Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 9 de 9
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
Clin Implant Dent Relat Res ; 26(2): 309-316, 2024 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37728030

ABSTRACT

AIMS: To compare the accuracy of the Yakebot dental implant robotic system with that of fully guided static computer-assisted implant surgery (CAIS) template in edentulous implantation. MATERIALS AND METHODS: Thirteen patients with edentulous were recruited and divided into two groups: the Yake robotic system group (experimental) (n = 5) and the CAIS group (control) (n = 8). Postoperative cone-beam computed tomography (CBCT) was performed immediately, and the 3-dimensional positions of implants were obtained and compared with that in the preoperative design. The comparison showed platform, apical, depth, and angular deviations. A value of p < 0.05 was considered statistically significant. RESULTS: A total of 84 implants (36 in the robotic group and 48 in the CAIS group) were placed. The mean deviation at the implant platform, apex, depth, and angle in the CAIS group was 1.37 ± 0.72 mm, 1.28 ± 0.68 mm, 0.88 ± 0.47 mm, and 3.47 ± 2.02°, respectively. However, the mean deviation at the implant platform, apex, depth, and angle in the robotic group was 0.65 ± 0.25 mm, 0.65 ± 0.22 mm, 0.49 ± 0.24 mm, and 1.43 ± 1.18°, respectively. Significant differences in the four types of deviation (p < 0.05) between the two groups were observed. CONCLUSION: The accuracy of robotic system in edentulous implant placement was superior to that of the CAIS template, suggesting that robotic system is more accurate, safe, and flexible, can be considered a promising treatment in clinical practice.


Subject(s)
Dental Implants , Jaw, Edentulous , Mouth, Edentulous , Robotic Surgical Procedures , Surgery, Computer-Assisted , Humans , Dental Implantation, Endosseous/methods , Jaw, Edentulous/surgery , Surgery, Computer-Assisted/methods , Mouth, Edentulous/diagnostic imaging , Mouth, Edentulous/surgery , Cone-Beam Computed Tomography , Computer-Aided Design , Computers , Imaging, Three-Dimensional
2.
J Prosthet Dent ; 2023 Jul 11.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37442750

ABSTRACT

Autogenous dental root placement is an available approach for horizontal alveolar ridge augmentation in patients with severe bony defects. However, in previous reports, bone augmentation has been done before the implant placement. This clinical report describes the use of dentin grafting for alveolar ridge augmentation and simultaneous implant placement in the maxillary left central incisor region with a severe horizontal bone defect. Under strict adherence to the recommended indications, dentin grafting and simultaneous implantation could be clinically feasible protocols.

3.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 44(3): 948-969, 2023 02 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36308407

ABSTRACT

As one of the commonly used folk psychological concepts, self-deception has been intensively discussed yet is short of solid ground from cognitive neuroscience. Self-deception is a biased cognitive process of information to obtain or maintain a false belief that could be both self-enhancing or self-diminishing. Study 1 (N = 152) captured self-deception by adopting a modified numerical discrimination task that provided cheating opportunities, quantifying errors in predicting future performance (via item-response theory model), and measuring the belief of how good they are at solving the task (i.e., self-efficacy belief). By examining whether self-efficacy belief is based upon actual ability (true belief) or prediction errors (false belief), Study 1 showed that self-deception occurred in the effortless (easier access to answer cues) rather than effortful (harder access to answer cues) cheating opportunity conditions, suggesting high ambiguity in attributions facilitates self-deception. Studies 2 and 3 probed the neural source of self-deception, linking self-deception with the metacognitive process. Both studies replicated behavioral results from Study 1. Study 2 (ERP study; N = 55) found that the amplitude of frontal slow wave significantly differed between participants with positive/self-enhancing and negative/self-diminishing self-deceiving tendencies in incorrect predictions while remaining similar in correct predictions. Study 3 (functional magnetic resonance imaging study; N = 33) identified self-deceiving associated activity in the anterior medial prefrontal cortex and showed that effortless cheating context increased cheating behaviors that further facilitated self-deception. Our findings suggest self-deception is a false belief associated with a distorted metacognitive mental process that requires ambiguity in attributions of behaviors.


Subject(s)
Metacognition , Humans , Deception , Prefrontal Cortex/diagnostic imaging , Prefrontal Cortex/physiology , Cues
4.
Front Psychol ; 12: 736353, 2021.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34721206

ABSTRACT

The current research examined the influence of subjective loss on financial risk-taking tendency and negative emotional experience through inducing the experience of subjective loss in auction scenarios. In Study 1, we found that the subjective loss experience (compared to no-loss experience) in an auction scenario induced greater financial risk propensity, especially in gambling, greater negative emotion, and greater decision regret. In addition, we found that the subjective loss experience induced stronger negative emotion but less risk propensity in investment than the actual loss experience did, but these two types of loss did not yield a difference in risk propensity in gambling in Study 2. These results implicate that subjective loss is a distinct experience from no-loss and actual loss experiences, which is reflected by the degree of associated emotional experience and subsequent risk-taking propensity. The current research highlights the complex psychological processes of the experience of loss in decision-making contexts.

5.
J Biomed Nanotechnol ; 17(3): 456-465, 2021 Mar 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33875080

ABSTRACT

Guided bone regeneration (GBR) technique is most commonly used to treat alveolar bone defect. Polylactic acid (PLA) attracts much attention to utilize as a GBR membrane because it has relatively high mechanical strength and biodegradability. However, randomized controlled trials of PLA as a GBR membrane in animals were rare. The aim of this work is to observe the efficacy of polylactic acid membrane in guiding bone regeneration in Beagle canine alveolar bone defect restoration and to compare efficacy with the collagen membrane, providing an experimental basis for further clinical use of the polylactic acid membrane. The tests of physical and chemical properties showed that the PLA membrane has well mechanical strength to maintenance the space for the new bone, and has proper aperture for the attachment of osteoblasts. Through X-ray and histopathological examination of the different time points, the bone grafting material covered with PLA membrane can form similar mature bone compared to collagen membrane ones. Meanwhile, biodegradable speed of the PLA membrane was slower. Thus, this study showed that polylactic acid membrane as synthetic biodegradable polymer was reliably effective in guiding bone regeneration of alveolar bone defects, showed the favorable osteogenic capability and forecasts well applications in bone augmentation.


Subject(s)
Bone Regeneration , Membranes, Artificial , Animals , Dogs , Guided Tissue Regeneration, Periodontal , Osteoblasts , Osteogenesis , Polymers
6.
Neuropsychologia ; 139: 107375, 2020 03 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32027920

ABSTRACT

People hate being deceived. However, what would it be if lies come from in-group members compared with that from out-group members? In the current Electroencephalography (EEG) study, we recruited thirty-six participants to play a modified estimator and advisor game to investigate the mental and neural processes to lies and truth conveyed by in-group and out-group members. At the behavioral level, lies are less morally acceptable, arose less positive emotion, and made participants distribute less money to the advisor in a dictator game. Meanwhile, participants liked the in-group university more than the out-group university and they thought they were more similar to in-group members than to out-group members. However, there were no significant interactions of group type (i.e., in-group and out-group) and message type (i.e., lies and truth) in the aforementioned behavioral assessments. At the neural level, significant interaction effects were found in the parietal N1 and P3 amplitude. More importantly, no significant N1 and P3 amplitude differences between in-group lies and truth were found, while outgroup lies elicited larger P3 amplitude than outgroup truth and out-group truth elicited larger N1 amplitude than outgroup lies. What's more, P3 amplitude differences between lies vs. truth positively correlated with fairness scores only in the in-group condition but not in the out-group condition. Current study showed that the P3 component was sensitive in capturing subtle differences when participants were processing different types of lies and truth that could not be captured by behavioral measurements. Besides, the fairness trait modulated the in-group bias related P3 patterns. The current study provides insight into the neurobiological mechanism underlying the mental process of in-group and out-group lies and truth, and suggests individuals' tendency of general in-group favoritism and out-group discrimination toward moral behaviors.


Subject(s)
Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Deception , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Group Processes , Morals , Social Behavior , Social Perception , Adolescent , Adult , Electroencephalography , Female , Humans , Male , Young Adult
7.
Mol Med Rep ; 21(1): 338-346, 2020 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31939628

ABSTRACT

Several factors trigger apoptosis in cochlear hair cells. Previous studies have shown that mitochondria play key roles in apoptosis, but the role of mitochondrial deoxyribonucleic acid (mtDNA) copy number in the pathogenesis of hair cell apoptosis remains largely unknown. We used mouse cochlear hair cells and House Ear Institute­Organ of Corti 1 (HEI­OC1) cells to explore the relationship between mtDNA copy number and cell apoptosis. We found that the mtDNA copy number of hair cells was reduced relative to mitochondrial mass and hypothesized that increasing it might have a protective effect. We then increased the mtDNA copy number of the hair and HEI­OC1 cells by transfecting them with an adeno­associated virus (AAV) vector containing mitochondrial transcription factor A (TFAM). We found that the apoptosis rates decreased upon inducing apoptosis with neomycin or cisplatin (DDP). To elucidate the mechanisms, we analyzed the mitochondrial­membrane permeability and mitochondrial function of HEI­OC1 cells. Our results suggested that the increase in mtDNA copy number could protect hair cells and HEI­OC1 cells against drug­induced apoptosis by stabilizing the permeability of the mitochondrial membrane and mitochondrial function.


Subject(s)
Apoptosis/genetics , DNA, Mitochondrial/genetics , Hair Cells, Auditory/metabolism , Mitochondria/metabolism , Animals , Apoptosis/drug effects , Cells, Cultured , Cisplatin/pharmacology , DNA Copy Number Variations , DNA, Mitochondrial/metabolism , DNA-Binding Proteins/genetics , DNA-Binding Proteins/metabolism , Dependovirus , Female , Genetic Vectors , Hair Cells, Auditory/pathology , In Situ Hybridization, Fluorescence , Male , Mice , Mice, Inbred C57BL , Mitochondria/genetics , Mitochondrial Membranes/metabolism , Mitochondrial Proteins/genetics , Mitochondrial Proteins/metabolism , Neomycin/pharmacology , Organ of Corti/cytology , Organ of Corti/metabolism , Reactive Oxygen Species/metabolism , Transcription Factors/genetics , Transcription Factors/metabolism
8.
Front Psychol ; 11: 623591, 2020.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33584457

ABSTRACT

Dialectical thinking refers to a constellation of beliefs that consist of expectation of change, tolerance of contradiction, and holism. The current research explored whether dialectical thinking would affect people's anticipation of climate change, which has been propagated globally. Study 1 compared the responses between Chinese participants, representing people from cultures that promote dialectical thinking, and North American participants, representing people from cultures that promote linear thinking. The results showed that Chinese participants demonstrated a stronger non-linear pattern regarding the anticipation of climate change as compared with American participants, in which Chinese participants were more likely to anticipate a stable trend but less likely to anticipate an increasing trend for global warming. Study 2 with a manipulation of dialectical and linear thinking was conducted and provided some generally supportive evidence for the causal relation between dialectical beliefs and the anticipation of climate change. Implications for cross-cultural environmental research and international climate change education programs were discussed.

9.
Appetite ; 140: 151-158, 2019 09 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31077773

ABSTRACT

We proposed that nostalgic labels strengthen the appeal of food items when the items are intrinsically nostalgic (e.g., related to one's childhood). Nostalgic labels do so by fostering a sense of food comfort (i.e., safety and security) among potential consumers. Experiment 1, testing a Chinese sample, confirmed that nostalgic (vs. descriptive) labels strengthen purchase intentions of food items when such items are intrinsically nostalgic. Experiment 2 showed that nostalgic (vs. descriptive) food labels strengthen purchase intentions of intrinsically nostalgic items, and do so by virtue of their capacity to elevate food comfort. This experiment tested a U.S. sample, thus broadening the cross-cultural generalizability of the findings. Experiment 3, testing a Chinese sample, replicated the Experiment 2 findings using nostalgic versus descriptive labels of a food item, and ruled out the influence of an alternative mediator, perceived food healthiness. Finally, Experiment 4, testing also a Chinese sample, revealed that restaurant dishes are more likely to be consumed when advertised with a nostalgic (than descriptive) label. The current work makes theoretical contributions to literature on food consumption and emotions, and has practical implications for harnessing nostalgia for dietary purposes.


Subject(s)
Consumer Behavior , Eating/psychology , Food Labeling/methods , Food Preferences/psychology , Memory, Episodic , Adolescent , Asian People/psychology , China , Emotions , Female , Humans , Hunger , Intention , Male , Students/psychology , Young Adult
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...