Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 7 de 7
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
Data Brief ; 54: 110420, 2024 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38698803

ABSTRACT

Energy system modelling can be used to provide scenario-based insights in energy system transition pathways. However, data accessibility is a common barrier for the model representation of energy systems, both regarding existing infrastructure, as well as planned developments consistent with current policies. This paper describes the 'Global Transmission Database', the first global dataset covering existing and planned electricity transmission developments between countries and selected regions. The dataset can be used as a starting point for the representation of cross-regional electricity grids globally in energy system models and other computational tools. All data is collected from publicly available sources and combined into a single machine-readable format for convenient application.

2.
Rapid Commun Mass Spectrom ; 36(3): e9228, 2022 Feb 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34845773

ABSTRACT

RATIONALE: Derivatization with dimethyl disulfide (DMDS) followed by gas chromatography/mass spectrometry (GC/MS) analysis is a well-established method for locating double-bond position on the alkyl chain of mono-unsaturated compounds such as alkenes. For alkenes containing more than one double bond, however, the conventional DMDS derivatization approach forms poly- or cyclized DMDS adducts whose mass spectra are difficult to interpret in terms of double-bond positions. In this study, we report an efficient experimental procedure to produce mono-DMDS adducts for polyunsaturated alkenes with two to six double bonds. GC/MS analyses of these mono-DMDS adducts yield highly characteristic mass fragments, allowing unambiguous assignments of double-bond positions on the alkyl chain. We also apply our new approach (i.e., preferential formation of mono-DMDS adducts during derivatization with DMDS) to determine the double-bond positions of unsaturated alkenes produced by laboratory cultured Isochrysis litoralis, a haptophyte algal species. METHODS: Alkenes from different sources were derivatized with DMDS at 25°C for 20 to 160 min. The mass spectra of mono-DMDS adducts were obtained by GC/EI-MS analysis of reaction products which contain chromatographically resolved mono-DMDS adducts. RESULTS: Mass spectra of corresponding mono-DMDS adducts contain prominent diagnostic ions that allow a conclusive elucidation of double-bond positions. In culture samples of Isochrysis litoralis, a series of novel mono- to tri-unsaturated C31 alkenes (9-C31:1 , 6,9-C31:2 , 6,22-C31:2 , 6,25-C31:2 , 9,22-C31:2 , 6,9,25-C31:3 ) were discovered for the first time. CONCLUSIONS: A highly efficient DMDS derivatization approach is developed to yield abundant mono-DMDS adducts of polyunsaturated alkyl alkenes for elucidating double-bond positions using GC/MS.

3.
Neuroimage ; 101: 653-66, 2014 Nov 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25067812

ABSTRACT

Despite anecdotal evidence of relative visuospatial processing strengths in individuals with reading disability (RD), only a few studies have assessed the presence or the extent of these putative strengths. The current study examined the cognitive and neural bases of visuospatial processing abilities in adolescents with RD relative to typically developing (TD) peers. Using both cognitive tasks and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) we contrasted printed word recognition with non-language visuospatial processing tasks. Behaviorally, lower reading skill was related to a visuospatial processing advantage (shorter latencies and equivalent accuracy) on a geometric figure processing task, similar to findings shown in two published studies. FMRI analyses revealed key group by task interactions in patterns of cortical and subcortical activation, particularly in frontostriatal networks, and in the distributions of right and left hemisphere activation on the two tasks. The results are discussed in terms of a possible neural tradeoff in visuospatial processing in RD.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping/methods , Dyslexia/physiopathology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Reading , Space Perception/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Form Perception/physiology , Functional Laterality/physiology , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Young Adult
4.
Brain Res ; 1037(1-2): 35-42, 2005 Mar 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15777750

ABSTRACT

Williams Syndrome (WS) is a neurodevelopment disorder associated with a hemizygous deletion on chromosome 7. WS is characterized with mental retardation, severe visual-spatial deficits, relative language preservation, and excellent facial recognition. Distinctive auditory features include musical ability, heightened sound sensitivity, and specific patterns of auditory evoked potentials. These features have led to the hypothesis that the dorsal forebrain is more affected than the ventral. Previously, we reported primary visual area 17 abnormalities in rostral striate cortex, a region contributing to the dorsal visual pathway. Based on the dorsal-ventral hypothesis, and language and auditory findings, we predicted a more normal histometric picture in auditory area 41. We used an optical dissector method to measure neurons in layers II-VI of area 41 in right and left hemispheres of the same 3 WS and 3 control brains used in the area 17 study. There was a hemisphere by diagnosis interaction in cell packing density (CPD) in layer IV and in cell size in layer III between WS and control brains. Post hoc analysis disclosed in control brains, but not WS, a layer IV left > right asymmetry in CPD, and a layer III left < right asymmetry in cell size. WS brains showed more large neurons bilaterally in layer II and in left layer VI. Histometric alterations in area 41 were less widespread than rostral visual cortex. Also, there was less asymmetry in the WS brain. We interpret layers II and VI differences as reflecting increased limbic connectivity in primary auditory cortex of WS.


Subject(s)
Auditory Cortex/pathology , Williams Syndrome/pathology , Adult , Cell Count , Cell Size , Female , Functional Laterality/physiology , Humans , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted , Male , Middle Aged , Neurons/pathology , Neurons/ultrastructure
5.
Brain Lang ; 85(3): 427-31, 2003 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12744954

ABSTRACT

Dyslexia has long been defined by deficit. Nevertheless, the view that visual-spatial talents accompany dyslexia has grown, due to reports of individuals with dyslexia who possess visual-spatial strengths, findings of elevated incidence of dyslexia in certain visual-spatial professions, and the hypothesis that left-hemisphere deficits accompany right-hemisphere strengths. Studies have reported superior, inferior, and average levels of visual-spatial abilities associated with dyslexia. In two investigations, we found an association between dyslexia and speed of recognition of impossible figures, a global visual-spatial task. This finding suggests that dyslexia is associated with a particular type of visual-spatial talent-enhanced ability to process visual-spatial information globally (holistically) rather than locally (part by part).


Subject(s)
Aptitude , Creativity , Dyslexia/physiopathology , Space Perception/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Random Allocation , Reaction Time
6.
Arch Neurol ; 59(9): 1461-7, 2002 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12223034

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Williams syndrome (WMS) is a rare, genetically based syndrome associated with a hemideletion in chromosome 7 (7q11.22-23) and characterized by a unique constellation of somatic, brain, and cognitive features. Individuals with WMS demonstrate an unusual and uneven neuropsychological profile showing cognitive and visual spatial deficits juxtaposed with relative language preservation and excellent facial recognition. OBJECTIVES: A neuroanatomical hypothesis for these behavioral findings suggests predominant involvement of the dorsal portions of the hemispheres relative to the ventral portions, including preferential involvement of peripheral visual field cortical representations over central representation. Predominant involvement of magnocellular visual pathways, as opposed to parvocellular pathways, is also suggested by this hypothesis. SUBJECTS: We examined primary visual cortical area 17 in the right and left hemispheres in 6 age- and sex-matched autopsy specimens from 3 WMS-affected brains (1 male and 2 females; mean [SD] age, 44 [14] years) and 3 control brains (1 male and 2 females; mean age, 43 [11] years). DESIGN: Neurons in layers II, III, IVA, IVB, IVCalpha, IVCbeta, V, and VI were measured using an optical dissector method to determine possible differences between WMS-affected and control brains in cell-packing density, neuronal size, and neuronal size distribution. RESULTS: We found abnormalities in peripheral visual cortex in WMS-affected brains, but not in magnocellular subdivisions. There was a hemisphere by layer IV interaction and a layer IV left hemisphere and diagnosis interaction in cell-packing density. Williams syndrome-affected brains showed increased cell-packing density in left sublayer IVCbeta and an excess of small neurons in left layers IVA, IVCalpha, IVCbeta, V, and VI. CONCLUSIONS: Cell measurements differ in peripheral visual cortical fields of WMS, with significantly smaller, more closely packed cells in some layers on the left side. These cell-packing density and neuronal size differences may be related to visuospatial deficits in this population.


Subject(s)
Neurons/pathology , Visual Cortex/pathology , Williams Syndrome/pathology , Adult , Cell Count , Cell Size , Female , Functional Laterality/physiology , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Visual Pathways/pathology
7.
Behav Brain Res ; 133(2): 247-59, 2002 Jul 18.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12110458

ABSTRACT

About half of BXSB/MpJ-Yaa (BXSB) mice have neocortical ectopias (misplaced clusters of neurons located in layer I of cortex). Previous behavioral studies have suggested that ectopic mice have superior spatial, but equivalent nonspatial, reference memory learning. However, since spatial and nonspatial learning were not assessed in the same apparatus and with the same testing procedure, it is unclear if this conclusion is accurate. We have created a new nonspatial Morris maze for mice that differs from the spatial task only in the type of cues that must be utilized to efficiently locate the platform (intra-maze black/white patterns vs. extra-maze room cues) and does not differ in the level of task complexity or the presence of objects within the maze. Ectopic mice were very good in utilizing extra-maze cues when learning the spatial version and in utilizing intra-maze cues when learning the nonspatial version of the Morris maze, while non-ectopics were not, suggesting that ectopics have superior spatial and nonspatial reference memory. Ectopias in BXSB mice are usually located in prefrontal and/or motor cortex. The prefrontal cortex is involved in behavioral flexibility (e.g. being able to easily switch from using spatial to nonspatial cues). Only ectopic mice with ectopias specifically located in the prefrontal region of cortex demonstrated difficulty switching from using extra-maze to intra-maze cues and vice versa. Thus, the presence of one or more ectopias in the prefrontal region of cortex disrupted one of the normal functions of the prefrontal cortex.


Subject(s)
Behavior, Animal/physiology , Choristoma/physiopathology , Maze Learning/physiology , Prefrontal Cortex/physiopathology , Space Perception/physiology , Animals , Choristoma/genetics , Choristoma/pathology , Cues , Male , Memory/physiology , Mice , Mice, Neurologic Mutants , Prefrontal Cortex/pathology
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL