Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 84
Filter
1.
Neuroimage Clin ; 39: 103470, 2023.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37459698

ABSTRACT

White matter connections enable the interaction within and between brain networks. Brain lesions can cause structural disconnections that disrupt networks and thereby cognitive functions supported by them. In recent years, novel methods have been developed to quantify the extent of structural disconnection after focal lesions, using tractography data from healthy controls. These methods, however, are indirect and their reliability and validity have yet to be fully established. In this study, we present our implementation of this approach, in a tool supplemented by uncertainty metrics for the predictions overall and at voxel-level. These metrics give an indication of the reliability and are used to compare predictions with direct measures from patients' diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) data in a sample of 95 first-ever stroke patients. Results show that, except for small lesions, the tool can predict fiber loss with high reliability and compares well to direct patient DTI estimates. Clinical utility of the method was demonstrated using lesion data from a subset of patients suffering from hemianopia. Both tract-based measures outperformed lesion localization in mapping visual field defects and showed a network consistent with the known anatomy of the visual system. This study offers an important contribution to the validation of structural disconnection mapping. We show that indirect measures of structural disconnection can be a reliable and valid substitute for direct estimations of fiber loss after focal lesions. Moreover, based on these results, we argue that indirect structural disconnection measures may even be preferable to lower-quality single subject diffusion MRI when based on high-quality healthy control datasets.


Subject(s)
Stroke , White Matter , Humans , Diffusion Tensor Imaging/methods , Reproducibility of Results , Diffusion Magnetic Resonance Imaging , White Matter/diagnostic imaging , White Matter/pathology , Stroke/diagnostic imaging , Stroke/pathology
2.
Neuroimage Clin ; 37: 103305, 2023.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36610310

ABSTRACT

INTRODUCTION: Lesion-symptom mapping is a key tool in understanding the relationship between brain structures and behavior. However, the behavioral consequences of lesions from different etiologies may vary because of how they affect brain tissue and how they are distributed. The inclusion of different etiologies would increase the statistical power but has been critically debated. Meanwhile, findings from lesion studies are a valuable resource for clinicians and used across different etiologies. Therefore, the main objective of the present study was to directly compare lesion-symptom maps for memory and language functions from two populations, a tumor versus a stroke population. METHODS: Data from two different studies were combined. Both the brain tumor (N = 196) and stroke (N = 147) patient populations underwent neuropsychological testing and an MRI, pre-operatively for the tumor population and within three months after stroke. For this study, we selected two internationally widely used standardized cognitive tasks, the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test and the Verbal Fluency Test. We used a state-of-the-art machine learning-based, multivariate voxel-wise approach to produce lesion-symptom maps for these cognitive tasks for both populations separately and combined. RESULTS: Our lesion-symptom mapping results for the separate patient populations largely followed the expected neuroanatomical pattern based on previous literature. Substantial differences in lesion distribution hindered direct comparison. Still, in brain areas with adequate coverage in both groups, considerable LSM differences between the two populations were present for both memory and fluency tasks. Post-hoc analyses of these locations confirmed that the cognitive consequences of focal brain damage varied between etiologies. CONCLUSION: The differences in the lesion-symptom maps between the stroke and tumor population could partly be explained by differences in lesion volume and topography. Despite these methodological limitations, both the lesion-symptom mapping results and the post-hoc analyses confirmed that etiology matters when investigating the cognitive consequences of lesions with lesion-symptom mapping. Therefore, caution is advised with generalizing lesion-symptom results across etiologies.


Subject(s)
Neoplasms , Stroke , Humans , Brain Mapping/methods , Stroke/pathology , Brain/diagnostic imaging , Brain/pathology , Neuropsychological Tests , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Neoplasms/pathology
3.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34493161

ABSTRACT

Apathy is common after stroke and has been associated with cognitive impairment. However, causality between post-stroke apathy and cognitive impairment remains unclear. We assessed the course of apathy in relation to changes in cognitive functioning in stroke survivors. Using the Apathy Scale (AS) and cognitive tests on memory, processing speed and executive functioning at six- and 15 months post-stroke we tested for associations between (1) AS-scores and (change in) cognitive scores; (2) apathy course (persistent/incident/resolved) and cognitive change scores. Of 117 included participants, 29% had persistent apathy, 13% apathy resolving over time and 10% apathy emerging between 6-15 months post-stroke. Higher AS-scores were cross-sectionally and longitudinally associated with lower cognitive scores. Relations between apathy and cognitive change scores were ambiguous. These inconsistent relations between apathy and changes in cognition over time suggest that post-stroke apathy does not directly impact cognitive performance. Both these sequelae of stroke require separate attention.


Subject(s)
Apathy , Cognitive Dysfunction , Stroke , Humans , Prospective Studies , Longitudinal Studies , Cognition , Stroke/complications , Cognitive Dysfunction/psychology
4.
Neuropsychologia ; 128: 270-275, 2019 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29604321

ABSTRACT

Blindsight refers to the observation of residual visual abilities in the hemianopic field of patients without a functional V1. Given the within- and between-subject variability in the preserved abilities and the phenomenal experience of blindsight patients, the fine-grained description of the phenomenon is still debated. Here we tested a patient with established "perceptual" and "attentional" blindsight (c.f. Danckert and Rossetti, 2005). Using a pointing paradigm patient MS, who suffers from a complete left homonymous hemianopia, showed clear above chance manual localisation of 'unseen' targets. In addition, target presentations in his blind field led MS, on occasion, to spontaneous responses towards his sighted field. Structural and functional magnetic resonance imaging was conducted to evaluate the magnitude of V1 damage. Results revealed the presence of a calcarine sulcus in both hemispheres, yet his right V1 is reduced, structurally disconnected and shows no fMRI response to visual stimuli. Thus, visual stimulation of his blind field can lead to "action blindsight" and spontaneous antipointing, in absence of a functional right V1. With respect to the antipointing, we suggest that MS may have registered the stimulation and subsequently presumes it must have been in his intact half field.


Subject(s)
Blindness/psychology , Hemianopsia/psychology , Vision, Ocular , Attention , Blindness/diagnostic imaging , Blindness/etiology , Hemianopsia/complications , Hemianopsia/diagnostic imaging , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Photic Stimulation , Psychomotor Performance , Visual Cortex/diagnostic imaging , Visual Cortex/physiopathology , Visual Fields , Visual Perception , Young Adult
5.
J Neuropsychol ; 5(Pt 1): 15-38, 2011 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21366885

ABSTRACT

Recent functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies addressing healthy subjects point towards posterior parietal cortex (PPC) involvement in episodic memory tasks. This is noteworthy, since neuropsychological studies usually do not connect parietal lesions to episodic memory impairments. Therefore an inventory of the possible factors behind this apparent paradox is warranted. This review compared fMRI studies which demonstrated PPC activity in episodic memory tasks, with findings with studies of patients with PPC lesions. A systematic evaluation of possible explanations for the posterior parietal paradox indicates that PPC activation in fMRI studies does not appear to be attributable to confounding cognitive/psychomotor processes, such as button pressing or stimulus processing. What may be of more importance is the extent to which an episodic memory task loads on three closely related cognitive processes: effort and attention, self-related activity, and scene and image construction. We discuss to what extent these cognitive processes can account for the paradox between lesion and fMRI results. They are strongly intertwined with the episodic memory and may critically determine in how far the PPC plays a role in a given memory task. Future patient studies might profit from specifically taking these cognitive factors into consideration in the task design.


Subject(s)
Brain Injuries/pathology , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Mental Recall/physiology , Parietal Lobe/blood supply , Parietal Lobe/physiology , Attention/physiology , Brain Injuries/physiopathology , Brain Mapping , Female , Functional Laterality/physiology , Humans , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted/methods , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Oxygen/blood , PubMed/statistics & numerical data
7.
Restor Neurol Neurosci ; 26(1): 1-12, 2008.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18431002

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: At present, prism adaptation is probably the most promising rehabilitation procedure for hemi-neglect. However, randomised controlled trials are lacking and no data are available on the effectiveness of prism adaptation in the treatment of acute neglect. METHODS: We followed sixteen neglect patients using a randomised controlled design in which six patients received four-day-in-a-row placebo treatment (CG) and ten patients received four-day-in-a row experimental treatment with 10 degrees rightward deviating prisms (EG) during their stay on the stroke unit. We examined whether patients in the EG improved faster than the CG by administering three neglect tasks (Schenkenberg Line Bisection, Letter Cancellation, Gainotti Scene Copying) immediately before and after each treatment. Second, we examined whether patients in the EG demonstrated a better long-term outcome at one month post-treatment (Behavioural Inattention Test). RESULTS: Patients in the EG improved faster on spatial tasks (line bisection, cancellation) than the CG but not on visuo-construction. Patients in the EG showed no differences with the CG in neglect outcome at one month post-treatment. CONCLUSIONS: Four consecutive prism sessions produced beneficial effects in patients with acute neglect. However, prism effects were either short-term, or placebo treatment with repeated pointing and/or repeated neglect testing was more helpful than we anticipated. Our results emphasize the importance of a placebo condition and a follow-up in rehabilitation studies.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Ocular/physiology , Eyeglasses , Perceptual Disorders/rehabilitation , Space Perception/physiology , Adult , Aged , Female , Functional Laterality/physiology , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Photic Stimulation , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Single-Blind Method
8.
Psychol Med ; 38(8): 1177-84, 2008 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18076771

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: It has recently been suggested that auditory hallucinations are the result of a criterion shift when deciding whether or not a meaningful signal has emerged. The approach proposes that a liberal criterion may result in increased false-positive identifications, without additional perceptual deficit. To test this hypothesis, we devised a speech discrimination task and used signal detection theory (SDT) to investigate the underlying cognitive mechanisms. METHOD: Schizophrenia patients with and without auditory hallucinations and a healthy control group completed a speech discrimination task. They had to decide whether a particular spoken word was identical to a previously presented speech stimulus, embedded in noise. SDT was used on the accuracy data to calculate a measure of perceptual sensitivity (Az) and a measure of response bias (beta). Thresholds for the perception of simple tones were determined. RESULTS: Compared to healthy controls, perceptual thresholds were higher and perceptual sensitivity in the speech task was lower in both patient groups. However, hallucinating patients showed increased sensitivity to speech stimuli compared to non-hallucinating patients. In addition, we found some evidence of a positive response bias in hallucinating patients, indicating a tendency to readily accept that a certain stimulus had been presented. CONCLUSIONS: Within the context of schizophrenia, patients with auditory hallucinations show enhanced sensitivity to speech stimuli, combined with a liberal criterion for deciding that a perceived event is an actual stimulus.


Subject(s)
Hallucinations/diagnosis , Noise , Speech Perception , Adult , Female , Hallucinations/epidemiology , Hallucinations/psychology , Humans , Male , Prevalence , Psychological Theory , Schizophrenia/epidemiology , Severity of Illness Index , Signal Detection, Psychological
9.
Eur Psychiatry ; 23(1): 26-8, 2008 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17937980

ABSTRACT

The study examined the perception of facial expressions of different emotional intensities in obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) subtypes. Results showed that the High Risk Assessment and Checking subtype was more sensitive in perceiving the emotions fear and happiness. This suggests that altered affective processing may underlie the clinical manifestation of OCD.


Subject(s)
Emotions , Facial Expression , Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder/diagnosis , Visual Perception , Adult , Fear/psychology , Female , Happiness , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Models, Psychological , Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder/classification , Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder/psychology , Social Perception
10.
Cerebrovasc Dis ; 23(5-6): 408-16, 2007.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17406110

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Although cognitive impairment early after stroke is a powerful predictor of long-term functional dependence and dementia, little is known about the characteristics and determinants of cognitive dysfunction in acute stroke. METHODS: We administered a neuropsychological examination covering 7 cognitive domains to 190 patients within 3 weeks after a first stroke. We also assembled lesion characteristics, clinical factors at admission, demographic characteristics and vascular risk factors. Multivariate logistic regression adjusted for age, gender and education was performed to examine determinants of acute cognitive impairment. RESULTS: Overall, 74% of patients with a cortical stroke, 46% with a subcortical stroke and 43% with an infratentorial stroke demonstrated acute cognitive impairment. Disorders in executive functioning (39%) and visual perception/construction (38%) were the most common. The prevalence and severity of deficits in executive functioning, language, verbal memory and abstract reasoning was more pronounced following left compared to right cortical stroke (all p < 0.05). Intracerebral haemorrhage (OR = 5.6; 95% CI = 1.2-25.4) and cortical involvement of the stroke (OR = 3.6; 95%, CI = 1.3-9.9) were independent determinants of acute cognitive impairment, whereas premorbid moderate alcohol consumption exerted a protective effect (OR = 0.4; 95% CI = 0.1-1.1). CONCLUSIONS: Cognitive impairment is common in the first weeks after stroke, with executive and perceptual disorders being the most frequent. Intracerebral haemorrhage, cortical involvement of the lesion and premorbid moderate alcohol consumption are independently associated with acute cognitive impairment.


Subject(s)
Cognition Disorders/etiology , Stroke/complications , Acute Disease , Aged , Alcohol Drinking/epidemiology , Case-Control Studies , Cerebral Cortex/pathology , Cerebral Hemorrhage/complications , Cognition Disorders/epidemiology , Cognition Disorders/pathology , Female , Humans , Logistic Models , Male , Middle Aged , Netherlands/epidemiology , Neuropsychological Tests , Odds Ratio , Prevalence , Risk Assessment , Risk Factors , Severity of Illness Index , Stroke/epidemiology , Stroke/pathology , Stroke/psychology
11.
Timely Top Med Cardiovasc Dis ; 11: E7, 2007 Feb 23.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17380217

ABSTRACT

Type 2 diabetes is a common metabolic disease with a rising global prevalence. It is associated with slowly progressive end-organ damage in the eyes and kidneys, but also in the brain. The latter complication is often referred to as "diabetic encephalopathy" and is characterized by mild to moderate impairments in cognitive functioning. It is also associated with an increased risk of dementia. To date, its pathogenetic mechanisms are largely unclear. Cognitive impairments in patients with type 2 diabetes have been associated both with vascular risk factors, such as hypertension and dyslipidemia, and with diabetes-related factors, such as glycemic control, duration of the disease and treatment modality. Studies that address these associations generally focus on statistical (in)dependence of certain risk factors in the association between type 2 diabetes and cognitive decline rather than the causality of the association, which, from a mechanistic point of view, is more relevant. In this review we describe the association between type 2 diabetes and cognitive dysfunction and dementia. Furthermore, potential determinants of impaired cognition in type 2 diabetes are addressed both from the perspective of statistical associations and from a mechanistic point of view.

12.
Neuropsychology ; 21(1): 65-73, 2007 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17201530

ABSTRACT

The authors assessed visual information processing in high-functioning individuals with pervasive developmental disorders (PDD) and their parents. The authors used tasks for contrast sensitivity, motion, and form perception to test visual processing occurring relatively early and late in the magnocellular-dorsal and parvocellular-ventral pathways. No deficits were found in contrast sensitivity for low or high spatial frequencies or for motion or form perception between individuals with PDD in comparison with a matched control group. Individuals with PDD performed equally with or better than controls on motion detection tasks. In addition, the authors did not find differences on any of the tasks between parents of the PDD group and matched control parents. These results indicate that high-functioning individuals with PDD and their parents are able to process visual stimuli that rely on early or late processing in the magnocellular-dorsal and parvocellular-ventral pathways as well as controls.


Subject(s)
Child Development Disorders, Pervasive/physiopathology , Child Development Disorders, Pervasive/psychology , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Parent-Child Relations , Visual Pathways/physiopathology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Child , Female , Humans , Intelligence , Male , Photic Stimulation/methods , Sensory Thresholds/physiology
13.
Drugs Today (Barc) ; 42(11): 741-54, 2006 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17171193

ABSTRACT

Type 2 diabetes is a common metabolic disease with a rising global prevalence. It is associated with slowly progressive end-organ damage in the eyes and kidneys, but also in the brain. The latter complication is often referred to as "diabetic encephalopathy" and is characterized by mild to moderate impairments in cognitive functioning. It is also associated with an increased risk of dementia. To date, its pathogenetic mechanisms are largely unclear. Cognitive impairments in patients with type 2 diabetes have been associated both with vascular risk factors, such as hypertension and dyslipidemia, and with diabetes-related factors, such as glycemic control, duration of the disease and treatment modality. Studies that address these associations generally focus on statistical (in)dependence of certain risk factors in the association between type 2 diabetes and cognitive decline rather than the causality of the association, which, from a mechanistic point of view, is more relevant. In this review we describe the association between type 2 diabetes and cognitive dysfunction and dementia. Furthermore, potential determinants of impaired cognition in type 2 diabetes are addressed both from the perspective of statistical associations and from a mechanistic point of view.


Subject(s)
Cognition Disorders/etiology , Dementia/etiology , Diabetes Mellitus, Type 2/complications , Brain/pathology , Cognition Disorders/pathology , Dementia/pathology , Diabetes Mellitus, Type 2/etiology , Diabetes Mellitus, Type 2/pathology , Dyslipidemias/complications , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Obesity/complications , Vascular Diseases/complications
14.
Brain ; 129(Pt 8): 2148-57, 2006 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16870885

ABSTRACT

Patients with left-sided neglect frequently show repetitive behaviour on the ipsilesional side, such as re-markings on cancellation tasks or extensive elaboration on drawings. It is unclear whether these perseverative responses occur as a symptom of hemi-neglect or inattention in general, and/or whether they are related to anatomical brain correlates such as lesion location, lesion side or volume. In a first study, we examined the prevalence and neuropsychological correlates of perseverative responses in 206 subacute stroke patients and 63 healthy controls. Perseverative responses were considered present when there was at least one re-marking on the Star Cancellation, and both the degree and spatial distribution of re-markings were examined. A distinction was made between hemi-neglect and non-lateralized inattention. Spatial and verbal working memory were assessed with the Corsi Block Span and the Digit Span. Verbal and non-verbal executive function was assessed with the Visual Elevator and Letter Fluency. Stroke patients without inattention demonstrated re-markings that were related to executive performance, and the degree of perseveration was equally distributed across the sheet. Hemi-neglect patients but not patients with generalized inattention demonstrated more re-markings than controls, suggesting that a lateralized spatial attention bias triggers the perseverative responses. Patients with left and right hemi-neglect showed the same prevalence of perseveration, but the distribution of re-markings was more lateralized towards the ipsilesional side in patients with right-hemispheric stroke. The degree of perseveration in patients with hemi-neglect was related to the severity of the neglect. The goal of the second study on a subset of patients (n = 127) was to determine the neuroanatomical correlates of perseverative responses in the early phase of stroke. Lesion anatomy was administered by indicating involvement of frontal, parietal, temporal, occipital lobe, caudate nucleus, lenticular nucleus and/or thalamus. Lesion volume was calculated using a manual tracing technique. Lesion analyses indicated that perseverative behaviour is strongly associated with lesions involving the caudate nucleus or the lenticular nucleus, independent of lesion volume. The caudate nucleus was an important correlate of perseveration independent of the presence of hemi-neglect. No association was found between lesion side and perseverative responses, in contrast to previous studies. In conclusion, a stroke involving the basal ganglia and the presence of (left- or right-sided) hemi-neglect are two important associates of perseverative responses in the subacute phase of stroke.


Subject(s)
Perceptual Disorders/etiology , Stroke/psychology , Acute Disease , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Attention , Brain/pathology , Brain Mapping , Caudate Nucleus/pathology , Female , Humans , Male , Memory, Short-Term , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Perceptual Disorders/diagnosis , Perceptual Disorders/pathology , Psychomotor Performance , Stroke/pathology , Stroke/physiopathology
15.
J Neurol Sci ; 247(2): 149-56, 2006 Sep 25.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16716359

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: The aim of the present study was to examine the predictive value of cognitive impairment in the acute phase after stroke as a risk factor for long-term (six to ten months after stroke) depressive symptoms (DS) and a reduced quality of life (QOL), independent of demographic and neurological predictors. METHODS: We evaluated 143 patients within the first 3 weeks post-stroke. Predictor variables included domain-specific cognitive function, demographic data, vascular risk factors, lesion characteristics, and clinical factors. Predictor variables associated with long-term DS (Montgomery Asberg Depression Rating Scale >or=7) and QOL (Stroke-Specific Quality of Life Scale) were identified with multiple logistic and linear regression. RESULTS: Long-term DS were independently predicted by cognitive impairment at baseline, DS at baseline, female sex, diabetes mellitus, and previous TIA(s). Cognitive impairment, increasing age, and functional dependence predicted a reduced QOL, whereas hypercholesterolaemia predicted a better QOL. Among all cognitive disorders, unilateral neglect was the greatest risk factor for DS after 6 months, whereas a disorder in visual perception and construction affected QOL the most. CONCLUSIONS: Cognitive impairment and vascular risk factors are important predictors of long-term DS and QOL after stroke. The prognostic value of cognition suggests a reactive component in the development or continuation of long-term DS.


Subject(s)
Cognition Disorders/etiology , Depression/diagnosis , Depression/etiology , Quality of Life , Stroke/physiopathology , Stroke/psychology , Aged , Demography , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests/statistics & numerical data , Predictive Value of Tests , Risk Factors , Sex Factors , Stroke/complications , Vascular Diseases/complications
16.
Neuropsychologia ; 44(13): 2766-73, 2006.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16321407

ABSTRACT

When reaching towards a visual stimulus, spatial information about the target must be transformed into an appropriate motor command. Visual information is coded initially in retinotopic coordinates, while the reaching movement ultimately requires the specification of the target position in limb-centred coordinates. It is well established that the posterior parietal cortex (PPC) plays an important role in transforming visual target information into motor commands. Lesions in the PPC can result in optic ataxia, a condition in which the visual guidance of goal-directed movements is impaired. Here, we present evidence from two patients with unilateral optic ataxia following right PPC lesions, that the pattern of reaching errors is linked to an eye-centred frame of reference. Both patients made large errors when reaching to visual targets on the left side of space, while facing and fixating straight ahead. By varying the location of fixation and the orientation of the head and body, we were able to establish that these large errors were made specifically to targets to the left of eye-fixation, rather than to the left of head-, body-, or limb-relative space. These data support the idea that visual targets for reaching movements are coded in eye-centred coordinates within the posterior parietal cortex.


Subject(s)
Ataxia/physiopathology , Movement/physiology , Posture , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Aged, 80 and over , Analysis of Variance , Ataxia/pathology , Eye , Female , Functional Laterality , Hand , Head , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Male , Middle Aged , Parietal Lobe/physiopathology , Photic Stimulation
17.
J Neurol ; 253(2): 237-41, 2006 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16142581

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: To examine whether intravenous recombinant tissue plasminogen activator (rt-PA) treatment given in the acute phase of ischaemic stroke has a favourable effect on cognitive and functional outcome at six months post-stroke. METHODS: The present study included 92 patients with a first-ever symptomatic infarct, of whom 25 (27%) were subjected to rt-PA treatment in the first three hours post-stroke. Multivariate logistic regression analyses adjusted for stroke severity, education, age, and sex were performed to examine whether rt-PA treatment influenced cognitive outcome (assessed with a neuropsychological examination covering 7 cognitive domains), basic ADL independence (modified Barthel Index > or = 19), and instrumental ADL independence (Frenchay Activities Index > or = 15) after six months. RESULTS: The adjusted odds ratio for intact cognition was 1.0 (95% CI 0.2 to 4.3), that for basic ADL outcome 13.5 (95 % CI 1.4 to 129.4) and for instrumental ADL 7.1 (95 % CI 1.2 to 42.2). CONCLUSION: Our findings suggest that rt-PA treatment is associated with a favourable basic and instrumental ADL outcome, but not with a beneficial cognitive outcome after 6 months.


Subject(s)
Brain Infarction/drug therapy , Cognition/drug effects , Fibrinolytic Agents/therapeutic use , Recovery of Function/drug effects , Tissue Plasminogen Activator/therapeutic use , Activities of Daily Living , Aged , Brain Infarction/physiopathology , Confidence Intervals , Female , Follow-Up Studies , Humans , Injections, Intravenous/methods , Logistic Models , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests/statistics & numerical data , Odds Ratio , Recombinant Proteins/therapeutic use , Time Factors , Treatment Outcome
19.
Clin Neurol Neurosurg ; 107(5): 385-92, 2005 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16023532

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVES: This study describes the feasibility and validity of neuropsychological evaluation in the early stage post-stroke. Early information on cognitive functioning in stroke patients could improve discharge decision, programming of rehabilitation strategies, and better prepare proxies for the problems they can be presented with in daily life. In this explorative study, our primary focus was on the feasibility of early neuropsychological evaluation. Furthermore, we looked at the possible prognostic relevance of early examination. PATIENTS AND METHODS: Fifty-seven consecutive patients (age 19-80) were enrolled within 4-20 days after their first ischaemic stroke (Modified-Rankin Scale (M-RS): 2-4). Patients were re-tested after 12-24 months, and functional outcome was assessed. RESULTS: In the early stage 44 (77%), patients could complete 82% of the administered tasks. At second evaluation, test performances improved, but a stable test profile was found with respect to abnormalities on the different tasks (P<0.0001). Moreover, initial sum scores of all composite cognitive domains including intellectual functioning (R2=0.80), language (R2=0.76), memory (R2=0.32), perception and visuospatial construction (R2=0.60), attention and psychomotor-functioning (R2=0.80) had significant predictive validity with respect to functional outcome (P<0.001). CONCLUSION: This study supports the feasibility of early neuropsychological evaluation after ischaemic stroke onset and the prognostic validity for cognitive outcome in the long term.


Subject(s)
Brain Ischemia/complications , Cognition Disorders/diagnosis , Cognition Disorders/etiology , Stroke/etiology , Stroke/psychology , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Brain Ischemia/psychology , Early Diagnosis , Feasibility Studies , Female , Follow-Up Studies , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Predictive Value of Tests , Prognosis , Reproducibility of Results , Severity of Illness Index , Time Factors
20.
J Physiol ; 567(Pt 2): 665-71, 2005 Sep 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15975983

ABSTRACT

The spatial differences between the two retinal images, called binocular disparities, can be used to recover the three-dimensional (3D) aspects of a scene. The computation of disparity depends upon the correct identification of corresponding features in the two images. Understanding what image features are used by the brain to solve this binocular matching problem is an important issue in research on stereoscopic vision. The role of colour in binocular vision is controversial and it has been argued that colour is ineffective in achieving binocular vision. In the current experiment subjects were required to indicate the amount of perceived depth. The stimulus consisted of an array of fronto-parallel bars uniformly distributed in a constant sized volume. We studied the perceived depth in those 3D stimuli by manipulating both colour (monochrome, trichrome) and luminance (congruent, incongruent). Our results demonstrate that the amount of perceived depth was influenced by colour, indicating that the visual system uses colour to achieve binocular matching. Physiological data have revealed cortical cells in macaque V2 that are tuned both to binocular disparity and to colour. We suggest that one of the functional roles of these cells may be to help solve the binocular matching problem.


Subject(s)
Color Perception/physiology , Color , Depth Perception/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Photic Stimulation/methods , Vision, Binocular/physiology
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...