Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 35
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev ; 163: 105742, 2024 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38830561

ABSTRACT

The causes of forgetting in working memory (WM) remain a source of debate in cognitive psychology, partly because it has always been challenging to probe the complex neural mechanisms that govern rapid cognitive processes in humans. In this review, we argue that neural, and more precisely animal models, provide valuable tools for exploring the precise mechanisms of WM forgetting. First, we discuss theoretical perspectives concerning WM forgetting in humans. Then, we present neuronal correlates of WM in animals, starting from the initial evidence of delay activity observed in the prefrontal cortex to the later synaptic theory of WM. In the third part, specific theories of WM are discussed, including the notion that silent versus non-silent activity is more consistent with the processes of refreshing and decay proposed in human cognitive models. The review concludes with an exploration of the relationship between long-term memory and WM, revealing connections between these two forms of memory through the long-term synaptic hypothesis, which suggests that long-term storage of interference can potentially disrupt WM.


Subject(s)
Memory, Short-Term , Humans , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Animals , Brain/physiology , Memory, Long-Term/physiology
2.
Neuroendocrinology ; : 1-16, 2024 Jun 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38852578

ABSTRACT

INTRODUCTION: Protein-enriched diets improve glycemic control in diabetes or emotional behavior in depressive patients. In mice, these benefits depend on intestinal gluconeogenesis activation by di-/tripeptides. Intestinal di-/tripeptides absorption is carried out by the peptide transporter 1, PEPT1. The lack of PEPT1 might thus alter glucose and emotional balance. METHODS: To determine the effects of PEPT1 deficiency under standard dietary conditions or during a dietary challenge known to promote both metabolic and cognitive dysfunction, insulin sensitivity, anxiety, and depressive-like traits, hippocampal serotonin (5-HT) and insulin signaling pathway were measured in wild-type (WT) and Pept1-/- mice fed either a chow or a high-fat high-sucrose (HF-HS) diet. RESULTS: Pept1-/- mice exhibited slight defects in insulin sensitivity and emotional behavior, which were aggravated by an HF-HS diet. Pept1-/- mice fed a chow diet had lower hippocampal 5-HT levels and exhibited cerebral insulin resistance under HF-HS diet. These defects were independent of intestinal gluconeogenesis but might be linked to increased plasma amino acids levels. CONCLUSION: Pept1-/- mice develop prediabetic and depressive-like traits and could thus be used to develop strategies to prevent or cure both diseases.

3.
J Neurosci ; 43(7): 1191-1210, 2023 02 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36631268

ABSTRACT

Synaptic changes play a major role in memory processes. Modulation of synaptic responses by brain states remains, however, poorly understood in hippocampal networks, even in basal conditions. We recorded evoked synaptic responses at five hippocampal pathways in freely moving male rats. We showed that, at the perforant path to dentate gyrus (PP-DG) synapse, responses increase during wakefulness compared with sleep. At the Schaffer collaterals to CA1 (SC-CA1) synapse, responses increase during non-REM sleep (NREM) compared with the other states. During REM sleep (REM), responses decreased at the PP-DG and SC-CA1 synapses compared with NREM, while they increased at the fornix to nucleus accumbens synapse (Fx-NAc) during REM compared with the other states. In contrast, responses at the fornix to medial PFC synapse (Fx-PFC) and at the fornix to amygdala synapse (Fx-Amy) were weakly modulated by vigilance states. Extended sleep periods led to synaptic changes at PP-DG and Fx-Amy synapses but not at the other synapses. Synaptic responses were also linked to local oscillations and were highly correlated between Fx-PFC and Fx-NAc but not between Fx-Amy and these synapses. These results reveal synapse-specific modulations that may contribute to memory consolidation during the sleep-wake cycle.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Surprisingly, the cortical network dynamics remains poorly known at the synaptic level. We tested the hypothesis that brain states would modulate synaptic changes in the same way at different cortical connections. To tackle this issue, we implemented an approach to explore the synaptic behavior of five connections upstream and downstream the rat hippocampus. Our study reveals that synaptic responses are modulated in a highly synapse-specific manner by wakefulness and sleep states as well as by local oscillations at these connections. Moreover, we found rapid synaptic changes during wake and sleep transitions as well as synaptic down and upregulations after extended periods of sleep. These synaptic changes are likely related to the mechanisms of sleep-dependent memory consolidation.


Subject(s)
Hippocampus , Synapses , Rats , Male , Animals , Hippocampus/physiology , Synapses/physiology , Sleep/physiology , Brain , Perforant Pathway/physiology
4.
Brain Commun ; 4(6): fcac307, 2022.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36751497

ABSTRACT

Neuromyelitis optica (NMO) is an autoimmune demyelinating disease of the central nervous system characterized by the presence of autoantibodies (called NMO-IgG) targeting aquaporin-4. Aquaporin-4 is expressed at the perivascular foot processes of astrocytes, in the glia limitans, but also at the ependyma. Most studies have focused on studying the pathogenicity of NMO-IgG on astrocytes, and NMO is now considered an astrocytopathy. However, periependymal lesions are observed in NMO suggesting that ependymal cells could also be targeted by NMO-IgG. Ependymal cells regulate CSF-parenchyma molecular exchanges and CSF flow, and are a niche for sub-ventricular neural stem cells. Our aim was to examine the effect of antibodies from NMO patients on ependymal cells. We exposed two models, i.e. primary cultures of rat ependymal cells and explant cultures of rat lateral ventricular wall whole mounts, to purified IgG of NMO patients (NMO-IgG) for 24 hours. We then evaluated the treatment effect using immunolabelling, functional assays, ependymal flow analysis and bulk RNA sequencing. For each experiment, the effects were compared with those of purified IgG from a healthy donors and non-treated cells. We found that: (i) NMO-IgG induced aquaporin-4 agglomeration at the surface of ependymal cells and induced cell enlargement in comparison to controls. In parallel, it induced an increase in gap junction connexin-43 plaque size; (ii) NMO-IgG altered the orientation of ciliary basal bodies and functionally impaired cilia motility; (iii) NMO-IgG activated the proliferation of sub-ventricular neural stem cells; (iv) treatment with NMO-IgG up-regulated the expression of pro-inflammatory cytokines and chemokines in the transcriptomic analysis. Our study showed that NMO-IgG can trigger an early and specific reactive phenotype in ependymal cells, with functional alterations of intercellular communication and cilia, activation of sub-ventricular stem cell proliferation and the secretion of pro-inflammatory cytokines. These findings suggest a key role for ependymal cells in the early phase of NMO lesion formation.

5.
Cereb Cortex ; 31(6): 2980-2992, 2021 05 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33506269

ABSTRACT

Long-term storage of information into memory is supposed to rely on long-term synaptic plasticity processes. The detection of such synaptic changes after training in long-term/reference memory (RM) tasks has yet been scarce, variable and only studied on a short time scale. Short-term or working memory (WM) is largely known to depend on persistent neuronal activity or short-term plasticity. However, processing information into WM could also involve long-term synaptic changes that could be responsible for the erasure/forgetting of items previously stored in WM and acting as proactive interference. In order to study long-term synaptic changes associated with RM or WM, we trained chronically implanted rats in 3 different radial maze tasks: a classical RM task and 2 WM tasks involving different levels of proactive interference. Synaptic responses in the dentate gyrus were recorded during 2 × 24 h in freely moving rats after training. We found that consolidation of long-term information leads first to a delayed synaptic potentiation, occurring 9 h after RM training that is replaced by a synaptic depression once the RM rule is fully acquired. In contrast, optimal information processing into WM triggers a synaptic depression immediately after training and lasting 3 h that could act as a mechanism for interference erasure/forgetting.


Subject(s)
Dentate Gyrus/physiology , Excitatory Postsynaptic Potentials/physiology , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Neuronal Plasticity/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Synapses/physiology , Animals , Electrodes, Implanted , Electroencephalography/methods , Electromyography/methods , Male , Maze Learning/physiology , Rats
6.
Neuroendocrinology ; 111(12): 1249-1265, 2021.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33429400

ABSTRACT

INTRODUCTION: Several studies have suggested that diet, especially the one enriched in microbiota-fermented fibers or fat, regulates behavior. The underlying mechanisms are currently unknown. We previously reported that certain macronutrients (fermentable fiber and protein) regulate energy homeostasis via the activation of intestinal gluconeogenesis (IGN), which generates a neural signal to the brain. We hypothesized that these nutriments might control behavior using the same gut-brain circuit. METHODS: Wild-type and IGN-deficient mice were fed chow or diets enriched in protein or fiber. Changes in their behavior were assessed using suited tests. Hippocampal neurogenesis, extracellular levels of serotonin, and protein expression levels were assessed by immunofluorescence, in vivo dialysis, and Western blotting, respectively. IGN was rescued by infusing glucose into the portal vein of IGN-deficient mice. RESULTS: We show here that both fiber- and protein-enriched diets exert beneficial actions on anxiety-like and depressive-like behaviors. These benefits do not occur in mice lacking IGN. Consistently, IGN-deficient mice display hallmarks of depressive-like disorders, including decreased hippocampal neurogenesis, basal hyperactivity, and deregulation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis, which are associated with increased expression of the precursor of corticotropin-releasing hormone in the hypothalamus and decreased expression of the glucocorticoid receptor in the hippocampus. These neurobiological alterations are corrected by portal glucose infusion mimicking IGN. CONCLUSION: IGN translates nutritional information, allowing the brain to finely coordinate energy metabolism and behavior.


Subject(s)
Anxiety/metabolism , Behavior, Animal/physiology , Depression/metabolism , Dietary Fiber/metabolism , Dietary Proteins/metabolism , Gluconeogenesis/physiology , Intestine, Small/metabolism , Animals , Disease Models, Animal , Mice
7.
Sleep ; 41(12)2018 12 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30285241

ABSTRACT

Study Objectives: Paradoxical sleep (PS) has been shown to play an important role in memory, in particular in emotional memory processes. However, the involvement of this particular sleep stage in the systemic consolidation of remote (30 days old) memory has never been tested. We examined whether post-learning PS could play a role in the consolidation of remote fearful memory and in the brain network reorganization that depends on it. Methods: Mice were PS-deprived during 6 hours after contextual fear conditioning using an automated method, and their memory was tested either 1 day or 30 days after learning. Brain activity during retrieval was assessed using the immediate early gene Egr1 (Zif 268) as a neuronal marker of activity. Results: We found that PS deprivation impaired the recall of remote (30 days)-but not recent (1 day)-memory. We also showed that the superficial layers of the anterior cingulate cortex were significantly less activated during the retrieval of remote memory after PS deprivation. In contrast, after such deprivation, retrieval of remote memory significantly activated several areas involved in emotional processing such as the CA1 area of the ventral hippocampus, the basolateral amygdala and the superficial layers of the ventral orbitofrontal cortex. By performing graph-theoretical analyses, our result also suggests that post-learning PS deprivation could impact the reorganization of the functional connections between limbic areas in order to reduce the level of global activity in this network. Conclusions: These findings suggest an important role for PS in the systemic consolidation of remote memory.


Subject(s)
Limbic System/physiology , Memory Consolidation/physiology , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Mental Recall/physiology , Sleep Deprivation/physiopathology , Sleep, REM/physiology , Animals , Basolateral Nuclear Complex/physiology , Early Growth Response Protein 1/genetics , Emotions , Fear/physiology , Gyrus Cinguli/physiology , Hippocampus/physiology , Learning/physiology , Male , Mice , Mice, Inbred C57BL , Prefrontal Cortex/physiology
8.
PLoS One ; 12(3): e0173834, 2017.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28288205

ABSTRACT

A distinction has always been made between long-term and short-term memory (also now called working memory, WM). The obvious difference between these two kinds of memory concerns the duration of information storage: information is supposedly transiently stored in WM while it is considered durably consolidated into long-term memory. It is well acknowledged that the content of WM is erased and reset after a short time, to prevent irrelevant information from proactively interfering with newly stored information. In the present study, we used typical WM radial maze tasks to question the brief lifespan of spatial WM content in rodents. Groups of rats were submitted to one of two different WM tasks in a radial maze: a WM task involving the repetitive presentation of a same pair of arms expected to induce a high level of proactive interference (PI) (HIWM task), or a task using a different pair in each trial expected to induce a low level of PI (LIWM task). Performance was effectively lower in the HIWM group than in LIWM in the final trial of each training session, indicative of a "within-session/short-term" PI effect. However, we also observed a different "between-session/long-term" PI effect between the two groups: while performance of LIWM trained rats remained stable over days, the performance of HIWM rats dropped after 10 days of training, and this impairment was visible from the very first trial of the day, hence not attributable to within-session PI. We also showed that a 24 hour-gap across training sessions known to allow consolidation processes to unfold, was a necessary and sufficient condition for the long-term PI effect to occur. These findings suggest that in the HIWM task, WM content was not entirely reset between training sessions and that, in specific conditions, WM content can outlast its purpose by being stored more permanently, generating a long-term deleterious effect of PI. The alternative explanation is that WM content could be transferred and stored more permanently in an intermediary form or memory between WM and long-term memory.


Subject(s)
Memory, Long-Term , Memory, Short-Term , Animals , Proactive Inhibition , Rats
9.
Learn Mem ; 24(2): 86-94, 2017 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28096498

ABSTRACT

Phosphorylation of CaMKII and AMPA receptor GluA1 subunit has been shown to play a major role in hippocampal-dependent long-term/reference memory (RM) and in the expression of long-term synaptic potentiation (LTP). In contrast, it has been proposed that dephosphorylation of these proteins could be involved in the opposite phenomenon of hippocampal long-term synaptic depression (LTD) and in adaptive forgetting. Adaptive forgetting allows interfering old memories to be forgotten to give new ones the opportunity to be stored in memory, and in particular in short-term/working memory (WM) that was shown to be very sensitive to proactive interference. To determine the role of CaMKII and GluA1 in adaptive forgetting, we adopted a comparative approach to assess the relative quantity and phosphorylation state of these proteins in the brain of rats trained in one of three radial maze paradigms: a RM task, a WM task involving a high level of adaptive forgetting, or a WM involving a low level of adaptive forgetting. Surprisingly, Western blot analyses revealed that training in a WM task involving a high level of adaptive forgetting specifically increased the expression of AMPA receptor GluA1 subunit and the activity of CaMKII in the dentate gyrus. These results highlight that WM with proactive interference involves mechanisms of synaptic plasticity selectively in the dentate gyrus.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Physiological/physiology , Calcium-Calmodulin-Dependent Protein Kinase Type 2/metabolism , Hippocampus/metabolism , Learning/physiology , Memory/physiology , Receptors, AMPA/metabolism , Animals , Food Deprivation , Male , Maze Learning/physiology , Phosphorylation , Rats , Serine/metabolism , Statistics, Nonparametric
10.
Sleep ; 39(12): 2173-2188, 2016 Dec 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27748246

ABSTRACT

STUDY OBJECTIVES: It is commonly accepted that sleep is beneficial to memory processes, but it is still unclear if this benefit originates from improved memory consolidation or enhanced information processing. It has thus been proposed that sleep may also promote forgetting of undesirable and non-essential memories, a process required for optimization of cognitive resources. We tested the hypothesis that non-rapid eye movement sleep (NREMS) promotes forgetting of irrelevant information, more specifically when processing information in working memory (WM), while REM sleep (REMS) facilitates the consolidation of important information. METHODS: We recorded sleep patterns of rats trained in a radial maze in three different tasks engaging either the long-term or short-term storage of information, as well as a gradual level of interference. RESULTS: We observed a transient increase in REMS amount on the day the animal learned the rule of a long-term/reference memory task (RM), and, in contrast, a positive correlation between the performance of rats trained in a WM task involving an important processing of interference and the amount of NREMS or slow wave activity. Various oscillatory events were also differentially modulated by the type of training involved. Notably, NREMS spindles and REMS rapid theta increase with RM training, while sharp-wave ripples increase with all types of training. CONCLUSIONS: These results suggest that REMS, but also rapid oscillations occurring during NREMS would be specifically implicated in the long-term memory in RM, whereas NREMS and slow oscillations could be involved in the forgetting of irrelevant information required for WM.


Subject(s)
Maze Learning/physiology , Memory, Long-Term/physiology , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Sleep Deprivation/physiopathology , Sleep, REM/physiology , Sleep/physiology , Animals , Attention/physiology , Electroencephalography , Rats , Sleep Deprivation/psychology
11.
Cereb Cortex ; 26(4): 1488-1500, 2016 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25585510

ABSTRACT

Prolonged rapid-eye-movement (REM) sleep deprivation has long been used to study the role of REM sleep in learning and memory processes. However, this method potentially induces stress and fatigue that may directly affect cognitive functions. Here, by using a short-term and nonstressful REM sleep deprivation (RSD) method we assessed in rats the bidirectional influence of reduced and increased REM sleep amount on hippocampal-dependent emotional memory and plasticity. Our results indicate that 4 h RSD impaired consolidation of contextual fear conditioning (CFC) and induction of long-term potentiation (LTP), while decreasing density of Egr1/Zif268-expressing neurons in the CA1 region of the dorsal hippocampus. LTP and Egr1 expression were not affected in ventral CA1. Conversely, an increase in REM sleep restores and further facilitates CFC consolidation and LTP induction, and also increases Egr1 expression in dorsal CA1. Moreover, CFC consolidation, Egr1 neuron density, and LTP amplitude in dorsal CA1 show a positive correlation with REM sleep amount. Altogether, these results indicate that mild changes in REM sleep amount bidirectionally affect memory and synaptic plasticity mechanisms occurring in the CA1 area of the dorsal hippocampus.


Subject(s)
Emotions/physiology , Hippocampus/physiopathology , Long-Term Potentiation , Memory Consolidation/physiology , Sleep Deprivation/physiopathology , Sleep, REM , Animals , Conditioning, Classical/physiology , Early Growth Response Protein 1/metabolism , Fear/physiology , Hippocampus/metabolism , Male , Rats , Rats, Sprague-Dawley
12.
Sci Adv ; 1(3): e1400177, 2015 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26601158

ABSTRACT

Evidence in humans suggests that limbic cortices are more active during rapid eye movement (REM or paradoxical) sleep than during waking, a phenomenon fitting with the presence of vivid dreaming during this state. In that context, it seemed essential to determine which populations of cortical neurons are activated during REM sleep. Our aim in the present study is to fill this gap by combining gene expression analysis, functional neuroanatomy, and neurochemical lesions in rats. We find in rats that, during REM sleep hypersomnia compared to control and REM sleep deprivation, the dentate gyrus, claustrum, cortical amygdaloid nucleus, and medial entorhinal and retrosplenial cortices are the only cortical structures containing neurons with an increased expression of Bdnf, FOS, and ARC, known markers of activation and/or synaptic plasticity. Further, the dentate gyrus is the only cortical structure containing more FOS-labeled neurons during REM sleep hypersomnia than during waking. Combining FOS staining, retrograde labeling, and neurochemical lesion, we then provide evidence that FOS overexpression occurring in the cortex during REM sleep hypersomnia is due to projections from the supramammillary nucleus and the claustrum. Our results strongly suggest that only a subset of cortical and hippocampal neurons are activated and display plasticity during REM sleep by means of ascending projections from the claustrum and the supramammillary nucleus. Our results pave the way for future studies to identify the function of REM sleep with regard to dreaming and emotional memory processing.

13.
PLoS One ; 10(11): e0142065, 2015.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26528714

ABSTRACT

How does the brain discriminate essential information aimed to be stored permanently from information required only temporarily, and that needs to be cleared away for not saturating our precious memory space? Reference Memory (RM) refers to the long-term storage of invariable information whereas Working Memory (WM) depends on the short-term storage of trial-unique information. Previous work has revealed that WM tasks are very sensitive to proactive interference. In order to prevent such interference, irrelevant old memories must be forgotten to give new ones the opportunity to be stabilized. However, unlike memory, physiological processes underlying this adaptive form of forgetting are still poorly understood. Here, we precisely ask what specific brain structure(s) could be responsible for such process to occur. To answer this question, we trained rats in a radial maze using three paradigms, a RM task and two WM tasks involving or not the processing of interference but strictly identical in terms of locomotion or motivation. We showed that an inhibition of the expression of Zif268 and c-Fos, two indirect markers of neuronal activity and synaptic plasticity, was observed in the dentate gyrus of the dorsal hippocampus when processing such interfering previously stored information. Conversely, we showed that inactivating the dentate gyrus impairs both RM and WM, but improves the processing of interference. Altogether, these results strongly suggest for the first time that the dentate gyrus could be a key structure involved in adaptive forgetting.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Psychological/physiology , Dentate Gyrus/physiology , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Neuronal Plasticity/physiology , Neurons/metabolism , Animals , Early Growth Response Protein 1/biosynthesis , Gene Expression Regulation/physiology , Proto-Oncogene Proteins c-fos/biosynthesis , Rats
14.
Hippocampus ; 25(11): 1361-73, 2015 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25808129

ABSTRACT

The cognitive role of melanin-concentrating hormone (MCH) neurons, a neuronal population located in the mammalian postero-lateral hypothalamus sending projections to all cortical areas, remains poorly understood. Mainly activated during paradoxical sleep (PS), MCH neurons have been implicated in sleep regulation. The genetic deletion of the only known MCH receptor in rodent leads to an impairment of hippocampal dependent forms of memory and to an alteration of hippocampal long-term synaptic plasticity. By using MCH/ataxin3 mice, a genetic model characterized by a selective deletion of MCH neurons in the adult, we investigated the role of MCH neurons in hippocampal synaptic plasticity and hippocampal-dependent forms of memory. MCH/ataxin3 mice exhibited a deficit in the early part of both long-term potentiation and depression in the CA1 area of the hippocampus. Post-tetanic potentiation (PTP) was diminished while synaptic depression induced by repetitive stimulation was enhanced suggesting an alteration of pre-synaptic forms of short-term plasticity in these mice. Behaviorally, MCH/ataxin3 mice spent more time and showed a higher level of hesitation as compared to their controls in performing a short-term memory T-maze task, displayed retardation in acquiring a reference memory task in a Morris water maze, and showed a habituation deficit in an open field task. Deletion of MCH neurons could thus alter spatial short-term memory by impairing short-term plasticity in the hippocampus. Altogether, these findings could provide a cellular mechanism by which PS may facilitate memory encoding. Via MCH neuron activation, PS could prepare the day's learning by increasing and modulating short-term synaptic plasticity in the hippocampus.


Subject(s)
Behavior, Animal/physiology , CA1 Region, Hippocampal/physiology , Hypothalamic Hormones/physiology , Hypothalamus/cytology , Melanins/physiology , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Neuronal Plasticity/physiology , Neurons/physiology , Pituitary Hormones/physiology , Sleep, REM/physiology , Animals , Ataxin-3/genetics , Hypothalamic Hormones/genetics , Hypothalamus/metabolism , Melanins/genetics , Mice , Mice, Inbred C57BL , Mice, Transgenic , Pituitary Hormones/genetics
15.
Neurobiol Learn Mem ; 122: 4-10, 2015 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25448317

ABSTRACT

Memory consolidation is the process for long-term storage of information and protection against interferences. It has been proposed that long-term potentiation (LTP), the long-lasting enhancement of synaptic transmission, is a cellular model for memory consolidation. Since consolidation of several forms of memory is facilitated by paradoxical sleep (PS) we ask whether PS modulates the cellular and molecular pathways underlying LTP. The long-lasting form of LTP (L-LTP) is dependent on the activation of transcription factors, enzymatic cascades and the secreted neurotrophin BDNF. By using PS deprivation, immunohistochemistry and quantitative real-time polymerase chain reaction (qPCR), we showed that an increase in PS amount (produced by rebound in PS deprived rats) is able to up-regulate the expression level of transcription factors Zif268 and c-Fos as well as Arc and BDNF in the CA1 and CA3 areas of the hippocampus. Several studies involved these factors in dendritic protein synthesis and in long-term structural changes of synapses underlying L-LTP. The present study together with the work of others (Ribeiro et al., 2002) suggest that by this mechanism, a post-learning increase in PS quantity (post-learning PS window) could convert a transient form of LTP to L-LTP.


Subject(s)
Arousal , Long-Term Potentiation , Memory Consolidation/physiology , Sleep, REM/physiology , Animals , Early Growth Response Protein 1/metabolism , Hippocampus/metabolism , Hippocampus/physiology , Male , Rats, Sprague-Dawley
16.
Front Pharmacol ; 6: 298, 2015.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26779016

ABSTRACT

Episodic memory, can be defined as the memory for unique events. The serotonergic system one of the main neuromodulatory systems in the brain appears to play a role in it. The serotonin 2a receptor (5-HT2aR) one of the principal post-synaptic receptors for 5-HT in the brain, is involved in neuropsychiatric and neurological disorders associated with memory deficits. Recognition memory can be defined as the ability to recognize if a particular event or item was previously encountered and is thus considered, under certain conditions, a form of episodic memory. As human data suggest that a constitutively decrease of 5-HT2A signaling might affect episodic memory performance we decided to compare the performance of mice with disrupted 5-HT2aR signaling (htr2a (-/-)) with wild type (htr2a (+/+)) littermates in different recognition memory and working memory tasks that differed in the level of proactive interference. We found that ablation of 5-HT2aR signaling throughout development produces a deficit in tasks that cannot be solved by single item strategy suggesting that 5-HT2aR signaling is involved in interference resolution. We also found that in the absence of 5-HT2aR signaling serotonin has a deleterious effect on recognition memory retrieval through the activation of 5-HT1aR in the medial prefrontal cortex.

17.
Learn Mem ; 21(4): 205-14, 2014 Mar 17.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24639487

ABSTRACT

A common genetic polymorphism that results in increased activity of the dopamine regulating enzyme COMT (the COMT Val(158) allele) has been found to associate with poorer cognitive performance and increased susceptibility to develop psychiatric disorders. It is generally assumed that this increase in COMT activity influences cognitive function and psychiatric disease risk by increasing dopamine turnover in cortical synapses, though this cannot be directly measured in humans. Here we explore a novel transgenic mouse model of increased COMT activity, equivalent to the relative increase in activity observed with the human COMT Val(158) allele. By performing an extensive battery of behavioral tests, we found that COMT overexpressing mice (COMT-OE mice) exhibit cognitive deficits selectively in the domains that are affected by the COMT Val(158) allele, stimulus-response learning and working memory, functionally validating our model of increased COMT activity. Although we detected no changes in the level of markers for dopamine synthesis and dopamine transport, we found that COMT-OE mice display an increase in dopamine release capacity in the striatum. This result suggests that increased COMT activity may not only affect dopamine signaling by enhancing synaptic clearance in the cortex, but may also cause changes in presynaptic dopamine function in the striatum. These changes may underlie the behavioral deficits observed in the mice and might also play a role in the cognitive deficits and increased psychiatric disease risk associated with genetic variation in COMT activity in humans.


Subject(s)
Catechol O-Methyltransferase/metabolism , Corpus Striatum/metabolism , Dopamine/metabolism , Learning Disabilities/metabolism , Learning/physiology , Animals , Catechol O-Methyltransferase/genetics , Cognition/physiology , Compulsive Behavior/genetics , Compulsive Behavior/metabolism , Dopamine Plasma Membrane Transport Proteins/metabolism , Impulsive Behavior , Learning Disabilities/genetics , Male , Memory Disorders/genetics , Memory Disorders/metabolism , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Mice , Mice, Inbred C57BL , Mice, Transgenic , Models, Animal , Motor Activity/genetics , Motor Activity/physiology , Neuropsychological Tests , Polymorphism, Genetic , Prosencephalon/metabolism , Tyrosine 3-Monooxygenase/metabolism
18.
Behav Brain Res ; 256: 43-50, 2013 Nov 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23933146

ABSTRACT

Cardinal symptoms of depression include helplessness and anhedonia. In addition, depression and anxiety are often comorbid disorders. H/Rouen mice, a genetic mouse model of depression, display helpless behavior in the tail suspension test, whereas non-helpless NH/Rouen mice show the opposite behavior. It is unknown whether H/Rouen mice display an anxious behavior as compared to NH/Rouen mice, and is unclear whether they display anhedonia. Time spent in the periphery of an open-field, an index of anxiety, was found to be higher in male and female H/Rouen mice as compared to NH/Rouen mice. In the elevated plus-maze, a decrease in the number of entries and in the time spent in the open arms was observed in both male and female H/Rouen. In the light/dark box, the number of entries and the time spent in the anxiogenic bright compartment was significantly reduced in male and female H/Rouen mice. In addition, the preference of consumption of a 2% sucrose solution was significantly reduced in male and female H/Rouen mice as compared to NH/Rouen and I/Rouen mice in a two-bottle choice paradigm but was restored by a chronic (3 weeks) fluoxetine treatment. H/Rouen mice thus display both anxiety and anhedonia making them a potent animal model in the treatment of forms depression comorbidly expressed with anxiety.


Subject(s)
Anxiety/complications , Depressive Disorder/complications , Disease Models, Animal , Anhedonia/drug effects , Anhedonia/physiology , Animals , Antidepressive Agents, Second-Generation/therapeutic use , Anxiety/drug therapy , Anxiety/epidemiology , Anxiety/physiopathology , Comorbidity , Depressive Disorder/drug therapy , Depressive Disorder/epidemiology , Depressive Disorder/physiopathology , Drinking Behavior/drug effects , Drinking Behavior/physiology , Exploratory Behavior/drug effects , Exploratory Behavior/physiology , Female , Fluoxetine/therapeutic use , Food Preferences/drug effects , Food Preferences/physiology , Male , Mice, Inbred Strains , Motor Activity/drug effects , Motor Activity/physiology , Neuropsychological Tests , Species Specificity , Sucrose/administration & dosage
19.
J Neurosci ; 33(34): 13583-99, 2013 Aug 21.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23966682

ABSTRACT

In many cortical neurons, HCN1 channels are the major contributors to Ih, the hyperpolarization-activated current, which regulates the intrinsic properties of neurons and shapes their integration of synaptic inputs, paces rhythmic activity, and regulates synaptic plasticity. Here, we examine the physiological role of Ih in deep layer pyramidal neurons in mouse prefrontal cortex (PFC), focusing on persistent activity, a form of sustained firing thought to be important for the behavioral function of the PFC during working memory tasks. We find that HCN1 contributes to the intrinsic persistent firing that is induced by a brief depolarizing current stimulus in the presence of muscarinic agonists. Deletion of HCN1 or acute pharmacological blockade of Ih decreases the fraction of neurons capable of generating persistent firing. The reduction in persistent firing is caused by the membrane hyperpolarization that results from the deletion of HCN1 or Ih blockade, rather than a specific role of the hyperpolarization-activated current in generating persistent activity. In vivo recordings show that deletion of HCN1 has no effect on up states, periods of enhanced synaptic network activity. Parallel behavioral studies demonstrate that HCN1 contributes to the PFC-dependent resolution of proactive interference during working memory. These results thus provide genetic evidence demonstrating the importance of HCN1 to intrinsic persistent firing and the behavioral output of the PFC. The causal role of intrinsic persistent firing in PFC-mediated behavior remains an open question.


Subject(s)
Action Potentials/genetics , Cyclic Nucleotide-Gated Cation Channels/metabolism , Executive Function/physiology , Memory/physiology , Neurons/physiology , Potassium Channels/metabolism , Prefrontal Cortex/cytology , Action Potentials/drug effects , Animals , Choice Behavior/drug effects , Cyclic Nucleotide-Gated Cation Channels/genetics , Gene Expression Regulation/drug effects , Gene Expression Regulation/physiology , Green Fluorescent Proteins/genetics , Hyperpolarization-Activated Cyclic Nucleotide-Gated Channels , In Vitro Techniques , Maze Learning/drug effects , Memory/drug effects , Mice , Mice, Inbred C57BL , Mice, Transgenic , Neurons/drug effects , Neurotransmitter Agents/pharmacology , Patch-Clamp Techniques , Potassium Channels/genetics , Serial Learning/drug effects , Serial Learning/physiology , Synaptic Potentials/drug effects , Synaptic Potentials/genetics
20.
Cell ; 147(6): 1369-83, 2011 Dec 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22153079

ABSTRACT

The cytoplasmic polyadenylation element-binding protein 3 (CPEB3), a regulator of local protein synthesis, is the mouse homolog of ApCPEB, a functional prion protein in Aplysia. Here, we provide evidence that CPEB3 is activated by Neuralized1, an E3 ubiquitin ligase. In hippocampal cultures, CPEB3 activated by Neuralized1-mediated ubiquitination leads both to the growth of new dendritic spines and to an increase of the GluA1 and GluA2 subunits of AMPA receptors, two CPEB3 targets essential for synaptic plasticity. Conditional overexpression of Neuralized1 similarly increases GluA1 and GluA2 and the number of spines and functional synapses in the hippocampus and is reflected in enhanced hippocampal-dependent memory and synaptic plasticity. By contrast, inhibition of Neuralized1 reduces GluA1 and GluA2 levels and impairs hippocampal-dependent memory and synaptic plasticity. These results suggest a model whereby Neuralized1-dependent ubiquitination facilitates hippocampal plasticity and hippocampal-dependent memory storage by modulating the activity of CPEB3 and CPEB3-dependent protein synthesis and synapse formation.


Subject(s)
Memory , Nerve Tissue Proteins/metabolism , Neuronal Plasticity , RNA-Binding Proteins/metabolism , Synapses , 3' Untranslated Regions , Animals , Base Sequence , Hippocampus/metabolism , Mice , Molecular Sequence Data , Poly A/metabolism , Receptors, AMPA/genetics , Receptors, AMPA/metabolism
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...