Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 239
Filter
1.
Neurogenetics ; 24(1): 29-41, 2023 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36449109

ABSTRACT

Feather pecking (FP) is a damaging nonaggressive behavior in laying hens with a heritable component. Its occurrence has been linked to the immune system, the circadian clock, and foraging behavior. Furthermore, dysregulation of miRNA biogenesis, disturbance of the gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABAergic) system, as well as neurodevelopmental deficiencies are currently under debate as factors influencing the propensity for FP behavior. Past studies, which focused on the dissection of the genetic factors involved in FP, relied on single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) and short insertions and deletions < 50 bp (InDels). These variant classes only represent a certain fraction of the genetic variation of an organism. Hence, we reanalyzed whole-genome sequencing data from two experimental populations, which have been divergently selected for FP behavior for over more than 15 generations, performed variant calling for structural variants (SVs) as well as tandem repeats (TRs), and jointly analyzed the data with SNPs and InDels. Genotype imputation and subsequent genome-wide association studies, in combination with expression quantitative trait loci analysis, led to the discovery of multiple variants influencing the GABAergic system. These include a significantly associated TR downstream of the GABA receptor subunit beta-3 (GABRB3) gene, two microRNAs targeting several GABA receptor genes, and dystrophin (DMD), a direct regulator of GABA receptor clustering. Furthermore, we found the transcription factor ETV1 to be associated with the differential expression of 23 genes, which points toward a role of ETV1, together with SMAD4 and KLF14, in the disturbed neurodevelopment of high-feather pecking chickens.


Subject(s)
Behavior, Animal , Chickens , Animals , Female , Behavior, Animal/physiology , Chickens/genetics , Feathers , Genome-Wide Association Study , Genotype , Genetics, Behavioral/methods
2.
Sci Rep ; 12(1): 3104, 2022 02 24.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35210468

ABSTRACT

Down syndrome (DS) is characterised by several clinical features including intellectual disability (ID) and craniofacial dysmorphisms. In 1976, Jackson and coll. identified a checklist of signs for clinical diagnosis of DS; the utility of these checklists in improving the accuracy of clinical diagnosis has been recently reaffirmed, but they have rarely been revised. The purpose of this work is to reassess the characteristic phenotypic signs and their frequencies in 233 DS subjects, following Jackson's checklist. 63.77% of the subjects showed more than 12 signs while none showed less than 5, confirming the effectiveness of Jackson's checklist for the clinical diagnosis of DS. An association between three phenotypic signs emerged, allowing us to distinguish two sub-phenotypes: Brachycephaly, short and broad Hands, short Neck (BHN), which is more frequent, and "non-BHN". The strong association of these signs might be interpreted in the context of the growth defects observed in DS children suggesting decreased cell proliferation. Lastly, cognitive assessments were investigated for 114 subjects. The lack of association between the presence of a physical sign or the number of signs present in a subject and cognitive skills disproves the stereotype that physical characteristics are predictive of degree of ID.


Subject(s)
Down Syndrome/diagnosis , Checklist , Down Syndrome/physiopathology , Genetics, Behavioral/methods , Humans , Intellectual Disability/diagnosis , Intellectual Disability/genetics , Neurology/methods , Phenotype
3.
Behav Genet ; 51(3): 319-330, 2021 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33638732

ABSTRACT

The classical twin model can be reparametrized as an equivalent multilevel model. The multilevel parameterization has underexplored advantages, such as the possibility to include higher-level clustering variables in which lower levels are nested. When this higher-level clustering is not modeled, its variance is captured by the common environmental variance component. In this paper we illustrate the application of a 3-level multilevel model to twin data by analyzing the regional clustering of 7-year-old children's height in the Netherlands. Our findings show that 1.8%, of the phenotypic variance in children's height is attributable to regional clustering, which is 7% of the variance explained by between-family or common environmental components. Since regional clustering may represent ancestry, we also investigate the effect of region after correcting for genetic principal components, in a subsample of participants with genome-wide SNP data. After correction, region no longer explained variation in height. Our results suggest that the phenotypic variance explained by region might represent ancestry effects on height.


Subject(s)
Body Height/genetics , Multilevel Analysis/methods , Statistics as Topic/methods , Child , Cluster Analysis , Female , Genetics, Behavioral/methods , Genetics, Behavioral/trends , Genome-Wide Association Study/methods , Genotype , Humans , Male , Models, Genetic , Netherlands , Phenotype , Polymorphism, Single Nucleotide/genetics , Twins/genetics
4.
Behav Genet ; 51(3): 170-180, 2021 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33585974

ABSTRACT

Since 1987, a group of behavior geneticists have been teaching an annual methodology workshop on how to use state-of-the-art methods to analyze genetically informative data. In the early years, the focus was on analyzing twin and family data, using information of their known genetic relatedness to infer the role of genetic and environmental factors on phenotypic variation. With the rapid evolution of genotyping and sequencing technology and availability of measured genetic data, new methods to detect genetic variants associated with human traits were developed and became the focus of workshop teaching in alternate years. Over the years, many of the methodological advances in the field of statistical genetics have been direct outgrowths of the workshop, as evidence by the software and methodological publications authored by workshop faculty. We provide data and demographics of workshop attendees and evaluate the impact of the methodology workshops on scientific output in the field by evaluating the number of papers applying specific statistical genetic methodologies authored by individuals who have attended workshops.


Subject(s)
Education/trends , Genetics, Behavioral/methods , Genetics, Behavioral/trends , Humans , Research/trends , Software
5.
Behav Genet ; 51(3): 301-318, 2021 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33609197

ABSTRACT

For more than a decade, it has been known that many common behavior genetics models for a single phenotype can be estimated as multilevel models (e.g., van den Oord 2001; Guo and Wang 2002; McArdle and Prescott 2005; Rabe-Hesketh et al. 2007). This paper extends the current knowledge to (1) multiple phenotypes such that the method is completely general to the variance structure hypothesized, and (2) both higher and lower levels of nesting. The multi-phenotype method also allows extended relationships to be considered (see also, Bard et al. 2012; Hadfield and Nakagawa 2010). The extended relationship model can then be continuously expanded to merge with the case typically seen in the molecular genetics analyses of unrelated individuals (e.g., Yang et al. 2011). We use the multilevel form of behavior genetics models to fit a multivariate three level model that allows for (1) child level variation from unique environments and additive genetics, (2) family level variation from additive genetics and common environments, and (3) neighborhood level variation from broader geographic contexts. Finally, we provide R (R Development Core Team 2020) functions and code for multilevel specification of several common behavior genetics models using OpenMx (Neale et al. 2016).


Subject(s)
Genetics, Behavioral/methods , Multilevel Analysis/methods , Statistics as Topic/methods , Environment , Gene-Environment Interaction , Genetics, Behavioral/trends , Genotype , Humans , Models, Genetic , Models, Theoretical , Phenotype , Software , Twins/genetics
6.
Behav Genet ; 51(3): 181-190, 2021 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32989645

ABSTRACT

The International Statistical Genetics Workshop (commonly referred to as the "Boulder Workshop") has been held annually in Boulder, Colorado almost every year since 1990. A staple feature of each workshop has been the presence of a "question box" (either a physical box or an online virtual one) where workshop participants are given the opportunity of asking questions to the faculty. In this manuscript, we have compiled a list of ten "classic" questions that have appeared in one form or another across multiple workshops and our attempts at answering them.


Subject(s)
Education/trends , Genetics, Behavioral/methods , Genetics, Behavioral/trends , Humans , Models, Genetic , Models, Theoretical
7.
Public Health Genomics ; 23(5-6): 171-183, 2020.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33147585

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Psychiatric genetics has had limited success in translational efforts. A thorough understanding of the present state of translation in this field will be useful in the facilitation and assessment of future translational progress. PURPOSE: A narrative literature review was conducted. Combinations of 3 groups of terms were searched in EBSCOhost, Google Scholar, and PubMed. The review occurred in multiple steps, including abstract collection, inclusion/exclusion criteria review, coding, and analysis of included papers. RESULTS: One hundred and fourteen articles were analyzed for the narrative review. Across those, 4 bottlenecks were noted that, if addressed, may provide insights and help improve and increase translation in the field of psychiatric genetics. These 4 bottlenecks are emphasizing linear translational frameworks, relying on molecular genomic findings, prioritizing certain psychiatric disorders, and publishing more reviews than experiments. CONCLUSIONS: These entwined bottlenecks are examined with one another. Awareness of these bottlenecks can inform stakeholders who work to translate and/or utilize psychiatric genetic information. Potential solutions include utilizing nonlinear translational frameworks as well as a wider array of psychiatric genetic information (e.g., family history and gene-environment interplay) in this area of research, expanding which psychiatric disorders are considered for translation, and when possible, conducting original research. Researchers are urged to consider how their research is translational in the context of the frameworks, genetic information, and psychiatric disorders discussed in this review. At a broader level, these efforts should be supported with translational efforts in funding and policy shifts.


Subject(s)
Genetics, Behavioral/methods , Mental Disorders/genetics , Translational Research, Biomedical/methods , Gene-Environment Interaction , Humans , Molecular Biology , Precision Medicine/methods , Research Design
8.
Genetics ; 216(2): 315-332, 2020 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33023930

ABSTRACT

Caenorhabditis elegans' behavioral states, like those of other animals, are shaped by its immediate environment, its past experiences, and by internal factors. We here review the literature on C. elegans behavioral states and their regulation. We discuss dwelling and roaming, local and global search, mate finding, sleep, and the interaction between internal metabolic states and behavior.


Subject(s)
Behavior, Animal , Caenorhabditis elegans/genetics , Animals , Caenorhabditis elegans/metabolism , Caenorhabditis elegans/physiology , Energy Metabolism , Genetics, Behavioral/methods , Sleep
9.
J Neurogenet ; 34(3-4): 453-465, 2020.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32811254

ABSTRACT

Following prolonged swimming, Caenorhabditis elegans cycle between active swimming bouts and inactive quiescent bouts. Swimming is exercise for C. elegans and here we suggest that inactive bouts are a recovery state akin to fatigue. It is known that cGMP-dependent kinase (PKG) activity plays a conserved role in sleep, rest, and arousal. Using C. elegans EGL-4 PKG, we first validate a novel learning-based computer vision approach to automatically analyze C. elegans locomotory behavior and an edge detection program that is able to distinguish between activity and inactivity during swimming for long periods of time. We find that C. elegans EGL-4 PKG function impacts timing of exercise-induced quiescent (EIQ) bout onset, fractional quiescence, bout number, and bout duration, suggesting that previously described pathways are engaged during EIQ bouts. However, EIQ bouts are likely not sleep as animals are feeding during the majority of EIQ bouts. We find that genetic perturbation of neurons required for other C. elegans sleep states also does not alter EIQ dynamics. Additionally, we find that EIQ onset is sensitive to age and DAF-16 FOXO function. In summary, we have validated behavioral analysis software that enables a quantitative and detailed assessment of swimming behavior, including EIQ. We found novel EIQ defects in aged animals and animals with mutations in a gene involved in stress tolerance. We anticipate that further use of this software will facilitate the analysis of genes and pathways critical for fatigue and other C. elegans behaviors.


Subject(s)
Artificial Intelligence , Caenorhabditis elegans/physiology , Fatigue/etiology , Genetics, Behavioral/methods , Physical Exertion/physiology , Sleep/physiology , Swimming/physiology , Aging/physiology , Animals , Biomechanical Phenomena , Caenorhabditis elegans/genetics , Caenorhabditis elegans Proteins/genetics , Caenorhabditis elegans Proteins/physiology , Cyclic GMP-Dependent Protein Kinases/genetics , Cyclic GMP-Dependent Protein Kinases/physiology , Escherichia coli , Lab-On-A-Chip Devices , Movement , Pharynx/physiology , Rest , Sleep/genetics
10.
Behav Genet ; 50(2): 127-138, 2020 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32040643

ABSTRACT

The univariate bootstrap is a relatively recently developed version of the bootstrap (Lee and Rodgers in Psychol Methods 3(1): 91, 1998). DeFries-Fulker (DF) analysis is a regression model used to estimate parameters in behavioral genetic models (DeFries and Fulker in Behav Genet 15(5): 467-473, 1985). It is appealing for its simplicity; however, it violates certain regression assumptions such as homogeneity of variance and independence of errors that make calculation of standard errors and confidence intervals problematic. Methods have been developed to account for these issues (Kohler and Rodgers in Behav Genet 31(2): 179-191, 2001), however the univariate bootstrap represents a unique means of doing so that is presaged by suggestions from previous DF research (e.g., Cherny et al. in Behav Genet 22(2): 153-162, 1992). In the present study we use simulations to examine the performance of the univariate bootstrap in the context of DF analysis. We compare a number of possible bootstrap schemes as well as more traditional confidence interval methods. We follow up with an empirical demonstration, applying results of the simulation to models estimated to investigate changes in body mass index in adults from the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth 1979 data.


Subject(s)
Data Interpretation, Statistical , Genetics, Behavioral/methods , Models, Statistical , Adolescent , Adult , Body Mass Index , Body Weight/genetics , Confidence Intervals , Female , Humans , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Models, Genetic , Phenotype , Regression Analysis , Social Environment , Twins, Dizygotic/genetics , Twins, Monozygotic/genetics , Young Adult
11.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31932526

ABSTRACT

Forward genetics is a powerful approach to understand the molecular basis of animal behaviors. Fruit flies were the first animal to which this genetic approach was applied systematically and have provided major discoveries on behaviors including sexual, learning, circadian, and sleep-like behaviors. The development of different classes of model organism such as nematodes, zebrafish, and mice has enabled genetic research to be conducted using more-suitable organisms. The unprecedented success of forward genetic approaches was the identification of the transcription-translation negative feedback loop composed of clock genes as a fundamental and conserved mechanism of circadian rhythm. This approach has now expanded to sleep/wakefulness in mice. A conventional strategy such as dominant and recessive screenings can be modified with advances in DNA sequencing and genome editing technologies.


Subject(s)
Behavior, Animal , Genetics, Behavioral/methods , Neurosciences/methods , Animals , Caenorhabditis elegans , Circadian Clocks/genetics , Circadian Rhythm/genetics , Drosophila , Learning , Mice , Models, Animal , Mutagenesis , Sleep/genetics , Zebrafish
12.
J Neurogenet ; 34(2): 203-215, 2020.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31902276

ABSTRACT

Due to its fully sequenced genome, high genetic homology to humans, external fertilization, fast development, transparency of embryos, low cost and active reproduction, the zebrafish (Danio rerio) has become a novel promising model organism in biomedicine. Zebrafish are a useful tool in genetic and neuroscience research, including linking various genetic mutations to brain mechanisms using forward and reverse genetics. These approaches have produced novel models of rare genetic CNS disorders and common brain illnesses, such as addiction, aggression, anxiety and depression. Genetically modified zebrafish also foster neuroanatomical studies, manipulating neural circuits and linking them to different behaviors. Here, we discuss recent advances in neurogenetics of zebrafish, and evaluate their unique strengths, inherent limitations and the rapidly growing potential for elucidating the conserved roles of genes in neuropsychiatric disorders.


Subject(s)
Genetics, Behavioral/methods , Neurosciences/methods , Zebrafish/genetics , Animals , Animals, Genetically Modified , Behavior, Animal , CRISPR-Cas Systems , Cell Lineage , Central Nervous System Diseases/genetics , Gene Editing/methods , Gene Targeting/methods , Models, Animal , Quantitative Trait Loci , RNA, Bacterial , RNA, Small Interfering/genetics , Reverse Genetics/methods , Species Specificity , Zebrafish/classification , Zebrafish Proteins/biosynthesis , Zebrafish Proteins/genetics
13.
Genes Brain Behav ; 19(2): e12598, 2020 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31286644

ABSTRACT

The field of behavioral genetics has recently begun to explore the effect of age on social behaviors. Such studies are particularly important, as certain neuropsychiatric disorders with abnormal social interactions, like autism and schizophrenia, have been linked to older parents. Appropriate social interaction can also have a positive impact on longevity, and is associated with successful aging in humans. Currently, there are few genetic models for understanding the effect of aging on social behavior and its potential transgenerational inheritance. The fly is emerging as a powerful model for identifying the basic molecular mechanisms underlying neurological and neuropsychiatric disorders. In this review, we discuss these recent advancements, with a focus on how studies in Drosophila melanogaster have provided insight into the effect of aging on aspects of social behavior, including across generations.


Subject(s)
Aging/physiology , Aging/psychology , Animals , Behavior, Animal/physiology , Courtship/psychology , Drosophila melanogaster/genetics , Female , Genetics, Behavioral/methods , Interpersonal Relations , Male , Models, Animal , Sexual Behavior, Animal/physiology , Social Behavior
14.
Stud Hist Philos Sci ; 77: 39-53, 2019 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31701880

ABSTRACT

The article introduces a framework for analyzing the knowledge that researchers draw upon when designing a research project by distinguishing four types of "project knowledge": goal knowledge, which concerns possible outcomes, and three forms of implementation knowledge that concern the realization of the project: 1) methodological knowledge that specifies possible experimental and non-experimental strategies to achieve the chosen goal; 2) representational knowledge that suggests ways to represent data, hypotheses, or outcomes; and 3) organizational knowledge that helps to build or navigate the material and social structures that enable a project. In the design of research projects such knowledge will be transferred from other successful projects and these processes will be analyzed in terms of modes of resituating knowledge. The account is developed by analyzing a case from the history of biology. In a reciprocal manner, it enables a better understanding of the historical episode in question: around 1970, several researchers who had made successful careers in the emerging field of molecular biology, working with bacterial model systems, attempted to create a molecular biology of the physiological processes in multicellular organisms. One of them was Seymour Benzer, who designed a research project addressing the physiological processes underlying behavior in Drosophila.


Subject(s)
Genetics, Behavioral/history , Research Design , Animals , Behavior, Animal , Drosophila melanogaster/genetics , Genetics, Behavioral/methods , History, 20th Century
15.
J Vis Exp ; (151)2019 09 17.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31609333

ABSTRACT

The Cholesky decomposition method is the gold standard used in the field of behavioral genetics. The method is popular because it is easy to program and solve. Using this method, researchers can explore individual differences in longitudinal relations of different variables across multiple time points. The method allows investigators to decompose variance into (1) unique genetic, shared and non-shared environmental effects that arise at specific time points as well as (2) overlapping genetic, shared and non-shared environmental effects that carry over from one time point to another. However, the method does not identify the mechanisms or origins underlying these effects. The current report focuses on application of the Cholesky decomposition method in the field of educational psychology. Specifically, it discusses individual differences in longitudinal relations between kindergarten letter knowledge, kindergarten phonological awareness, first grade word-level reading skills, and seventh grade reading comprehension.


Subject(s)
Genetics, Behavioral/methods , Individuality , Reading , Child , Female , Humans , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Phonetics
16.
Methods Mol Biol ; 2018: 319-326, 2019.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31228165

ABSTRACT

In this chapter, we briefly review the use of rats as a genetic model for the study of behavior. Rats were the first mammalian species used for genetic and biological research. Since the development of the first inbred rat strain in 1909, more than 700 unique inbred and outbred rat lines have been generated. Although rats have been somewhat eclipsed by mice in the last few decades, a renewed appreciation of the advantages of rats for behavioral and other types of research is upon us. We briefly review the pertinent characteristics of the rat and highlight the key advantages of using the rat to examine behavioral phenotypes.


Subject(s)
Behavior/physiology , Genetics, Behavioral/methods , Animals , Animals, Outbred Strains , Rats , Rats, Inbred Strains
17.
Sci Rep ; 9(1): 7554, 2019 05 17.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31101867

ABSTRACT

Dogs were the first domesticated animal and, according to the archaeological evidence, have had a close relationship with humans for at least 15,000 years. Today, dogs are common pets in our society and have been linked to increased well-being and improved health outcomes in their owners. A dog in the family during childhood is associated with ownership in adult life. The underlying factors behind this association could be related to experiences or to genetic influences. We aimed to investigate the heritability of dog ownership in a large twin sample including all twins in the Swedish Twin Registry born between 1926 and 1996 and alive in 2006. Information about dog ownership was available from 2001 to 2016 from national dog registers. The final data set included 85,542 twins from 50,507 twin pairs with known zygosity, where information on both twins were available in 35,035 pairs. Structural equation modeling was performed to estimate additive genetic effects (the heritability), common/shared environmental, and unique/non-shared environmental effects. We found that additive genetic factors largely contributed to dog ownership, with heritability estimated at 57% for females and 51% for males. An effect of shared environmental factors was only observed in early adulthood. In conclusion, we show a strong genetic contribution to dog ownership in adulthood in a large twin study. We see two main implications of this finding: (1) genetic variation may have contributed to our ability to domesticate dogs and other animals and (2) potential pleiotropic effects of genetic variation affecting dog ownership should be considered in studies examining health impacts of dog ownership.


Subject(s)
Behavior/physiology , Pets , Twins/genetics , Animals , Dogs , Domestication , Female , Genetic Variation/genetics , Genetics, Behavioral/methods , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Models, Genetic , Sweden
18.
BMC Biol ; 17(1): 30, 2019 04 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30967153

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Epigenetic mechanisms play fundamental roles in brain function and behavior and stressors such as social isolation can alter animal behavior via epigenetic mechanisms. However, due to cellular heterogeneity, identifying cell-type-specific epigenetic changes in the brain is challenging. Here, we report the first use of a modified isolation of nuclei tagged in specific cell type (INTACT) method in behavioral epigenetics of Drosophila melanogaster, a method we call mini-INTACT. RESULTS: Using ChIP-seq on mini-INTACT purified dopaminergic nuclei, we identified epigenetic signatures in socially isolated and socially enriched Drosophila males. Social experience altered the epigenetic landscape in clusters of genes involved in transcription and neural function. Some of these alterations could be predicted by expression changes of four transcription factors and the prevalence of their binding sites in several clusters. These transcription factors were previously identified as activity-regulated genes, and their knockdown in dopaminergic neurons reduced the effects of social experience on sleep. CONCLUSIONS: Our work enables the use of Drosophila as a model for cell-type-specific behavioral epigenetics and establishes that social environment shifts the epigenetic landscape in dopaminergic neurons. Four activity-related transcription factors are required in dopaminergic neurons for the effects of social environment on sleep.


Subject(s)
Dopaminergic Neurons/physiology , Drosophila melanogaster/genetics , Epigenesis, Genetic/genetics , Genetics, Behavioral/methods , Social Environment , Animals , Drosophila Proteins/genetics , Drosophila Proteins/metabolism , Epigenomics/methods , Male , Models, Animal , Sleep/genetics , Transcription Factors/genetics , Transcription Factors/metabolism
19.
PLoS Genet ; 15(2): e1008014, 2019 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30817793
20.
Behav Genet ; 49(3): 340-346, 2019 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30739272

ABSTRACT

Better characterization of the sources of phenotypic variation in human behavioural traits-stemming from genetic and environmental influences-will allow for more informed decisions about how to approach a range of challenges arising from variation, ranging from societal issues to the treatment of diseases. In particular, understanding how the environment moderates genetic influence on phenotypes (i.e., genotype-environment interactions, or G × E) is a central component of the behavioral sciences. Yet, understanding of this phenomenon is lagging somewhat, due in part to the difficulties of detecting G × E. We discuss the logic behind one of the primary ways to detect G × E: comparing heritability estimates across environments. Then, we highlight some pitfalls, with an emphasis on how very strong G × E can sometimes be undetectable using this method when high heritability is present in multiple environments. We conclude by forwarding some initial, yet tentative, suggestions for how best to address to the problem.


Subject(s)
Genetic Variation/genetics , Genetics, Behavioral/methods , Quantitative Trait, Heritable , Environment , Gene-Environment Interaction , Genotype , Humans , Phenotype , Research
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...