Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 9 de 9
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
J Pediatr ; 257: 113326, 2023 06.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36646247

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: To describe bilirubin levels in neonates ≥350/7 gestational weeks, receiving delayed cord clamping (CC), in relation to the updated Bhutani nomogram. STUDY DESIGN: This was a retrospective, observational study based on data from medical records and local data sheets. Singleton neonates, born vaginally at a gestational age ≥350/7, and with a registered time to CC and at least one registered bilirubin, were included. We excluded neonates with positive direct antiglobulin test or hemolytic disorders. Adjusted analyses were performed using ANOVA and linear or logistic regression. RESULTS: We analyzed 558 neonates, mean gestational age (SD) 39.9 (1.3) weeks. CC was performed at a median (IQR) time of 6 (5-8) minutes. The dataset contained 1330 bilirubin measurements. Median (IQR) age at bilirubin measurement was 37 (22-54) hours. Bilirubin percentiles in neonates with CC time ≥2 minutes were similar, or lower, compared with the Bhutani nomogram between 12 and 72 hours, but with greater 95th percentile at later hours of age. Phototherapy was initiated in 13 (2.3 %) of the neonates. We found no association between time to CC and hyperbilirubinemia (ß = -0.05, P = .07). Need for phototherapy was marginally greater in neonates with shorter time to CC. CONCLUSIONS: Bilirubin levels were not correlated to time to CC. Our findings indicate that CC beyond 2 minutes can be performed without additional monitoring for jaundice.


Subject(s)
Hyperbilirubinemia , Umbilical Cord Clamping , Female , Pregnancy , Humans , Infant , Bilirubin , Gestational Age , Retrospective Studies
2.
Acta Obstet Gynecol Scand ; 97(5): 591-597, 2018 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29360141

ABSTRACT

INTRODUCTION: An antenatal corticosteroid (ACS) delivery interval of 24 h to seven days is commonly referred to as optimal timing. We aimed to investigate whether the ACS delivery interval was associated with the obstetric indication for treatment and with neonatal complications. MATERIAL AND METHODS: The study was a retrospective chart review of clinical data from preterm neonates delivered at the Skåne University Hospital, Lund University, Sweden, from 1 January 2013 to 31 December 2016. The ACS delivery intervals were compared between groups of women with various clinical scenarios and related to neonatal outcomes. RESULTS: The study included 498 preterm neonates from 431 women. One to seven days before delivery, 41% of the women received ACS. Women with preterm prelabor rupture of membranes or vaginal bleeding had a median ACS delivery interval of 7.5 and eight days, respectively, compared with women with maternal/fetal indications or preterm labor (three and two days, respectively) (p < 0.001). Neonates with an ACS delivery interval of more than seven days were at a higher risk of respiratory distress syndrome [odds ratio (OR) 2.00, 95% confidence interval (CI) 1.05-3.79] and moderate or severe bronchopulmonary dysplasia (OR 2.78, 95% CI 1.45-5.33) than were neonates with an ACS delivery interval of one to seven days. CONCLUSION: Optimal timing of ACS treatment varied significantly based on the clinical indication. Women with preterm prelabor rupture of membranes or vaginal bleeding were more likely to have an ACS delivery interval of more than seven days. A prolonged ACS delivery interval was associated with an increased risk of neonatal respiratory morbidity and a prolonged stay in the neonatal care unit, but not with neonatal mortality.


Subject(s)
Adrenal Cortex Hormones/administration & dosage , Anti-Inflammatory Agents/administration & dosage , Fetal Membranes, Premature Rupture/drug therapy , Obstetric Labor, Premature/drug therapy , Prenatal Care/methods , Respiratory Distress Syndrome, Newborn/prevention & control , Adolescent , Adrenal Cortex Hormones/therapeutic use , Adult , Anti-Inflammatory Agents/therapeutic use , Drug Administration Schedule , Female , Humans , Infant, Newborn , Infant, Premature , Male , Middle Aged , Pregnancy , Retrospective Studies , Treatment Outcome , Young Adult
3.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 20(5): 779-86, 2008 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18201127

ABSTRACT

Intraindividual variability (IIV) in cognitive performance shares systematic associations with aging-related processes, brain injury, and neurodegenerative pathology. However, little research has examined the neural underpinnings of IIV, with no studies investigating brain correlates of IIV in relation to retrieval success. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging, we examined links between IIV, recognition memory performance, and blood oxygenation level dependent activations. Nineteen older adults (70-79 years) were presented with 80 words at encoding, with brain scans and response latencies obtained during subsequent recognition. An index of IIV, the intraindividual standard deviation (ISD), was computed across successful latency trials. Decreasing ISDs were systematically associated with better recognition, faster latencies, and increased activation in the inferior parietal cortex (BA 40). Demonstrated links between less behavioral variability and parietal activations are consistent with the known importance of the parietal cortex for retrieval success. In support of extant findings and theory from neuroscience, neuropsychology, and cognitive aging, the present results suggest that behavioral IIV represents a proxy for neural integrity.


Subject(s)
Aging/physiology , Parietal Lobe/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Recognition, Psychology/physiology , Verbal Learning/physiology , Aged , Cognition/physiology , Female , Humans , Individuality , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Oxygen/blood , Parietal Lobe/metabolism , Reference Values , Reproducibility of Results
4.
Emotion ; 7(4): 767-73, 2007 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18039046

ABSTRACT

Event-related functional MRI (fMRI) was used to assess brain activity during encoding of fearful and neutral faces in 12 women and 12 men. In a subsequent memory analysis, the authors separated successful from unsuccessful encoding of both types of faces, based on whether they were remembered or forgotten in a later recognition memory test. Overall, women and men recruited overlapping neural circuitries. Both sexes activated right-sided medial-temporal regions during successful encoding of fearful faces. Successful encoding of neutral faces was associated with left-sided lateral prefrontal and right-sided superior frontal activation in both sexes. In women, relatively greater encoding related activity for neutral faces was seen in the superior parietal and parahippocampal cortices. By contrast, men activated the left and right superior/middle frontal cortex more than women during successful encoding of the same neutral faces. These findings suggest that women and men use similar neural networks to encode facial information, with only subtle sex differences observed for neutral faces.


Subject(s)
Affect , Brain/physiology , Face , Facial Expression , Memory , Adult , Brain/anatomy & histology , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male
5.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev ; 30(6): 864-71, 2006.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16904746

ABSTRACT

There is evidence for cognitive as well as neural plasticity across the adult life span, although aging is associated with certain constraints on plasticity. In the current paper, we argue that the age-related reduction in cognitive plasticity may be due to (a) deficits in general processing resources, and (b) failure to engage in task-relevant cognitive operations. Memory-training research suggests that age-related processing deficits (e.g., executive functions, speed) hinder older adults from utilizing mnemonic techniques as efficiently as the young, and that this age difference is reflected by diminished frontal activity during mnemonic use. Additional constraints on memory plasticity in old age are related to difficulties that are specific to the task, such as creating visual images, as well as in binding together the information to be remembered. These deficiencies are paralleled by reduced activity in occipito-parietal and medial-temporal regions, respectively. Future attempts to optimize intervention-related gains in old age should consider targeting both general processing and task-specific origins of age-associated reductions in cognitive plasticity.


Subject(s)
Aging/physiology , Cognition/physiology , Learning/physiology , Neuronal Plasticity/physiology , Age Factors , Animals , Humans , Neuropsychological Tests/statistics & numerical data
6.
Neurosci Lett ; 386(2): 99-104, 2005 Sep 30.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15993537

ABSTRACT

Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) was used to study age-related differences in the neural circuitry involved in perception of negative facial affect. During scanning, 24 younger and 22 older adults viewed blocks of angry and neutral faces. The fMRI data analysis of the angry versus neutral faces contrast demonstrated greater activation in younger versus older individuals in the right amygdala/hippocampus region, whereas older adults demonstrated greater activation in the right anterior-ventral insula cortex. Hence, normal aging seems to affect specific nodes in the neural network involved in processing negative emotional face information. This age-related change from more subcortical to more cortical involvement could reflect functional compensation within the neural system involved in perception of facial affect, or the fact that older adults process emotional information in a different manner than do young adults.


Subject(s)
Aging , Anger , Brain Mapping , Brain/physiology , Facial Expression , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male
7.
Neuroreport ; 15(2): 235-8, 2004 Feb 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15076743

ABSTRACT

Sex-discriminating facial features are examples of visual information involved in guiding social behavior. We used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to assess brain responses in face-relevant brain areas in men and women during exposure to neutral male and female faces. An increased fMRI signal was found in the left amygdala and adjacent anterior temporal regions in men, but not in women, during exposure to faces of the opposite versus the same sex. These data indicate that the relationship between the sex of the subject and the sex of the face affects activity in the inferior temporal lobe. The sex-differential nature of this activation pattern may reflect sex differences in cognitive style and attentional processes when confronting faces of the opposite sex.


Subject(s)
Amygdala/physiology , Face , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Sex Characteristics , Temporal Lobe/physiology , Adult , Attention/physiology , Brain Mapping , Cognition/physiology , Female , Functional Laterality/physiology , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Photic Stimulation , Temporal Lobe/anatomy & histology
8.
Neuroimage ; 20(3): 1795-810, 2003 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14642489

ABSTRACT

In a within-subject design we investigated the levels-of-processing (LOP) effect using visual material in a behavioral and a corresponding PET study. In the behavioral study we characterize a generalized LOP effect, using pleasantness and graphical quality judgments in the encoding situation, with two types of visual material, figurative and nonfigurative line drawings. In the PET study we investigate the related pattern of brain activations along these two dimensions. The behavioral results indicate that instruction and material contribute independently to the level of recognition performance. Therefore the LOP effect appears to stem both from the relative relevance of the stimuli (encoding opportunity) and an altered processing of stimuli brought about by the explicit instruction (encoding mode). In the PET study, encoding of visual material under the pleasantness (deep) instruction yielded left lateralized frontoparietal and anterior temporal activations while surface-based perceptually oriented processing (shallow instruction) yielded right lateralized frontoparietal, posterior temporal, and occipitotemporal activations. The result that deep encoding was related to the left prefrontal cortex while shallow encoding was related to the right prefrontal cortex, holding the material constant, is not consistent with the HERA model. In addition, we suggest that the anterior medial superior frontal region is related to aspects of self-referential semantic processing and that the inferior parts of the anterior cingulate as well as the medial orbitofrontal cortex is related to affective processing, in this case pleasantness evaluation of the stimuli regardless of explicit semantic content. Finally, the left medial temporal lobe appears more actively engaged by elaborate meaning-based processing and the complex response pattern observed in different subregions of the MTL lends support to the suggestion that this region is functionally segregated.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Brain/diagnostic imaging , Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Cerebrovascular Circulation/physiology , Female , Frontal Lobe/physiology , Humans , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted , Male , Parietal Lobe/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Tomography, Emission-Computed
9.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 100(23): 13728-33, 2003 Nov 11.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14597711

ABSTRACT

Cognitive studies show that both younger and older adults can increase their memory performance after training in using a visuospatial mnemonic, although age-related memory deficits tend to be magnified rather than reduced after training. Little is known about the changes in functional brain activity that accompany training-induced memory enhancement, and whether age-related activity changes are associated with the size of training-related gains. Here, we demonstrate that younger adults show increased activity during memory encoding in occipito-parietal and frontal brain regions after learning the mnemonic. Older adults did not show increased frontal activity, and only those elderly persons who benefited from the mnemonic showed increased occipito-parietal activity. These findings suggest that age-related differences in cognitive reserve capacity may reflect both a frontal processing deficiency and a posterior production deficiency.


Subject(s)
Aging , Memory , Neurons/pathology , Adult , Aged , Brain/pathology , Female , Humans , Male , Memory Disorders , Memory, Short-Term , Mental Recall , Practice, Psychological , Prefrontal Cortex/pathology
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...