simultaneously targeted for better developmental outcomes.


Subject(s)
Pediatric Obesity , Sedentary Behavior , Child , Humans , Adolescent , Cross-Sectional Studies , Cognition , Sleep , Guideline Adherence , Brain
4.
Front Behav Neurosci ; 16: 911994, 2022.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36062257

ABSTRACT

Laboratory-based sleep manipulations show asymmetries between positive and negative affect, but say little about how more specific moods might change. We report extensive analyzes of items from the Positive and Negative Affect Scale (PANAS) during days following nights of chronic sleep restriction (6 h sleep opportunity), during 40 h of acute sleep deprivation under constant routine conditions, and during a week-long forced desynchrony protocol in which participants lived on a 28-h day. Living in the laboratory resulted in medium effects sizes on all positive moods (Attentiveness, General Positive Affect, Joviality, Assuredness), with a general deterioration as the days wore on. These effects were not found with negative moods. Sleep restriction reduced some positive moods, particularly Attentiveness (also General Positive), and increased Hostility. A burden of chronic sleep loss also led to lower positive moods when participants confronted the acute sleep loss challenge, and all positive moods, as well as Fearfulness, General Negative Affect and Hostility were affected. Sleeping at atypical circadian phases resulted in mood changes: all positive moods reduced, Hostility and General Negative Affect increased. Deteriorations increased the further participants slept from their typical nocturnal sleep. In most cases the changes induced by chronic or acute sleep loss or mistimed sleep waxed or waned across the waking day, with linear or various non-linear trends best fitting these time-awake-based changes. While extended laboratory stays do not emulate the fluctuating emotional demands of everyday living, these findings demonstrate that even in controlled settings mood changes systematically as sleep is shortened or mistimed.

5.
Sleep Med Rev ; 65: 101666, 2022 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36041284

ABSTRACT

Naps are increasingly considered a means to boost cognitive performance. We quantified the cognitive effects of napping in 60 samples from 54 studies. 52 samples evaluated memory. We first evaluated effect sizes for all tests together, before separately assessing their effects on memory, vigilance, speed of processing and executive function. We next examined whether nap effects were moderated by study features of age, nap length, nap start time, habituality and prior sleep restriction. Naps showed significant benefits for the total aggregate of cognitive tests (Cohen's d = 0.379, CI95 = 0.296-0.462). Significant domain specific effects were present for declarative (Cohen's d = 0.376, CI95 = 0.269-0.482) and procedural memory (Cohen's d = 0.494, CI95 = 0.301-0.686), vigilance (Cohen's d = 0.610, CI95 = 0.291-0.929) and speed of processing (Cohen's d = 0.211, CI95 = 0.052-0.369). There were no significant moderation effects of any of the study features. Nap effects were of comparable magnitude across subgroups of each of the 5 moderators (Q values = 0.009 to 8.572, p values > 0.116). Afternoon naps have a small to medium benefit over multiple cognitive tests. These effects transcend age, nap duration and tentatively, habituality and prior nocturnal sleep.


Subject(s)
Sleep , Wakefulness , Humans , Cognition , Executive Function
6.
Sleep Adv ; 3(1): zpac040, 2022.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37193393

ABSTRACT

Study Objectives: We attempted to predict vigilance performance in adolescents during partial sleep deprivation using task summary metrics and drift diffusion modelling measures (DDM) derived from baseline vigilance performance. Methods: In the Need for Sleep studies, 57 adolescents (age = 15-19 years) underwent two baseline nights of 9-h time-in-bed (TIB), followed by two cycles of weekday sleep-restricted nights (5-h or 6.5-h TIB) and weekend recovery nights (9-h TIB). Vigilance was assessed daily with the Psychomotor Vigilance Task (PVT), with the number of lapses (response times ≥ 500 ms) as the primary outcome measure. The two DDM predictors were drift rate, which quantifies the speed of information accumulation and determines how quickly an individual derives a decision response, and non-decision time range, which indicates within-subject variation in physical, non-cognitive responding, e.g. motor actions. Results: In the first week of sleep curtailment, faster accumulation of lapses was significantly associated with more lapses at baseline (p = .02), but not the two baseline DDM metrics: drift and non-decision time range (p > .07). On the other hand, faster accumulation of lapses and greater increment in reaction time variability from the first to the second week of sleep restriction were associated with lower drift (p < .007) at baseline. Conclusions: Among adolescents, baseline PVT lapses can predict inter-individual differences in vigilance vulnerability during 1 week of sleep restriction on weekdays, while drift more consistently predicts vulnerability during more weeks of sleep curtailment. Clinical Trial Information: Effects of Napping in Sleep-Restricted Adolescents, clinicaltrials.gov, NCT02838095. The Cognitive and Metabolic Effects of Sleep Restriction in Adolescents (NFS4), clinicaltrials.gov, NCT03333512.

7.
Sleep ; 45(1)2022 01 11.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34379782

ABSTRACT

STUDY OBJECTIVES: Gains in cognitive test performance that occur during adolescence are associated with brain maturation. Cortical thinning and reduced sleep slow wave activity (SWA) are markers of such developmental changes. Here we investigate whether they mediate age-related improvements in cognition. METHODS: 109 adolescents aged 15-19 years (49 males) underwent magnetic resonance imaging, polysomnography (PSG), and a battery of cognitive tasks within a 2-month time window. Cognitive tasks assessed nonverbal intelligence, sustained attention, speed of processing and working memory and executive function. To minimize the effect of sleep history on SWA and cognitive performance, PSG and test batteries were administered only after at least 8 nights of 9-h time-in-bed (TIB) sleep opportunity. RESULTS: Age-related improvements in speed of processing (r = 0.33, p = 0.001) and nonverbal intelligence (r = 0.24, p = 0.01) domains were observed. These cognitive changes were associated with reduced cortical thickness, particularly in bilateral temporoparietal regions (rs = -0.21 to -0.45, ps < 0.05), as well as SWA (r = -0.35, p < 0.001). Serial mediation models found that ROIs in the middle/superior temporal cortices, together with SWA mediated the age-related improvement observed on cognition. CONCLUSIONS: During adolescence, age-related improvements in cognition are mediated by reductions in cortical thickness and sleep SWA.


Subject(s)
Cerebral Cortical Thinning , Sleep , Adolescent , Adult , Cognition , Electroencephalography/methods , Executive Function , Humans , Male , Polysomnography , Young Adult
8.
Sleep ; 44(9)2021 09 13.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33755184

ABSTRACT

STUDY OBJECTIVES: Existing literature suggests that sleep-dependent memory consolidation is impaired in older adults but may be preserved for personally relevant information. Prospective memory (PM) involves remembering to execute future intentions in a timely manner and has behavioral importance. As previous work suggests that N3 sleep is important for PM in young adults, we investigated if the role of N3 sleep in PM consolidation would be maintained in older adults. METHODS: Forty-nine young adults (mean age ± SD: 21.8 ± 1.61 years) and 49 healthy older adults (mean age ± SD: 65.7 ± 6.30 years) were randomized into sleep and wake groups. After a semantic categorization task, participants encoded intentions comprising four related and four unrelated cue-action pairs. They were instructed to remember to perform these actions in response to cue words presented during a second semantic categorization task 12 h later that encompassed either daytime wake (09:00 am-21:00 pm) or overnight sleep with polysomnography (21:00 pm-09:00 am). RESULTS: The significant condition × age group × relatedness interaction suggested that the sleep benefit on PM intentions varied according to age group and relatedness (p = 0.01). For related intentions, sleep relative to wake benefitted young adults' performance (p < 0.001) but not older adults (p = 0.30). For unrelated intentions, sleep did not improve PM for either age group. While post-encoding N3 was significantly associated with related intentions' execution in young adults (r = 0.43, p = 0.02), this relationship was not found for older adults (r = -0.07, p = 0.763). CONCLUSIONS: The age-related impairment of sleep-dependent memory consolidation extends to PM. Our findings add to an existing body of work suggesting that the link between sleep and memory is functionally weakened in older adulthood.


Subject(s)
Memory Consolidation , Memory, Episodic , Aged , Aging , Humans , Mental Recall , Sleep , Young Adult
9.
J Sleep Res ; 30(4): e13252, 2021 08.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33331126

ABSTRACT

We investigated whether variable sleep schedules might mitigate the neurobehavioural deficits induced by multiple nights of sleep restriction. In this 4-night experiment, 78 young adults (age: 18-28 years) were randomly assigned to four groups: 8888, 8666, 8846 and 8486, where each digit corresponded to time-in-bed in hours for each study night. After one baseline night of 8-hr time-in-bed, time-in-bed remained unchanged for the 8888 group, while the other groups had short sleep schedules (total time-in-bed = 18 hr) that differed in the number of time-in-bed changes. Sleep was monitored using actigraphy at home. Daytime neurobehavioural functions were assessed in the laboratory at single time points, after the baseline night, and again after 3 nights of the sleep manipulation period. For sustained attention, the 8888 group responded faster in the Psychomotor Vigilance Task after the manipulation period (p = .01), while responses became slower for the less variable sleep schedules (8666 and 8846; p < .01), but not the most variable sleep schedule (8486; p = .14). Processing speed also improved in the 8888 group and the variable 8846 and 8486 groups (p < .01), but not in the stable 8666 group (p = .09). Furthermore, subjective sleepiness was preserved in the 8888 and, importantly, 8486 groups (p > .05), but was elevated in the 8666 and 8846 groups (p < .05). These findings suggest that when sleep opportunities are limited across multiple nights, a variable sleep schedule that allows for prophylactic and/or recovery sleep on some nights may mitigate some daytime neurobehavioural deficits as compared with a schedule with no opportunity for recovery.


Subject(s)
Sleep Deprivation/physiopathology , Sleep Deprivation/psychology , Sleep , Attention , Cognition , Female , Humans , Male , Psychomotor Performance , Wakefulness , Young Adult
10.
Sleep ; 43(12)2020 12 14.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32619240

ABSTRACT

STUDY OBJECTIVES: We compared the basic cognitive functions of adolescents undergoing split (nocturnal sleep + daytime nap) and continuous nocturnal sleep schedules when total sleep opportunity was either below or within the recommended range (i.e. 6.5 or 8 h). METHODS: Adolescent participants (age: 15-19 year) in the 8-h split (n = 24) and continuous (n = 29) sleep groups were compared with 6.5-h split and continuous sleep groups from a previous study (n = 58). These protocols involved two baseline nights (9-h time-in-bed [TIB]), 5 nights of sleep manipulation, 2 recovery nights (9-h TIB), followed by a second cycle of sleep manipulation (3 nights) and recovery (2 nights). Cognitive performance, subjective sleepiness, and mood were evaluated daily; sleep was assessed using polysomnography. RESULTS: Splitting 6.5 h of sleep with a mid-afternoon nap offered a boost to cognitive function compared to continuous nocturnal sleep. However, when total TIB across 24 h increased to 8 h, the split and continuous sleep groups performed comparably in tests evaluating vigilance, working memory, executive function, processing speed, subjective sleepiness, and mood. CONCLUSIONS: In adolescents, the effects of split sleep on basic cognitive functions vary by the amount of total sleep obtained. As long as the total sleep opportunity across 24 h is within the recommended range, students may fulfill sleep requirements by adopting a split sleep schedule consisting of a shorter period of nocturnal sleep combined with a mid-afternoon nap, without significant impact on basic cognitive functions. CLINICAL TRIAL REGISTRATION: NCT04044885.


Subject(s)
Sleep Deprivation , Sleep , Adolescent , Adult , Cognition , Humans , Polysomnography , Wakefulness , Young Adult
11.
Sleep Health ; 6(6): 758-766, 2020 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32536472

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: To investigate associations of adolescents' time spent on homework/studying with nocturnal time for sleep and depression symptoms, in a competitive academic environment. DESIGN: Cross-sectional, anonymous survey of sleep habits, school life, and health-related measures. SETTING: Eight schools in Singapore. PARTICIPANTS: Total 1225 adolescents aged 13-19 years. MEASUREMENTS: Self-reported sleep behavior and time use data were collected separately for school days and weekends. Multiple regression models were used to test covariation of time spent on homework/studying with other activities, and associations of homework/studying duration with depression symptoms. RESULTS: Time in bed for sleep and media use were inversely related with homework/studying duration on both school days and weekends, adjusting for time spent on other activities and demographic variables. Face-to-face family time and hanging out with friends were also reciprocally related with homework/studying duration on weekends. Depression scores were higher in adolescents who spent long hours on homework/studying. On school days, this was mediated by reduced time in bed for sleep. On weekends, homework/studying duration associated with depression symptoms, adjusting for time in bed and other covariates. Adolescents who spent ≥5 hours on homework/studying per day on weekends had greater symptoms of anhedonia and anxiety. CONCLUSIONS: In a competitive academic setting, adolescents who spent more time on homework/studying spent less time on sleep, media use, and social activities. Independent of effects on sleep, long hours on homework/studying on weekends may be a risk factor for depression. Reducing adolescents' workload outside of class may benefit their sleep, schoolwork-life balance, and mental well-being.


Subject(s)
Adolescent Behavior , Depression/epidemiology , Sleep , Students/psychology , Adolescent , Cross-Sectional Studies , Female , Humans , Male , Schools , Self Report , Singapore/epidemiology , Students/statistics & numerical data , Time Factors , Young Adult
12.
Sleep Health ; 6(2): 137-144, 2020 04.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31812609

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVES: Shortened sleep has negative consequences on adolescents' well-being. The present study evaluated an interactive school-based sleep education program (SEP) aimed at increasing adolescent sleep duration. DESIGN AND INTERVENTION: A cluster-randomized controlled trial with 12 clusters (classes) was used. The intervention group received a SEP and the active control group received a healthy living program (HLP). Both groups underwent a 4-week class-based education program. The SEP students learned about the importance of sleep, the barriers to getting enough sleep, and how to improve their time management to increase their sleep opportunity. The HLP students learned about various health-related topics not including sleep. PARTICIPANTS: A total of 210 students (mean age = 14.04 ± 0.32 years) were randomly assigned to the SEP (n = 102) or the HLP (n = 108) group, with 6 classes per group. MEASUREMENTS: Sleep (actigraphically measured), sleep knowledge, and time usage were assessed using linear mixed models at three time points: baseline, immediately after intervention, and 1-month follow-up. RESULTS: Sleep knowledge improved at follow-up in the SEP relative to the HLP group (p = .017). Although students were receptive of the program and self-reported the intention to create more time for sleep, no changes in sleep were found following the SEP. Some benefit may have been masked by exam preparations at the follow-up evaluation. CONCLUSIONS: Sleep education alone may not be sufficient to change sleep behavior. A combination of sleep education, starting school later, and parental involvement may be needed to encourage and enable changes in adolescent sleep duration.


Subject(s)
Health Education , School Health Services , Sleep , Students/psychology , Actigraphy , Adolescent , Follow-Up Studies , Health Knowledge, Attitudes, Practice , Humans , Male , Program Evaluation , Singapore , Students/statistics & numerical data , Time Factors , Time Management
13.
J Clin Sleep Med ; 15(9): 1337-1346, 2019 09 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31538605

ABSTRACT

STUDY OBJECTIVES: To compare the quality and consistency in sleep measurement of a consumer wearable device and a research-grade actigraph with polysomnography (PSG) in adolescents. METHODS: Fifty-eight healthy adolescents (aged 15-19 years; 30 males) underwent overnight PSG while wearing both a Fitbit Alta HR and a Philips Respironics Actiwatch 2 (AW2) for 5 nights, with either 5 hours or 6.5 hours time in bed (TIB) and for 4 nights with 9 hours TIB. AW2 data were evaluated using two different wake and immobility thresholds. Discrepancies in estimated total sleep time (TST) and wake after sleep onset (WASO) between devices and PSG, as well as epoch-by-epoch agreements in sleep/wake classification, were assessed. Fitbit-generated sleep staging was compared to PSG. RESULTS: Fitbit and AW2 under default settings similarly underestimated TST and overestimated WASO (TST: medium setting (M10) ≤ 38 minutes, Fitbit ≤ 47 minutes; WASO: M10 ≤ 38 minutes; Fitbit ≤ 42 minutes). AW2 at the high motion threshold setting provided readings closest to PSG (TST: ≤ 12 minutes; WASO: ≤ 18 minutes). Sensitivity for detecting sleep was ≥ 90% for both wearable devices and further improved to 95% by using the high threshold (H5) setting for the AW2 (0.95). Wake detection specificity was highest in Fitbit (≥ 0.88), followed by the AW2 at M10 (≥ 0.80) and H5 thresholds (≤ 0.73). In addition, Fitbit inconsistently estimated stage N1 + N2 sleep depending on TIB, underestimated stage N3 sleep (21-46 min), but was comparable to PSG for rapid eye movement sleep. Fitbit sensitivity values for the detection of N1 + N2, N3 and rapid eye movement sleep were ≥ 0.68, ≥ 0.50, and ≥ 0.72, respectively. CONCLUSIONS: A consumer-grade wearable device can measure sleep duration as well as a research actigraph. However, sleep staging would benefit from further refinement before these methods can be reliably used for adolescents. CLINICAL TRIAL REGISTRATION: Registry: ClinicalTrials.gov; Title: The Cognitive and Metabolic Effects of Sleep Restriction in Adolescents; Identifier: NCT03333512; URL: https://clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT03333512. CITATION: Lee XK, Chee NIYN, Ong JL, Teo TB, van Rijn E, Lo JC, Chee MWL. Validation of a consumer sleep wearable device with actigraphy and polysomnography in adolescents across sleep opportunity manipulations. J Clin Sleep Med. 2019;15(9):1337-1346.


Subject(s)
Actigraphy/statistics & numerical data , Sleep/physiology , Wearable Electronic Devices/statistics & numerical data , Actigraphy/methods , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Polysomnography/methods , Polysomnography/statistics & numerical data , Reproducibility of Results , Sensitivity and Specificity , Time , Young Adult
14.
Sleep Med Rev ; 47: 18-27, 2019 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31216498

ABSTRACT

Prospective memory (PM) enables us to execute previously conceived intentions at a later time and is used when remembering to call a friend or submitting a proposal on time. Evidence that sleep benefits PM is presently mixed. Further, when a benefit is observed, it is unclear if this is achieved through improvements in strategic monitoring (maintaining an intention in mind and searching for cues) or spontaneous retrieval (an automatic process occurring without preparatory attention). We conducted a meta-analysis of 24 independent samples (N = 165,432) to quantify the effect of sleep on PM and gain clarity regarding the retrieval process benefitted by sleep. Cohen's d with 95% confidence intervals (CI95) were derived using random-effects models. The benefit of sleep on PM was statistically significant and in the small to medium range (d = 0.41, CI95 = 0.25-0.56). Moreover, sleep did not appear to influence monitoring (d = -0.11, CI95 = -0.40-0.17). In contrast, the benefits of sleep are significantly greater when the likelihood of spontaneous retrieval is high (d = 0.94, CI95 = 0.44-1.44) versus low (d = 0.45, CI95 = -0.02-0.93), suggesting that sleep may leverage on spontaneous retrieval processes to improve PM. These findings inform theoretical models of sleep and PM that could sharpen strategies to improve memory function in vulnerable populations.


Subject(s)
Memory, Episodic , Sleep , Humans , Sleep/physiology
15.
J Neurosci ; 39(28): 5534-5550, 2019 07 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31109962

ABSTRACT

Healthy aging is accompanied by disruptions in the functional modular organization of the human brain. Cross-sectional studies have shown age-related reductions in the functional segregation and distinctiveness of brain networks. However, less is known about the longitudinal changes in brain functional modular organization and their associations with aging-related cognitive decline. We examined age- and aging-related changes in functional architecture of the cerebral cortex using a dataset comprising a cross-sectional healthy young cohort of 57 individuals (mean ± SD age, 23.71 ± 3.61 years, 22 males) and a longitudinal healthy elderly cohort of 72 individuals (mean ± baseline age, 68.22 ± 5.80 years, 39 males) with 2-3 time points (18-24 months apart) of task-free fMRI data. We found both cross-sectional (elderly vs young) and longitudinal (in elderly) global decreases in network segregation (decreased local efficiency), integration (decreased global efficiency), and module distinctiveness (increased participation coefficient and decreased system segregation). At the modular level, whereas cross-sectional analyses revealed higher participation coefficient across all modules in the elderly compared with young participants, longitudinal analyses revealed focal longitudinal participation coefficient increases in three higher-order cognitive modules: control network, default mode network, and salience/ventral attention network. Cross-sectionally, elderly participants also showed worse attention performance with lower local efficiency and higher mean participation coefficient, and worse global cognitive performance with higher participation coefficient in the dorsal attention/control network. These findings suggest that healthy aging is associated with whole-brain connectome-wide changes in the functional modular organization of the brain, accompanied by loss of functional segregation, particularly in higher-order cognitive networks.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Cross-sectional studies have demonstrated age-related reductions in the functional segregation and distinctiveness of brain networks. However, longitudinal aging-related changes in brain functional modular architecture and their links to cognitive decline remain relatively understudied. Using graph theoretical and community detection approaches to study task-free functional network changes in a cross-sectional young and longitudinal healthy elderly cohort, we showed that aging was associated with global declines in network segregation, integration, and module distinctiveness, and specific declines in distinctiveness of higher-order cognitive networks. Further, such functional network deterioration was associated with poorer cognitive performance cross-sectionally. Our findings suggest that healthy aging is associated with system-level changes in brain functional modular organization, accompanied by functional segregation loss particularly in higher-order networks specialized for cognition.


Subject(s)
Aging/physiology , Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Connectome , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Attention , Cerebral Cortex/growth & development , Cognition , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Middle Aged
16.
Sci Rep ; 9(1): 3415, 2019 03 04.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30833649

ABSTRACT

Prior reports on geographical differences in sleep duration have relied on samples collected at different time points with a variety of subjective instruments. Using sleep data from a total of 553,559 nights from 23,680 Fitbit users (aged 15-80y), we found objective evidence for regional disparities in sleep duration of 32-43 min between Oceanian and East Asian users on weekdays. This was primarily driven by later bedtimes in East Asians. Although users in all countries extended sleep on weekends, East Asians continued to sleep less than their Oceanian counterparts. Women generally slept more than men, and older users slept less than younger users. Reasons for shorter sleep duration in East Asians on both weekdays and weekends, across the lifespan and in both sexes remain to be investigated.


Subject(s)
Sleep/physiology , Wearable Electronic Devices , Adolescent , Adult , Age Factors , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Asian People , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Sex Factors , Surveys and Questionnaires , Time Factors , Young Adult
17.
Sleep Med ; 56: 111-116, 2019 04.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30852129

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: Sleep benefits prospective memory in young adults probably in part due to its well-established role in enhancing declarative memory, thereby facilitating retrieval of the intention content. In prior work on adolescents, we did not detect differences in prospective memory comparing five nights of sleep restriction and adequate sleep. Here, we examined whether this might be attributed to a limited role of sleep in benefiting the declarative content in this age group, and whether a sleep benefit on prospective memory would be uncovered with a shorter retention interval. METHODS: A total of 59 adolescents (mean ± standard deviation: 16.55 ± 0.94 years) were instructed to remember to press a special key in response to two target words embedded in a semantic categorization task. Memory was tested after a 12-h retention interval, which included either overnight sleep (21:00-09:00, n = 29) or daytime wakefulness (09:00-21:00, n = 30). RESULTS: We found no significant group difference in the percentage of target words correctly responded to (mean ± standard error of the mean for the sleep group: 32.76 ± 6.69%; wake group: 41.67 ± 7.61%, t = 0.88, p = 0.38). However, participants who slept recalled more target words compared to those who stayed awake (98.28 ± 1.72% vs. 86.67 ± 5.32%, t = 2.05, p < 0.05). In addition, a significantly greater proportion of sleep participants (n = 28 of 29) compared to wake participants (n = 24 of 30) recalled both target words correctly (χ2 = 3.76, p < 0.05). CONCLUSION: These findings suggest that during adolescence, sleep plays a more prominent role in improving memory for the content as compared to the execution of intentions.


Subject(s)
Intention , Memory Consolidation/physiology , Memory, Episodic , Mental Recall/physiology , Sleep/physiology , Wakefulness/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Young Adult
18.
Sleep ; 42(5)2019 05 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30753648

ABSTRACT

STUDY OBJECTIVES: Many adolescents are exposed to sleep restriction on school nights. We assessed how different apportionment of restricted sleep (continuous vs. split sleep) influences neurobehavioral function and glucose levels. METHODS: Adolescents, aged 15-19 years, were evaluated in a dormitory setting using a parallel-group design. Following two baseline nights of 9-hour time-in-bed (TIB), participants underwent either 5 nights of continuous 6.5-h TIB (n = 29) or 5-hour nocturnal TIB with a 1.5-hour afternoon nap (n = 29). After two recovery nights of 9-hour TIB, participants were sleep restricted for another three nights. Sleep was assessed using polysomnography (PSG). Cognitive performance and mood were evaluated three times per day. Oral glucose tolerance tests (OGTT) were conducted on mornings after baseline sleep, recovery sleep, and the third day of each sleep restriction cycle. RESULTS: The split sleep group had fewer vigilance lapses, better working memory and executive function, faster processing speed, lower level of subjective sleepiness, and more positive mood, even though PSG-verified total sleep time was less than the continuous sleep group. However, vigilance in both sleep-restricted groups was inferior to adolescents in a prior sample given 9-hour nocturnal TIB. During both cycles of sleep restriction, blood glucose during the OGTT increased by a greater amount in the split sleep schedule compared with persons receiving 6.5-hour continuous sleep. CONCLUSIONS: In adolescents, modest multinight sleep restriction had divergent negative effects on cognitive performance and glucose levels depending on how the restricted sleep was apportioned. They are best advised to obtain the recommended amount of nocturnal sleep. TRIAL REGISTRATION: https://clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT03333512.


Subject(s)
Adolescent Behavior/physiology , Adolescent Behavior/psychology , Glucose/metabolism , Sleep Deprivation/blood , Sleep Deprivation/psychology , Sleep/physiology , Adolescent , Affect/physiology , Attention/physiology , Executive Function/physiology , Female , Humans , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Polysomnography/trends , Sleep Deprivation/physiopathology , Time Factors , Wakefulness/physiology , Young Adult
19.
J Sleep Res ; 28(5): e12824, 2019 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30724415

ABSTRACT

The electroencephalographic power spectra of non-rapid eye movement sleep in adults demonstrate trait-like consistency within participants across multiple nights, even when prior sleep deprivation is present. Here, we examined the extent to which this finding applies to adolescents who are habitually sleep restricted on school-days and sleep longer on weekends. We evaluated 78 adolescents across three sleep restriction groups who underwent different permutations of adequate sleep (9 hr time-in-bed), sleep restriction (5 hr time-in-bed), afternoon naps (1 hr afternoon) and recovery sleep (9 hr time-in-bed) that simulate behaviour on school-days and weekends. The control group comprised a further 22 adolescents who had 9 hr of sleep opportunity each night. Intra-class correlation coefficients showed moderate to almost perfect within-subject stability in electroencephalographic power spectra across multiple nights in both sleep restriction and control groups, even when changes to sleep macrostructure were observed. While nocturnal intra-class correlation metrics were lower in the low-frequency and spindle frequency bins in the sleep restriction compared with the control group, hierarchical clustering measures could still identify multi-night electroencephalographic spectra as originating from the same individual. The trait-like characteristics of electroencephalographic spectra from an adolescent remain identifiable despite the disruptive effects of multi-night sleep restriction to sleep architecture.


Subject(s)
Electroencephalography/methods , Polysomnography/methods , Sleep/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Young Adult
20.
Sleep ; 42(4)2019 04 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30649558

ABSTRACT

STUDY OBJECTIVES: Previous studies have shown that sleep benefits prospective memory by facilitating spontaneous retrieval processes. Here, we investigated the sleep features supporting such a benefit. METHODS: Forty-nine young adults (mean age ± SD: 22.06 ± 1.71 years; 18 males) encoded intentions comprising four related (phone-unplug earphones) and four unrelated (mirror-close the book) cue-action pairs. They were instructed to remember to perform these actions in response to cue words presented during a semantic categorization task 12 h later. The retention interval involved either a period of wakefulness (09:30-21:30; n = 24) or overnight sleep with polysomnographic monitoring (21:30-09:30; n = 25). RESULTS: We found a significant Group × Relatedness interaction for prospective memory accuracy (F = 8.35, p < 0.01). The sleep group successfully executed a significantly higher percentage of related intentions compared to the wake group (mean ± standard error of the mean (SEM): 94.00 ± 2.61% vs 66.67 ± 6.84%, p < 0.001). This benefit for related intentions was associated with longer post-learning slow wave sleep (r = 0.46, p < 0.05). In contrast, the percentage of unrelated intentions successfully executed did not differ between groups (82.00 ± 5.10% vs 72.92 ± 6.88%, p = 0.29). CONCLUSION: Slow wave sleep after memory encoding may strengthen the preexisting associations between semantically related cues and actions, thereby facilitating subsequent spontaneous retrieval processes.


Subject(s)
Memory Consolidation/physiology , Memory, Episodic , Mental Recall/physiology , Sleep, Slow-Wave/physiology , Adult , Cues , Female , Humans , Male , Semantics , Wakefulness/physiology , Young Adult
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...