Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 9 de 9
Filter
Add more filters











Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
Perception ; 23(7): 833-48, 1994.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7845773

ABSTRACT

Development of the haptic system was evaluated by examining object exploration and recognition in sighted children between the ages of 3 and 8 years. To determine the importance of visual experience for these abilities, the performance of seven congenitally blind children was compared with that of sighted peers matched for age and gender. Performance was evaluated in terms of the speed and correctness of object identification, thoroughness of exploration of object parts, representation of the global form versus local parts of objects, and the possible role of critical parts in object identification. Four types of common objects were presented: normal-sized, miniaturized small, miniaturized large, and oversized objects. All subjects were required to manipulate and identify these objects haptically, without the aid of vision. Results revealed the emergence of a developmental pattern in all performance measures for sighted children. Older sighted children were not only able to recognize more objects and to do so more quickly, but also were more thorough in their exploration patterns. With increasing age, children appear to change their representation of objects from one based predominantly on global shape to one that incorporates a balance of global shape and specific local parts. In agreement with this, critical parts also played a role in object identification, particularly in older children. Blind and sighted children did not differ in any performance measures, which suggests that previous visual experiences do not determine tactile exploration strategies and are not essential for haptic object recognition.


Subject(s)
Blindness/psychology , Mental Recall , Stereognosis , Attention , Blindness/congenital , Child , Child Development , Child, Preschool , Discrimination Learning , Female , Humans , Male , Reaction Time , Size Perception
2.
Child Dev ; 61(4): 1258-70, 1990 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2209194

ABSTRACT

Infants' acuity in localizing sounds within hemifields was examined by determining the smallest sound shift off 60 degrees and along the horizontal axis that infants could discriminate reliably, that is, minimum audible angle (MAA). Infants 6, 12, and 18 months of age were tested using a Go/No-Go conditioned head-turn procedure in which infants received an equal number of no-change (control) trials and sound-shift (experimental) trials. A correct response (i.e., a head turn toward the loud-speakers) on an experimental trial resulted in visual reinforcement. Localization acuity for sounds within hemifields was fairly poor and improved systematically with increasing age. MAA estimates at each age were significantly higher, indicating poorer localization acuity, in comparison to those obtained previously in research examining infants' resolution of auditory space near midline. These findings are consistent with adult data and demonstrate a finer partitioning of auditory space near midline than within hemifields. The implications of these findings for our understanding of the development of auditory processing mechanisms in sound localization are discussed.


Subject(s)
Attention , Dominance, Cerebral , Psychology, Child , Sound Localization , Age Factors , Female , Humans , Infant , Male , Orientation , Pilot Projects
3.
Percept Mot Skills ; 69(3 Pt 1): 787-802, 1989 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2608392

ABSTRACT

Infants 5, 7, and 9 mo. of age were videorecorded while reaching for a dowel that either was stationary or changed orientation after initiation of a reach. Visual fixation and hand orientation were monitored throughout each reach, as was the efficiency with which infants contacted the stimulus. In reaching for the stationary stimulus, the 5-mo.-olds showed significantly poorer hand orientation adjustment throughout their reach than 9-mo.-olds. The 7-mo.-olds began a reach with the hand poorly oriented as did 5-mo.-olds, but just before object contact their performance matched that of the 9-mo.-olds. On change-orientation trials, the 5-mo.-olds' reaching was disrupted. They missed, stopped, or withdrew their reach on over half of these trials, and hand orientation on contact was significantly worse than on no-change trials. By contrast, older infants were more skilled in using visual feedback to make corrective adjustments in hand orientation during execution of the reach to secure a smooth contact with the stimulus. These infants showed no difference in hand orientation on contact for change in comparison to no-change trials. The 9-mo.-olds' performance surpassed that of the 7-mo.-olds only in the efficiency with which they contacted the stimulus and the incidence of grasps on first contact.


Subject(s)
Attention , Orientation , Psychology, Child , Psychomotor Performance , Set, Psychology , Feedback , Fixation, Ocular , Humans , Infant
4.
Child Dev ; 58(4): 918-27, 1987 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3608663

ABSTRACT

Infants 6, 9, 12, 15, and 18 months were seated in a dark room facing a semicircular array comprising 10 loudspeakers, 5 to their right and left at 18 degrees, 36 degrees, 54 degrees, 72 degrees, and 90 degrees from midline. Each infant received 2 types of trials: auditory-alone trials (only a sequence of 8 clicks played), and auditory-visual trials (following the first 4 clicks, a light display at the location of the loudspeaker was activated and remained on for the remaining 4 clicks). Calibration markers on the infant's head were used to measure azimuth error (i.e., the discrepancy between the angle of head turn and loudspeaker location). For auditory-visual trials, all infants turned their head within 4 degrees-6 degrees of the sound source; there was no effect of age or loudspeaker location. For auditory-alone trials, there were significant age differences in performance, with a systematic decrease in discrepancy angle with increasing age. By 18 months there was no difference in performance as a function of localization cue.


Subject(s)
Auditory Perception , Cues , Orientation , Psychology, Child , Sound Localization , Visual Perception , Attention , Child Development , Female , Humans , Infant , Male
5.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 43(2): 181-93, 1987 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3585247

ABSTRACT

Infants 6, 9, 12, 15, and 18 months of age were seated in a dark room directly facing an array of nine loudspeakers positioned along the median vertical plane. One loudspeaker was positioned at ear level, 0 degree, and four others each were positioned above and below 0 degree. To examine infants' resolution of auditory space in the median vertical plane we sought to determine the smallest angular shift in the vertical location of a sound that infants could reliably detect (i.e., minimum audible angle). A two-alternative forced-choice procedure was used in which a sequence of white noise bursts was presented initially at 0 degree, and then shifted vertically (i.e., above or below 0 degree) and continued to be presented until the infant made a directional response; correct responses were visually reinforced. The smallest angular shift in vertical location that was reliably detected systematically decreased with increasing age between 6 months (15 degrees) and 18 months (4 degrees), suggesting a finer partitioning of auditory space along the vertical axis over this age range. By 18 months infants' performance matched that of a group of adults tested under the same circumstances.


Subject(s)
Auditory Perception , Auditory Threshold , Psychology, Child , Sound Localization , Child Development , Female , Humans , Infant , Male
6.
Toxicol Appl Pharmacol ; 82(1): 104-11, 1986 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3003964

ABSTRACT

Acute and 1-month toxicity studies with SCH 31846, a nonsulfhydryl anti-hypertensive agent which acts by inhibiting angiotensin-converting enzyme, were initiated to evaluate its toxicity. The oral LD50s in mice and rats were approximately 1.8 and 2.5 g/kg, respectively, while the iv LD50 was approximately 450 mg/kg in mice and 150 mg/kg in rats. Signs of acute toxicity in rats and mice included salivation, hypoactivity, ataxia, prostration, and convulsions. In a 1-month dog study at oral doses of 25, 75, or 150 mg/kg, there was a dose-related increase in emesis between 1 and 2 hr after dosing. Absorption studies showed peak blood concentrations occurring in dogs between 0.3 and 1 hr after dosing. No other noteworthy antemortem changes were observed. In a 1-month rat study at oral doses of 30, 180, or 600 mg/kg, the hematocrit and hemoglobin values of the 600 mg/kg-dosed female rats were slightly but significantly (p less than 0.05) decreased and the blood urea nitrogen was slightly but significantly (p less than 0.05) increased in all SCH 31846-dosed male rats and the 600 mg/kg-dosed female rats. Absorption studies in male rats at doses of 30, 180, and 600 mg/kg indicate that SCH 31846 is well absorbed in rats. The 150 mg/kg-dosed dogs and the 180- and 600 mg/kg-dosed rats had a slight increase in the number of renin-containing granules in the renal juxtaglomerular cells. No other compound-related microscopic changes were observed. These data are similar to data reported for Captopril and suggest that in the dog and rat the toxicity of ACE inhibitors is not dependent upon the presence or absence of a sulfhydryl group.


Subject(s)
Angiotensin-Converting Enzyme Inhibitors , Indoles/toxicity , Administration, Oral , Animals , Blood Urea Nitrogen , Dogs , Female , Heart/drug effects , Hematocrit , Hemoglobins/analysis , Indoles/administration & dosage , Lethal Dose 50 , Male , Mice , Organ Size/drug effects , Rats , Time Factors
7.
Fundam Appl Toxicol ; 5(5): 986-90, 1985 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-4065467

ABSTRACT

G-418 is a unique aminoglycoside antibiotic that is structurally related to gentamicin; however, unlike gentamicin, G-418 inhibits growth of both procaryotic and eucaryotic cells. In a preliminary acute oral safety study, adult male and female beagles were given a single oral dose of either 2000, 1000, 500, 200, or 50 mg/kg of G-418. Ulceration of the lip, tongue, and/or gingiva occurred in all G-418-dosed dogs 1 to 9 days after dosing. Ulceration of the glans penis, penis sheath, and scrotum occurred 7 to 14 days after a single oral dose with 1000 and 500 mg/kg G-418, and ulceration of the vaginal mucosa of the 2000-, 1000-, 500-, and 50-mg/kg-dosed female dogs occurred 2 to 8 days after dosing. Ulcers of the lip and vaginal area began at the mucocutaneous border and were more severe at these borders. In some dogs a yellow membrane formed over these lesions. Ulceration of the oral and vaginal mucosa disappeared 10 days after the first occurrence and reoccurred 3-7 days later. All ulcers healed within 30 days after the single oral dose; however, at necropsy hemorhagic areas of the urinary bladder were observed in at least one of two dogs at each dose level. Similar lesions have not been reported in animals treated with any other aminoglycoside antibiotics. The etiology of these lesions is unknown.


Subject(s)
Anti-Bacterial Agents/toxicity , Gentamicins/toxicity , Skin Ulcer/chemically induced , Stomatitis/chemically induced , Aminoglycosides/toxicity , Animals , Body Weight/drug effects , Dogs , Female , Male , Mouth Mucosa/drug effects , Skin Ulcer/pathology , Time Factors
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL