Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 6 de 6
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
Neuron ; 79(3): 594-606, 2013 Aug 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23932002

ABSTRACT

There is increasing evidence that the phase of ongoing oscillations plays a role in neural coding, but its relative importance throughout the brain has yet to be understood. We assessed single-trial phase coding in four temporal lobe and four frontal lobe regions of the human brain using local field potentials (LFPs) recorded during a card-matching task. In the temporal lobe, classification of correct/incorrect matches based on LFP phase was significantly better than classification based on amplitude and comparable to the full LFP signal. Surprisingly, in these regions, the correct/incorrect mean phases became aligned to one another before they diverged and coded for trial outcome. Neural responses in the amygdala were consistent with a mechanism of phase resetting, while parahippocampal gyrus activity was indicative of evoked potentials. These findings highlight the importance of phase coding in human medial temporal lobe and suggest that different brain regions may represent information in diverse ways.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Mental Processes/physiology , Temporal Lobe/physiology , Adult , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Electrodes, Implanted , Electroencephalography , Female , Frontal Lobe/physiology , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Middle Aged , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Photic Stimulation , Spectrum Analysis , Statistics, Nonparametric
2.
Curr Biol ; 20(3): R93-4, 2010 Feb 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20144775

ABSTRACT

Visual information is used by the brain to construct a conscious experience of the visual world and to guide motor actions [1]. Here we report a study of how eye movements and perception relate to each other. We compared the ability of human observers to perceive image motion with the reliability of their eyes to track the motion of a target [2], [3] and [4], the goal being to test whether both motor and sensory processes are based on the same set of signals and limited by a shared source of noise [2] and [4]. We found that the oculomotor system can detect fluctuations in the velocity of a moving target better than the observer. Surprisingly, in some conditions, eye movements reliably respond to the velocity fluctuations of a moving target that are otherwise perceptually invisible to the subjects. The implication is that visual motion signals exist in the brain that can be used to guide motor actions without evoking a perceptual outcome nor being accessible to conscious scrutiny.


Subject(s)
Eye Movements/physiology , Motion Perception/physiology , Humans , Oculomotor Nerve/physiology , Photic Stimulation , Pursuit, Smooth/physiology
3.
J Neurophysiol ; 102(1): 110-8, 2009 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19369357

ABSTRACT

Smooth pursuit eye movements allow the approximate stabilization of a moving visual target on the retina. To study the dynamics of smooth pursuit, we measured eye velocity during the visual tracking of a Gabor target moving at a constant velocity plus a noisy perturbation term. The optimal linear filter linking fluctuations in target velocity to evoked fluctuations in eye velocity was computed. These filters predicted eye velocity to novel stimuli in the 0- to 15-Hz band with good accuracy, showing that pursuit maintenance is approximately linear under these conditions. The shape of the filters were indicative of fast dynamics, with pure delays of merely approximately 67 ms, times-to-peak of approximately 115 ms, and effective integration times of approximately 45 ms. The gain of the system, reflected in the amplitude of the filters, was inversely proportional to the size of the velocity fluctuations and independent of the target mean speed. A modest slow-down of the dynamics was observed as the contrast of the target decreased. Finally, the temporal filters recovered during fixation and pursuit were similar in shape, supporting the notion that they might share a common underlying circuitry. These findings show that the visual tracking of moving objects by the human eye includes a reflexive-like pathway with high contrast sensitivity and fast dynamics.


Subject(s)
Nonlinear Dynamics , Pursuit, Smooth/physiology , Adult , Attention/physiology , Fixation, Ocular/physiology , Humans , Linear Models , Male , Motion Perception/physiology , Photic Stimulation , Predictive Value of Tests , Reaction Time/physiology , Time Factors , Visual Fields
4.
J Vis ; 7(12): 11.1-8, 2007 Sep 28.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17997653

ABSTRACT

The human visual system is remarkably adept at finding objects of interest in cluttered visual environments, a task termed visual search. Because the human eye is highly foveated, it accomplishes this by making many discrete fixations linked by rapid eye movements called saccades. In such naturalistic tasks, we know very little about how the brain selects saccadic targets (the fixation loci). In this paper, we use a novel technique akin to psychophysical reverse correlation and stimuli that emulate the natural visual environment to measure observers' ability to locate a low-contrast target of unknown orientation. We present three main discoveries. First, we provide strong evidence for saccadic selectivity for spatial frequencies close to the target's central frequency. Second, we demonstrate that observers have distinct, idiosyncratic biases to certain orientations in saccadic programming, although there were no priors imposed on the target's orientation. These orientation biases cover a subset of the near-cardinal (horizontal/vertical) and near-oblique orientations, with orientations near vertical being the most common across observers. Further, these idiosyncratic biases were stable across time. Third, within observers, very similar biases exist for foveal target detection accuracy. These results suggest that saccadic targeting is tuned for known stimulus dimensions (here, spatial frequency) and also has some preference or default tuning for uncertain stimulus dimensions (here, orientation).


Subject(s)
Light , Orientation/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Anisotropy , Fovea Centralis/physiology , Humans , Observer Variation , Saccades/physiology , Signal Detection, Psychological/physiology , Space Perception/physiology
5.
Percept Psychophys ; 69(1): 103-12, 2007 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17515220

ABSTRACT

We propose a novel variant of the classification image paradigm that allows us to rapidly reveal strategies used by observers in visual search tasks. We make use of eye tracking, 1/f noise, and a grid-like stimulus ensemble and also introduce a new classification taxonomy that distinguishes between foveal and peripheral processes. We tested our method for 3 human observers and two simple shapes used as search targets. The classification images obtained show the efficacy of the proposed method by revealing the features used by the observers in as few as 200 trials. Using two control experiments, we evaluated the use of naturalistic 1/f noise with classification images, in comparison with the more commonly used white noise, and compared the performance of our technique with that of an earlier approach without a stimulus grid.


Subject(s)
Attention , Field Dependence-Independence , Orientation , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Saccades , Visual Fields , Adult , Contrast Sensitivity , Humans , Perceptual Distortion , Perceptual Masking , Psychophysics , Reaction Time
6.
Appl Opt ; 43(8): 1695-702, 2004 Mar 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15046173

ABSTRACT

The optical cross correlation of an image with another image that was spatial-frequency shifted in one dimension was demonstrated in a photorefractive VanderLugt correlator. The first image was stored as a Fourier-transform hologram in a photorefractive Bi12SiO20 crystal (BSO) and was successively correlated with different spatial-frequency-shifted versions of a second image. We implemented the spatial-frequency shift by rotating a galvanometer mirror in an image plane, causing the Fourier transform to be shifted laterally in the BSO. We verified that the resulting operation in the BSO was an accurate complex multiplication of the shifted and the stored Fourier transforms. As many as 20 successive readouts were conducted without measurable erasure of the stored hologram. The dynamic range, saturation behavior, and other performance parameters were measured and are discussed.

SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...