Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 43
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis ; 35(4): B11-B18, 2018 Apr 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29603933

ABSTRACT

We used triple silent substitution stimuli to characterize human S-cone electroretinograms (ERGs) in normal trichromats. Short-wavelength-cone (S-cone) ERGs were found to have different morphological features and temporal frequency response characteristics compared to ERGs derived from L-cones, M-cones, and rod photoreceptors in normal participants. Furthermore, in two cases of retinal pathology, blue cone monochromatism (BCM) and enhanced S-cone syndrome (ESCS), S-cone ERGs elicited by our stimuli were preserved and enhanced, respectively. The results from both normal and pathological retinae demonstrate that triple silent substitution stimuli can be used to generate ERGs that provide an assay of human S-cone function.


Subject(s)
Color Vision Defects/physiopathology , Cone Opsins/physiology , Electroretinography , Eye Diseases, Hereditary/physiopathology , Retinal Cone Photoreceptor Cells/physiology , Retinal Degeneration/physiopathology , Vision Disorders/physiopathology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Photic Stimulation , Young Adult
2.
Doc Ophthalmol ; 136(1): 27-43, 2018 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29134295

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: To study how rod- and cone-driven responses depend on stimulus size in normal subjects and patients with retinitis pigmentosa (RP), and to show that comparisons between responses to full-field (FF) and smaller stimuli can be useful in diagnosing and monitoring disorders of the peripheral retina without the need for lengthy dark adaptation periods. METHOD: The triple silent substitution technique was used to isolate L-cone-, M-cone- and rod-driven ERGs with 19, 18 and 33% photoreceptor contrasts, respectively, under identical mean luminance conditions. Experiments were conducted on five normal subjects and three RP patients. ERGs on control subjects were recorded at nine different temporal frequencies (between 2 and 60 Hz) for five different stimulus sizes: FF, 70°, 60°, 50° and 40° diameter circular stimuli. Experiments on RP patients involved rod- and L-cone-driven ERG measurements with FF and 40° stimuli at 8 and 48 Hz. Response amplitudes were defined as those of the first harmonic component after Fourier analysis. RESULTS: In normal subjects, rod-driven responses displayed a fundamentally different behavior than cone-driven responses, particularly at low temporal frequencies. At low and intermediate temporal frequencies (≤ 12 Hz), rod-driven signals increased by a factor of about four when measured with smaller stimuli. In contrast, L- and M-cone-driven responses in this frequency region did not change substantially with stimulus size. At high temporal frequencies (≥ 24 Hz), both rod- and cone-driven response amplitudes decreased with decreasing stimulus size. Signals obtained from rod-isolating stimuli under these conditions are likely artefactual. Interestingly, in RP patients, both rod-driven and L-cone-driven ERGs were similar using 40° and FF stimuli. CONCLUSION: The increased responses with smaller stimuli in normal subjects to rod-isolating stimuli indicate that a fundamentally different mechanism drives the ERGs in comparison with the cone-driven responses. We propose that the increased responses are caused by stray light stimulating the peripheral retina, thereby allowing peripheral rod-driven function to be studied using the triple silent substitution technique at photopic luminances. The method is effective in studying impaired peripheral rod- and cone- function in RP patients.


Subject(s)
Electroretinography , Retinal Cone Photoreceptor Cells/physiology , Retinal Rod Photoreceptor Cells/physiology , Retinitis Pigmentosa/physiopathology , Adult , Dark Adaptation , Electroretinography/methods , Female , Fourier Analysis , Healthy Volunteers , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Photic Stimulation/methods
3.
Doc Ophthalmol ; 134(1): 11-24, 2017 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28091887

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: To record transient ERGs from the light-adapted human retina using silent substitution stimuli which selectively reflect the activity of rod photoreceptors. We aim to describe the morphology of these waveforms and examine how they are affected by the use of less selective stimuli and by retinal pathology. METHODS: Rod-isolating stimuli with square-wave temporal profiles (250/250 ms onset/offset) were presented using a 4 primary LED ganzfeld stimulator. Experiment 1: ERGs were recorded using a rod-isolating stimulus (63 ph Td, rod contrast, C rod = 0.25) from a group (n = 20) of normal trichromatic observers. Experiment 2: Rod ERGs were recorded from a group (n = 5) using a rod-isolating stimulus (C rod = 0.25) which varied in retinal illuminance from 40 to 10,000 ph Td. Experiment 3: ERGs were elicited using 2 kinds of non-isolating stimuli; (1) broadband and (2) rod-isolating stimuli which contained varying degrees of L- and M-cone excitation. Experiment 4: Rod ERGs were recorded from two patient groups with rod monochromacy (n = 3) and CSNB (type 1; n = 2). RESULTS: The rod-isolated ERGs elicited from normal subjects had a waveform with a positive onset component followed by a negative offset. Response amplitude was maximal at retinal illuminances <100 ph Td and was virtually abolished at 400 ph Td. The use of non-selective stimuli altered the ERG waveform eliciting more photopic-like ERG responses. Rod ERGs recorded from rod monochromats had similar features to those recorded from normal trichromats, in contrast to those recorded from participants with CSNB which had an electronegative appearance. CONCLUSIONS: Our results demonstrate that ERGs elicited by silent substitution stimuli can selectively reflect the operation of rod photoreceptors in the normal, light-adapted human retina.


Subject(s)
Color Perception/physiology , Electroretinography/methods , Retina/physiology , Retinal Rod Photoreceptor Cells/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Photic Stimulation/methods , Young Adult
4.
J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis ; 33(3): A77-84, 2016 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26974944

ABSTRACT

The asymmetric sequential color-matching technique was used to determine the shifts in chromaticity of real Munsell chips induced by four test illuminants. The reference illuminant was C. Illuminants green (G) and purple (P) induced shifts orthogonal to the Planckian locus, while illuminants S and A induced shifts along the Planckian locus. Vectors describing the shifts induced by A and S were quantitatively and qualitatively different from those due to G and P. The data suggest that physiological factors, influenced by the proximity of the Planckian locus, affect chromatic constancy under nonsimulated viewing conditions.


Subject(s)
Color Perception Tests/methods , Lighting/methods , Adult , Color , Color Perception Tests/instrumentation , Female , Humans , Lighting/instrumentation , Male , Middle Aged , Young Adult
5.
J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis ; 31(4): A350-6, 2014 Apr 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24695193

ABSTRACT

The link between chromatic constancy (compensation for hue and saturation shifts) and lightness constancy (compensation for a change in surface reflectance) was tested theoretically by computing cone contrasts and by asymmetric matching experiments. The effect of a thin achromatic line (a frame) around the test sample was tested empirically. When the samples were outlined by the frame, lightness constancy was increased and chromatic constancy reduced (p<0.001). Changes in luminance are more likely to be compensated when the luminance contrast edge around the test stimulus is disturbed as with the addition of an achromatic frame.


Subject(s)
Contrast Sensitivity , Light , Retinal Cone Photoreceptor Cells/cytology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged
7.
Ophthalmic Physiol Opt ; 30(5): 503-10, 2010 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20883333

ABSTRACT

We recorded L- and M-cone isolating ERGs from human subjects using a silent substitution technique at temporal rates of 12 and 30 Hz. These frequencies isolate the activity of cone-opponent and non-opponent post-receptoral mechanisms, respectively. ERGs were obtained using a sequence of stimuli with different spatial configurations comprising; (1) circular stimuli of different sizes which increased in 10° steps up to 70°diameter, or (2) annular stimuli with a 70° outer diameter but with different sized central ablations from 10° up to 60°. L- and M-cone isolating ERGs were obtained from five colour normal subjects using a DTL fibre electrode. Fourier analysis of the ERGs was performed and we measured the amplitude of the first harmonic of the response. For 12 Hz ERGs the L:M cone response amplitude ratio (L:M(ERG)) was close to unity and remained stable irrespective of the spatial configuration of the stimulus. The maintenance of this balanced ratio points to the existence of cone selective input across the human retina for the L-M cone opponent mechanism. For 30 Hz the L:M(ERG) ratio was greater than unity but varied depending upon which region of the retina was being stimulated. This variation we consider to be a consequence of the global response properties of M-cone ERGs rather than representing a real variation in L:M cone ratios across the retina.


Subject(s)
Color Vision/physiology , Retinal Cone Photoreceptor Cells/physiology , Electroretinography/methods , Flicker Fusion , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation/methods , Retina/physiology
8.
Ophthalmic Physiol Opt ; 30(5): 553-9, 2010 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20883339

ABSTRACT

It is known that there is a distortion of hue and saturation in the peripheral visual field. In a previous study, when an asymmetric matching paradigm was used, four hues in the blue, red, yellow and green regions of colour space were unchanged and these were referred to as peripherally invariant (Parry et al., J Opt Soc Am A, 23, 2006, 1586). Three of these invariant hues were similar to unique blue, red and yellow. However, for most observers there was a marked difference between unique and invariant green. To investigate this apparent paradox, we have measured unique hues using a range of eccentricities and colourimetric purities. An asymmetric matching and a 4-AFC paradigm were used to establish peripherally invariant and unique hues, respectively. In the asymmetric matching task the observer matched a peripheral spot with a para-foveal spot, for 24 different hues at 18° eccentricity. In the 4-AFC paradigm, 41 hues were presented 20 times at three purities (0.5, 0.75 and 1.0) and three eccentricities (18°, 10° and 1°). The observer had to name the hues as red, blue, green or yellow. Unique hues were found to be constant with eccentricity and purity. The unique green, established with 4-AFC, was found to differ from the invariant green, determined using the matching task. However, red, blue and yellow invariant hues correspond well with unique hues. The data suggest that different mechanisms mediate the matching of green compared with the identification of unique hues. This is similar to the difference between detection and discrimination of spectral stimuli: the detection process is dominated by the cone opponent mechanisms and is most sensitive, whereas more central processes, serving unique hues, influence discrimination.


Subject(s)
Color Perception/physiology , Adult , Confounding Factors, Epidemiologic , Fovea Centralis/physiology , Humans , Photic Stimulation/methods , Psychophysics , Visual Fields/physiology
9.
Ophthalmic Physiol Opt ; 29(3): 338-44, 2009 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19422566

ABSTRACT

We report a series of experiments designed to ensure that Macular Pigment Optical Density (MPOD) measurements obtained with a clinical instrument are not influenced by lens yellowing and ocular media optical density. These effects were determined in six subjects using seven Lee Colour Temperature Correcting filters to simulate changes in the transmittance of the ocular media with age. Calculated simulated age matched the data linking age and optical density reported in the literature, and the MPOD was independent of simulated age. The instrument allows an estimation of MPOD to be made which is based only on a foveal (centre-only) measurement rather than, as is conventional, making a comparison between foveal and peripheral measurements. We assessed the performance of this facility by comparing the centre-only estimate of MPOD with that obtained from both central and peripheral measurements in 5616 eyes. The 95% limits of agreement for the two estimates was 0.13 OD units.


Subject(s)
Aging/physiology , Macula Lutea/metabolism , Macular Degeneration/metabolism , Photic Stimulation/methods , Retinal Pigments/analysis , Adolescent , Adult , Age Factors , Aged , Child , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Photometry/methods , Retinal Pigments/metabolism , Statistics as Topic , Time Factors , Young Adult
10.
Ophthalmic Physiol Opt ; 29(3): 375-81, 2009 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19422572

ABSTRACT

The purpose of this study is to establish whether nasal-temporal differences in cone photoreceptor distributions are linked to differences in colour matching performance in the two hemi-fields. Perceived shifts in chromaticity were measured using an asymmetric matching paradigm. They were expressed in terms of hue rotations and relative saturation changes and also in terms of activation levels of L-M or S-(L+M) cone-opponent channels. Up to 19 degrees eccentricity there was little difference in chromaticity shifts between nasal and temporal retina for either channel. For matches beyond 19 degrees L-M activation is significantly lower in the nasal field and the S-(L+M) channel was equally activated in both fields. The data are consistent with the asymmetric distribution of L- and M-cones in the nasal and temporal retinae.


Subject(s)
Color Perception/physiology , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Retina/physiology , Retinal Cone Photoreceptor Cells/physiology , Visual Fields/physiology , Color Perception Tests/methods , Humans , Photic Stimulation/methods
11.
Vision Res ; 49(16): 2056-66, 2009 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19460399

ABSTRACT

Many classical experiments have shown that two superimposed gratings are more easily detected than a single grating, in keeping with probability theory. Here we test the rules for the detection of 2-component compound gratings by extending the range of parameters used in previous experiments. Two complementary methods of deriving summation indices are described. Data are presented so that the conditions for the transition from probability to neural summation are easily identified. True probability summation occurs only when grating contrasts are carefully perceptually equalised and spatial frequency differs by more than a factor of 2. A wide range of contrast ratios of the component gratings were explored such that gratings were at different contrasts, relative to respective thresholds. We find clear evidence of suppressive interactions when the compound gratings are composed of a close to threshold low frequency component and a below-threshold higher spatial frequency component.


Subject(s)
Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Adult , Humans , Middle Aged , Motion Perception/physiology , Perceptual Masking , Photic Stimulation , Psychophysics , Sensory Thresholds
12.
Vis Neurosci ; 25(3): 327-31, 2008.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18598403

ABSTRACT

Using double silent substitution, it is possible to generate L-cone and M-cone isolating electroretinograms (ERGs) on a CRT. A major limitation of the technique is that the depth of modulation of cone classes is limited by the restricted luminance of the phosphors and their spectral overlap. To address this problem we have ported the technique to a four-color LED Ganzfeld stimulus (Diagnosis ColorDome). This allows higher retinal illuminances, higher contrasts, and triple silent substitution. With careful control over the retinal area stimulated, we show that the same data can be recorded from both CRT and LED stimuli when luminance, size and cone contrast are kept constant. Importantly, the different temporal profiles of the two devices do not influence the ERG amplitude and phase plots. We present data over a much wider range of luminances (up to about 10,000 trolands) and contrasts with the LED stimulator than previously reported with CRT screens. We conclude that the close resemblance between data obtained with an LED stimulator and with a CRT screen indicate that the differences have a purely physiological origin.


Subject(s)
Color Perception/physiology , Electroretinography/methods , Photoreceptor Cells/physiology , Retina/physiology , Retinal Cone Photoreceptor Cells/physiology , Adult , Contrast Sensitivity , Humans , Middle Aged , Photic Stimulation , Photoreceptor Cells/cytology , Retinal Rod Photoreceptor Cells/physiology
13.
Vis Neurosci ; 25(3): 405-10, 2008.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18598410

ABSTRACT

Simple reaction times (RTs) were measured to brief temporally blurred (total onset 570 ms) Gaussian isoluminant chromatic patches (s.d. 0.5 degrees) whose chromaticities lay along the cardinal chromatic axes (0 degrees, 90 degrees, 180 degrees, and 270 degrees in MBDKL color space). Bipolar adapting stimuli were employed (0 degrees versus 180 degrees or 90 degrees versus 270 degrees). These were larger Gaussian blobs (s.d. 1 degree), modulating sinusoidally between the two hues at 1 Hz. Throughout, the background was illuminant "C" (x = 0.31, y = 0.316, L = 12.5). In a single run, a series of 64 or 32 stimuli were presented without adaptation, followed by 64 or 32 stimuli each of which was preceded by 3 s of adaptation, either along the same or the orthogonal chromatic axis. Finally, 192 or 128 RTs were recorded to measure the time course of recovery from adaptation. Both adapting and test stimuli were presented at fixed supra-threshold contrasts. The effect of adaptation was seen as a lengthening of the RT, which occurred in the first few seconds of the adaptation period. After cessation of adaptation, there was a similarly rapid shortening of RT, although full recovery took 60-90 s. Adaptation gain functions suggested that the S-(L + M) system was less prone to adaptation than L-M.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Ocular/physiology , Color Perception/physiology , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Reaction Time , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Humans , Time Factors
14.
Vis Neurosci ; 23(3-4): 503-7, 2006.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16961987

ABSTRACT

Changes of color perception in the peripheral field are measured using an asymmetric simultaneous matching paradigm. The data confirm previous observations in that saturation changes can be neutralized if the test target is increased in size. However, this compensation does not apply to hue shifts. We show that some hues remain unchanged with eccentricity whereas others exhibit substantial changes. Here the color shifts are plotted in terms of a second-stage cone opponent model. The data suggest that the S-L+M channel is more robust to increasing eccentricity than the L-M channel. Observations are interpreted in terms of the known underlying morphological and physiological differences in these channels.


Subject(s)
Color Perception/physiology , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Retina/cytology , Retinal Cone Photoreceptor Cells/physiology , Visual Fields/physiology , Adaptation, Ocular/physiology , Adult , Attention/physiology , Color Perception Tests/methods , Female , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation/methods , Retina/physiology
15.
Vis Neurosci ; 23(3-4): 543-7, 2006.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16961993

ABSTRACT

A successive, asymmetric color-matching paradigm was used to investigate the link between cone contrast and the stability of perceived colors. We measured the perceived color shifts of 10 Munsell samples, induced by test illuminant A, simulated in u'v' color space. The capacity of the visual system to resist these shifts, otherwise known as color constancy, is measured in terms of the Brunswik ratio, BR. Cone contrasts are calculated with respect to either the physical or perceived background. Subjective cone contrasts show a better fit to the von Kries law than those based on the physical background. Complete cone adaptation occurs when color constancy is high. However we show conditions where cone adaptation seems complete but color constancy is poor.


Subject(s)
Color Perception/physiology , Color , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Retinal Cone Photoreceptor Cells/physiology , Humans , Light , Photic Stimulation/methods , Predictive Value of Tests , Psychophysics , Space Perception
16.
Spat Vis ; 19(2-4): 173-92, 2006.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16862839

ABSTRACT

The light reflected from an object depends both on the object's surface and on the illuminant. Visual systems attempt to resolve this intrinsic ambiguity by comparing the light reflected from the object with respect to the background by computing the difference between the object-background light sampled by three cones. The cone-contrasts for the sample-background stimulus under the test illumination should correspond to the cone-contrasts for samples matched in appearance under the standard background (C). The validity of this cone-contrast rule (Whittle, 2003) and its possible link with stability of perceived colour was studied here using six test illuminants. A successive asymmetric colour-matching task with 40 simulated Munsell samples (value 7, chroma 4) on a neutral background (N7) was used. The subject adjusted the sample under standard illuminant C to match the colour appearance of the test sample under one of the test illuminants. Brunswik Ratio (BR) was used as an index of stability of colour appearance. When computed with respect to the reference illuminant C, the cone contrast rule was violated (particularly for S-cone-contrast). However, if a new reference point based on the perceived colour of the neutral background under the test illumination was used, the cone contrast rule applied. That is, when cone contrasts of the matching samples are computed with respect to this perceived background, they correspond to cone contrasts of the test stimuli. This represents a form of discounting the illuminant for the purpose of determining an object's cone-contrast against the background, which does not vary with background illumination. These cone contrasts, however, do not provide any information about the colour appearance of objects under particular viewing conditions, unless calibrated against a standard by allowing subjects to learn particular colours.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Ocular/physiology , Color Perception/physiology , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Models, Theoretical , Retinal Cone Photoreceptor Cells/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation , Reference Values
17.
Ophthalmic Physiol Opt ; 26(3): 318-25, 2006 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16684158

ABSTRACT

Under dark adapted or dim conditions the mammalian visual system is carefully programmed to respond rapidly to the sudden onset of bright lights. This response, called the dazzle reflex, is controlled from sub-cortical structures of the brain. It is known anecdotally that exposure to a bright light when dark adapted induces an instinctive closure of one eye to reduce the pain associated with dazzle. This binocular summation of the dazzle response has not previously been reported. The dazzle reflex can be measured in human subjects by recording the electrical activity from surface electrodes located near the muscles around the eye. In this paper we report an investigation of the apparent binocular summation of the dazzle reflex using this technique. The data reveal a clear difference between monocular and binocular stimulation, with the binocular response being much larger than the monocular response. Furthermore this monocular/binocular difference arises only if the stimulus duration is longer than approximately 1 s. These observations are interpreted in terms of the known physiology of blink mechanisms.


Subject(s)
Dark Adaptation/physiology , Glare , Oculomotor Muscles/physiology , Reflex/physiology , Vision, Binocular/physiology , Adult , Automobile Driving , Blinking/physiology , Cornea/physiology , Electromyography/methods , Humans , Middle Aged , Photic Stimulation/methods
18.
Vision Res ; 46(19): 3067-78, 2006 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16650450

ABSTRACT

A successive asymmetric colour-matching task was used to study the changes in colour appearance of simulated Munsell samples. Colour shifts were induced with two Planckian illuminants, standard illuminant A (u'=0.256, v'=0.524) and illuminant S (u'=0.174, v'=0.392). Measurements were conducted with a 20 degrees field and a 120 degrees field. Adaptation period varied from 1 to 30s with the smaller field and from 1 to 60s with the larger field. Colour shifts were specified in terms of a modified Brunswik ratio (BR). Higher values of BR were associated with longer adaptation periods but only when the larger background was used. Supplementary experiments showed that the changes in colour appearance were related to a slight shift in the perceived colour of the background. The timing of the colour shifts are modelled in terms of cone opponent responses. High values of BR correspond to almost complete von Kries adaptation in all three cone types.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Ocular/physiology , Color Perception/physiology , Color Perception Tests , Computer Graphics , Humans , Photic Stimulation , Retinal Cone Photoreceptor Cells/physiology , Visual Fields
19.
Inj Prev ; 12(2): 125-8, 2006 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16595429

ABSTRACT

A disproportionate number of fatal injuries occur after dark. The paper presents some statistics of road traffic injuries in a novel way which suggests that low luminance plays a major role in this effect. A sound physiological explanation for this is advanced based on the poor temporal characteristics of rod photoreceptors. It is argued that processing information based on low luminance, low contrast targets is much slower than that for high contrast bright targets. To test the idea, simple visual reaction times were measured under typical low visibility conditions encountered on non-lit roads and were found to be substantially longer than under optimal conditions. It is shown that longer reaction times translate into significantly increased stopping distances. This important point has received insufficient attention in the road safety literature, by the Highways Agency, the police, injury prevention officials, and the UK Highway Code.


Subject(s)
Accidents, Traffic/mortality , Automobile Driving , Lighting , Motion Perception/physiology , Dark Adaptation/physiology , Greece/epidemiology , Humans , Reaction Time/physiology , Task Performance and Analysis , Time Factors , United Kingdom/epidemiology , Vision Tests
20.
Vis Neurosci ; 21(3): 231-5, 2004.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15518193

ABSTRACT

It is implicit in many cone-specific ERG studies that the amplitude is proportional to the numbers of cones stimulated. The objective of these experiments was to test this idea by comparing ERGs obtained from different areas of the retina with histological data on cone-density distributions. The histology (Curcio et al., 1990) shows that the cumulative number of cones in the human retina increases exponentially with stimulus diameter between 0- and 40-deg eccentricity. L-, M-, and (L+M) cone-driven 30-Hz ERGs were obtained from a series of stimuli with one of the following configurations: (1) Circular stimuli of different angular subtense up to 70-deg diameter. (2) Annuli with 70-deg outer diameter but variable inner diameter. (3) Annuli of constant area but increasing eccentricity. Cone contrasts were equalized for each stimulus condition. The modulated and nonmodulated regions of the screen had the same mean hue and luminance. The data suggest that the L+M cone ERG amplitude increases with stimulus diameter in direct proportion to the estimated number of cones stimulated. Furthermore, the total L+M responses appear to be predicted from individual L and M responses by simple linear summation for both the disc and annular stimuli.


Subject(s)
Electroretinography/methods , Photoreceptor Cells/anatomy & histology , Retinal Cone Photoreceptor Cells/anatomy & histology , Retinal Cone Photoreceptor Cells/physiology , Humans
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...