Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 34
Filter
2.
Neurocase ; 26(6): 368-371, 2020 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33175666

ABSTRACT

A 48-year-old female suffered from cerebral infarction involving the left inferior frontal gyrus. This was due to ischemic complications of endovascular treatment for subarachnoid hemorrhage. She exhibited severe acalculia, agraphia, finger agnosia, and right-left disorientation (the four features of Gerstmann syndrome), but aphasia was scarcely noticeable. Single-photon emission tomography revealed hypoperfusion in the left inferior frontal area and also in the left parietal area. It is possible that Gerstmann syndrome was caused in the present case by disruption of the association fiber connecting the inferior frontal area with the inferior parietal area.


Subject(s)
Cerebral Infarction/complications , Gerstmann Syndrome/etiology , Prefrontal Cortex/pathology , Cerebral Infarction/diagnostic imaging , Cerebral Infarction/pathology , Female , Gerstmann Syndrome/diagnostic imaging , Gerstmann Syndrome/pathology , Gerstmann Syndrome/physiopathology , Humans , Middle Aged , Prefrontal Cortex/diagnostic imaging , Tomography, Emission-Computed, Single-Photon
3.
Handb Clin Neurol ; 151: 395-411, 2018.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29519471

ABSTRACT

This chapter offers a perspective on the origin, operational definition, historic vicissitudes, and current status of Gerstmann syndrome. The main issues and controversy accompanying Gerstmann syndrome throughout the years are reviewed. The clinical picture of Gerstmann syndrome as it emerges from a series of modern-day pure cases is described. In current clinical practice, a diagnosis of Gerstmann syndrome indicates the concomitant presence of four acquired symptoms: finger agnosia, acalculia, left-right disorientation, and agraphia. Finally, based on empiric work conducted in recent years, the chapter concludes with a new interpretation of Gerstmann syndrome. If seen as an instance of intraparietal disconnection, this classic parietal syndrome will acquire fresh clinical and theoretic significance.


Subject(s)
Gerstmann Syndrome/history , Gerstmann Syndrome/physiopathology , Gerstmann Syndrome/diagnosis , History, 20th Century , History, 21st Century , Humans
4.
Arch Clin Neuropsychol ; 29(8): 828-33, 2014 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25377466

ABSTRACT

Gerstmann's syndrome includes the clinical tetrad of finger agnosia, agraphia, acalculia, and right-left confusion. Some disagreement remains with regard to the exact localization of the syndrome, but most probable it involves the left angular gyrus with a subcortical extension. Several authors have suggested that a defect in mental spatial rotations could simultaneously account for acalculia, right-left disorientation, and finger agnosia. It has been also suggested that semantic aphasia is always associated with acalculia; as a matter of fact, left angular gyrus has a significant involvement in semantic processing. In this paper, it is proposed that Gerstmann's syndrome should include: acalculia, finger agnosia, right-left disorientation, and semantic aphasia, but not agraphia. When the pathology extends toward the superior parietal gyrus, agraphia can be found. A fundamental defect (i.e., an impairment in verbally mediated spatial operations) could explain these apparently unrelated clinical signs.


Subject(s)
Gerstmann Syndrome/physiopathology , Gerstmann Syndrome/pathology , Humans
5.
Brain Cogn ; 88: 14-20, 2014 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24833400

ABSTRACT

A number of recent studies have shown conflicting evidence as to common or distinct representations between symbolic ordinality and quantity. We investigated this issue through a series of neuropsychological tests in a unique Chinese patient with the left angular gyrus and left supramarginal gyrus lesions. Behavioral experiments revealed that (1) the patient showed Gerstmann syndrome, with minimal anomia and alexia and (2) the patient showed the dissociation among number semantic representations with relatively preserved symbolic quantity knowledge and impaired processing of symbolic order meaning. Together with existing evidence in the literature, results of the current study suggest that there might be two separate cognitive representations of symbolic ordinality and quantity in logographic language according to this dissociation. Most importantly, another merit of this study is that the left angular gyrus and left supramarginal gyrus might be necessary to symbolic ordinality representation.


Subject(s)
Concept Formation , Gerstmann Syndrome/psychology , Aphasia/physiopathology , Brain/physiopathology , Concept Formation/physiology , Diffusion Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Female , Gerstmann Syndrome/physiopathology , Humans , Mathematics , Middle Aged , Neuroimaging , Neuropsychological Tests
6.
J Neurosurg ; 116(6): 1182-6, 2012 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22443504

ABSTRACT

OBJECT: The impact of parietal lobe gliomas is typically studied in the context of parietal lobe syndromes. However, critical language pathways traverse the parietal lobe and are susceptible during tumor resection. The authors of this study reviewed their experience with parietal gliomas to characterize the impact of resection and the morbidity associated with language. METHODS: The study population included adults who had undergone resection of parietal gliomas of all grades. Tumor location was identified according to a proposed classification system for parietal region gliomas. Low- and high-grade tumors were volumetrically analyzed using FLAIR and T1-weighted contrast-enhanced MR imaging. RESULTS: One hundred nineteen patients with parietal gliomas were identified--34 with low-grade gliomas and 85 with high-grade gliomas. The median patient age was 45 years, and most patients (53) presented with seizures, whereas only 4 patients had an appreciable parietal lobe syndrome. The median preoperative tumor volume was 31.3 cm(3), the median extent of resection was 96%, and the median postoperative tumor volume was 0.9 cm(3). Surprisingly, the most common early postoperative neurological deficit was dysphasia (16 patients), not weakness (12 patients), sensory deficits (14 patients), or parietal lobe syndrome (10 patients). A proposed parietal glioma classification system, based on surgical anatomy, was predictive of language deficits. CONCLUSIONS: This is the largest reported experience with parietal lobe gliomas. The findings suggested that parietal language pathways are compromised at a surprisingly high rate. The proposed parietal glioma classification system is predictive of postoperative morbidity associated with language and can assist with preoperative planning. Taken together, these data emphasize the value of identifying language pathways when operating within the parietal lobe.


Subject(s)
Aphasia/diagnosis , Brain Neoplasms/surgery , Gerstmann Syndrome/diagnosis , Glioma/surgery , Parietal Lobe/surgery , Postoperative Complications/diagnosis , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Aphasia/physiopathology , Brain Mapping , Brain Neoplasms/pathology , Brain Neoplasms/physiopathology , Female , Gerstmann Syndrome/physiopathology , Glioma/pathology , Humans , Image Enhancement , Image Interpretation, Computer-Assisted , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Middle Aged , Neoplasm Grading , Parietal Lobe/pathology , Parietal Lobe/physiopathology , Postoperative Complications/physiopathology , Retrospective Studies , Risk Factors , Tumor Burden/physiology , Young Adult
7.
Neuroscientist ; 17(6): 633-44, 2011 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21670425

ABSTRACT

That disconnection causes clinical symptoms is a very influential concept in behavioral neurology. Criteria for subcortical disconnection usually are symptoms that are distinct from those following cortical lesions and damage to a single, long-range fiber tract. Yet, a recent study combining functional magnetic resonance imaging and fiber tracking concluded that a focal lesion in left parietal white matter provides the only tenable explanation for pure Gerstmann's syndrome, an enigmatic tetrad of acalculia, agraphia, finger agnosia, and left-right disorientation. Such a lesion would affect not only a single fiber tract but crossing or "kissing" of different fiber tracts and hence disconnect separate cortical networks. As fiber crossing is prominent in the cerebral white matter, the authors propose an extension to the subcortical disconnection framework that opens the door to ascribing a more diversified clinical phenomenology to white matter damage and ensuing disconnection than has been the case so far.


Subject(s)
Brain/pathology , Gerstmann Syndrome/pathology , Gerstmann Syndrome/physiopathology , Agraphia/pathology , Agraphia/physiopathology , Brain/physiopathology , Diffusion Tensor Imaging , Dyscalculia/pathology , Dyscalculia/physiopathology , Gerstmann Syndrome/etiology , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Neuroimaging
8.
Brain ; 133(Pt 2): 320-32, 2010 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19903731

ABSTRACT

Eighty years ago, the Austrian neurologist Josef Gerstmann observed in a few patients a concomitant impairment in discriminating their own fingers, writing by hand, distinguishing left from right and performing calculations. He claimed that this tetrad of symptoms constituted a syndromal entity, assigned it to a lesion of the dominant parietal lobe and suggested that it was due to damage of a common functional denominator. Ever since, these claims have been debated and an astute synopsis and sceptical discussion was presented 40 years ago by MacDonald Critchley in this journal. Nonetheless, Gerstmann's syndrome has continued to intrigue both clinical neurologists and researchers in neuropsychology, and more frequently than not is described in textbooks as an example of parietal lobe damage. In this review, we revisit the chequered history of this syndrome, which can be seen as a case study of the dialectic evolution of concepts in neuropsychology. In light of several modern era findings of pure cases we conclude that it is legitimate to label the conjunction of symptoms first described by Gerstmann as a 'syndrome', but that it is very unlikely that damage to the same population of cortical neurons should account for all of the four symptoms. Instead, we propose that a pure form of Gerstmann's syndrome might arise from disconnection, via a lesion, to separate but co-localized fibre tracts in the subcortical parietal white matter, a hypothesis for which we have recently provided evidence using combined imaging of functional and structural organization in the healthy brain.


Subject(s)
Gerstmann Syndrome/diagnosis , Gerstmann Syndrome/psychology , Neuropsychology/methods , Animals , Gerstmann Syndrome/physiopathology , Humans
9.
Ann Neurol ; 66(5): 654-62, 2009 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19938150

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: To examine the functional neuroanatomy that could account for pure Gerstmann syndrome, which is the selective association of acalculia, finger agnosia, left-right disorientation, and agraphia. METHODS: We used structural and functional neuroimaging at high spatial resolution in healthy subjects to seek a shared cortical substrate of the Grundstörung posited by Gerstmann, ie, a common functional denominator accounting for this clinical tetrad. We construed a functional activation paradigm that mirrors each of the four clinical deficits in Gerstmann syndrome and determined cortical activation patterns. We then applied fiber tracking to diffusion tensor images and used cortical activation foci in the four functional domains as seed regions. RESULTS: None of the subjects showed parietal overlap of cortical activation patterns from the four cognitive domains. In every subject, however, the parietal activation patterns across all four domains consistently connected to a small region of subcortical parietal white matter at a location that is congruent with the lesion in a well-documented case of pure Gerstmann syndrome. INTERPRETATION: Our functional neuroimaging findings are not in agreement with Gerstmann's postulate of damage to a common cognitive function underpinning clinical semiology. Our evidence from intact functional neuroanatomy suggests that pure forms of Gerstmann's tetrad do not arise from lesion to a shared cortical substrate but from intraparietal disconnection after damage to a focal region of subcortical white matter.


Subject(s)
Cerebral Cortex/anatomy & histology , Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Functional Laterality/physiology , Gerstmann Syndrome/pathology , Brain Mapping/methods , Cerebral Cortex/physiopathology , Gerstmann Syndrome/physiopathology , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Male , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Young Adult
11.
CNS Spectr ; 12(7): 527-36, 2007 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17603404

ABSTRACT

Brain-damaged patients with lesion or dysfunction involving the parietal cortex may show a variety of neuropsychological impairments involving spatial cognition. The more frequent and disabling deficit is the syndrome of unilateral spatial neglect that, in a nutshell, consists in a bias of spatial representation and attention ipsilateral to of extrapersonal, personal (ie, the body) space, or both, toward the side of the hemispheric lesion. The deficit is more frequent and severe after damage to the right hemisphere, involving particularly the posterior-inferior parietal cortex at the temporo-parietal junction. Damage to these posterior parietal regions may also impair visuospatial short-term memory, which may be associated with and worsen spatial neglect. The neural network supporting spatial representation, attention and short-term memory is, however, more extensive, including the right premotor cortex. Also disorders of drawing and building objects (traditionally termed constructional apraxia) are a frequent indicator of posterior parietal damage in the left and in the right hemispheres. Other less frequent deficits, which, however, have a relevant localizing value, include optic ataxia (namely, the defective reaching of visual objects, in the absence of elementary visuo-motor impairments), which is typically brought about by damage to the superior parietal lobule. Optic ataxia, together with deficits of visual attention, of estimating distances and depth, and with apraxia of gaze, constitutes the severely disabling Balint-Holmes' syndrome, which is typically associated with bilateral posterior parietal and occipital damage. Finally, lesions of the posterior parietal lobule (angular gyrus) in the left hemisphere may bring about a tetrad of symptoms (left-right disorientation, acalculia, finger agnosia, and agraphia) termed Gerstmann's syndrome, that also exists in a developmental form.


Subject(s)
Ataxia/complications , Gerstmann Syndrome/diagnosis , Gerstmann Syndrome/physiopathology , Optic Atrophy/complications , Perceptual Disorders/diagnosis , Space Perception/physiology , Ataxia/diagnosis , Ataxia/epidemiology , Functional Laterality/physiology , Gerstmann Syndrome/epidemiology , Humans , Neuropsychological Tests , Optic Atrophy/epidemiology , Perceptual Disorders/epidemiology , Perceptual Disorders/physiopathology , Severity of Illness Index , Syndrome
12.
Eur Neurol ; 52(1): 1-6, 2004.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15218337

ABSTRACT

Gerstmann's syndrome comprises finger agnosia, peripheral agraphia, anarithmetia, and right-left confusion. We here report a single-case study of an 85-year-old ambidextrous man who exhibited pure Gerstmann's syndrome (i.e., without aphasia) 10 weeks after a stroke involving the angular gyrus in the left parietal lobe. We hypothesize that, in this case, the main cognitive denominator of Gerstmann's tetrad was a severe dysfunction in mental rotation and translation. This report provides further evidence for the spatial nature of Gerstmann's syndrome.


Subject(s)
Cognition Disorders/etiology , Gerstmann Syndrome/physiopathology , Imagery, Psychotherapy , Space Perception/physiology , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Functional Laterality/physiology , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Male , Recognition, Psychology
13.
J Neurosurg ; 99(4): 716-27, 2003 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14567608

ABSTRACT

OBJECT: In an attempt to gain a better understanding of the cerebral functions represented in the angular gyrus and to spare them during surgery, the authors studied patients with brain tumors located close to the angular gyrus and mapped cortical sites by using electrostimulation. METHODS: Before undergoing tumor removal, six right-handed patients (five with left and one with right hemisphere tumors) were studied using cortical mapping with the aid of calculating, writing, finger-recognition, and color-naming tasks in addition to standard reading and object-naming tasks (for a total of 36 brain mapping studies). Strict conditions of functional site validation were applied to include only those cortical sites that produced repetitive interferences in the function tested. Preoperatively, four of the patients exhibited discrete symptoms related to Gerstmann syndrome while performing very specific tasks, whereas the other two patients presented with no symptoms of the syndrome. No patient had significant language or apraxic deficits. Distinct or shared cortical sites producing interferences in calculating, finger recognition, and writing were repeatedly found in the angular gyrus. Object- or color-naming sites and reading-interference sites were also found in or close to the angular gyrus; although frequently demonstrated, these latter results were variable and unpredictable in the group of patients studied. Finger agnosia and acalculia sites were also found elsewhere, such as in the supramarginal gyrus or close to the intraparietal sulcus. Mechanisms involved in acalculia, agraphia, or finger agnosia (either complete interferences or hesitations) during stimulation were various, from an aphasia-like form (for instance, the patient did not understand the numbers or words given for calculating or writing tasks) to an apparently pure interference in the function tested (patients understood the numbers, but were unable to perform a simple addition). CONCLUSIONS: Symptoms of Gerstmann syndrome can be found during direct brain mapping in the angular gyrus region. In this series of patients, sites producing interferences in writing, calculating, and finger recognition were demonstrated in the angular gyrus, which may or may not have been associated with object-naming, color-naming, or reading sites.


Subject(s)
Agnosia/diagnosis , Brain Mapping/instrumentation , Cognition Disorders/diagnosis , Electric Stimulation Therapy/instrumentation , Frontal Lobe/physiopathology , Gerstmann Syndrome/physiopathology , Gerstmann Syndrome/therapy , Occipital Lobe/physiopathology , Parietal Lobe/physiopathology , Psychomotor Disorders/diagnosis , Recognition, Psychology , Temporal Lobe/physiopathology , Brain Neoplasms/complications , Female , Frontal Lobe/pathology , Gerstmann Syndrome/etiology , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Mathematics , Middle Aged , Occipital Lobe/pathology , Parietal Lobe/pathology , Postoperative Complications , Prospective Studies , Severity of Illness Index , Temporal Lobe/pathology
14.
Neuropsychologia ; 41(14): 1942-58, 2003.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14572527

ABSTRACT

Numerical abilities are thought to rest on the integration of two distinct systems, a verbal system of number words and a non-symbolic representation of approximate quantities. This view has lead to the classification of acalculias into two broad categories depending on whether the deficit affects the verbal or the quantity system. Here, we test the association of deficits predicted by this theory, and particularly the presence or absence of impairments in non-symbolic quantity processing. We describe two acalculic patients, one with a focal lesion of the left parietal lobe and Gerstmann's syndrome and another with semantic dementia with predominantly left temporal hypometabolism. As predicted by a quantity deficit, the first patient was more impaired in subtraction than in multiplication, showed a severe slowness in approximation, and exhibited associated impairments in subitizing and numerical comparison tasks, both with Arabic digits and with arrays of dots. As predicted by a verbal deficit, the second patient was more impaired in multiplication than in subtraction, had intact approximation abilities, and showed preserved processing of non-symbolic numerosities.


Subject(s)
Brain Diseases/physiopathology , Language Disorders/physiopathology , Problem Solving/physiology , Verbal Behavior/physiology , Verbal Learning/physiology , Aged , Aphasia/physiopathology , Brain Diseases/complications , Chi-Square Distribution , Dissociative Disorders/complications , Dissociative Disorders/physiopathology , Female , Functional Laterality , Gerstmann Syndrome/physiopathology , Humans , Language Disorders/etiology , Mathematics , Mental Status Schedule , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Reaction Time , Weights and Measures
15.
Neuroreport ; 14(6): 861-5, 2003 May 06.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12858048

ABSTRACT

This study investigated the neural mechanisms involved in the writing of Arabic numerals, kanji, and kana. Tasks involving writing numerals in Arabic, kanji, and, kana were administered to four patients with Gerstmann's syndrome and to five Wernicke aphasics. The results indicated that the ability to write Arabic numerals was well preserved in the Wernicke aphasics despite their serious phonological disturbances. The patients with Gerstmann's syndrome, who have a deficit with the concept of number, could write kanji numerals better than Arabic and kana numerals. Unlike Arabic numerals (ideogram) and kana (syllabogram), kanji (morphogram) have both semantic and phonetic values. The results suggested that Arabic numerals may be somesthetic and linked directly to the concept of number bypassing phonological analysis.


Subject(s)
Aphasia, Wernicke/physiopathology , Brain/physiopathology , Gerstmann Syndrome/physiopathology , Handwriting , Aged , Aphasia, Wernicke/psychology , Female , Gerstmann Syndrome/psychology , Humans , Language Tests , Male , Mathematics , Middle Aged , Phonetics , Semantics
16.
J Psychiatry Neurosci ; 27(5): 364-8, 2002 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12271792

ABSTRACT

A 67-year-old left-handed woman with a diagnosis of pseudodementia was being treated for depression with little benefit. Neuropsychological evaluations revealed features of angular gyrus syndrome, namely, agraphia, alexia, Gerstmann's syndrome and behavioural manifestations such as depression, poor memory, frustration and irritability. A computed tomographic scan showed a right occipito-temporal infarction, which had occurred 18 months earlier. The patient demonstrated aspects of language dysfunction associated with the syndrome and showed reversed lateralization of cerebral functions. Recognizing and distinguishing between angular gyrus syndrome and depression is important because the appropriate therapies differ. The use of the term pseudodementia can be misleading.


Subject(s)
Dementia/diagnosis , Dementia/physiopathology , Dentate Gyrus/physiopathology , Depression/diagnosis , Depression/physiopathology , Gerstmann Syndrome/diagnosis , Gerstmann Syndrome/physiopathology , Aged , Dentate Gyrus/diagnostic imaging , Diagnosis, Differential , Female , Humans , Tomography, X-Ray Computed
17.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry ; 72(3): 403-5, 2002 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11861708

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: It remains unclear from lesion studies whether the four signs of the Gerstmann syndrome (finger agnosia, acalculia, agraphia, and right-left confusion) cluster because the neuronal nets that mediate these activities have anatomical proximity, or because these four functions share a common network. If there is a common network, with degeneration, as may occur in Alzheimer's disease, each of the signs associated with Gerstmann's syndrome should correlate with the other three signs more closely than they correlate with other cognitive deficits. METHODS: Thirty eight patients with probable Alzheimer's disease were included in a retrospective analysis of neuropsychological functions. RESULTS: The four Gerstmann's syndrome signs did not cluster together. Finger naming and calculations were not significantly correlated. Right-left knowledge and calculations also did not correlate. CONCLUSIONS: The four cognitive functions impaired in Gerstmann's syndrome do not share a common neuronal network, and their co-occurrence with dominant parietal lobe injuries may be related to the anatomical proximity of the different networks mediating these functions.


Subject(s)
Alzheimer Disease/diagnosis , Gerstmann Syndrome/diagnosis , Neuropsychological Tests , Adult , Aged , Alzheimer Disease/physiopathology , Anomia/diagnosis , Anomia/physiopathology , Diagnosis, Differential , Dominance, Cerebral/physiology , Female , Gerstmann Syndrome/physiopathology , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Nerve Net/physiopathology , Parietal Lobe/physiopathology , Problem Solving
18.
Rev Neurol ; 33(6): 568-76, 2001.
Article in Spanish | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11727242

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: To suggest a cerebral map of elementary mathematical thinking, and integrate the most relevant findings from neuropsychology with those from cerebral imaging techniques and cognitive behavior experiments. DEVELOPMENT: Firstly we describe investigations into our numerical sense and the way in which numerical information is represented in the human brain. Then, using a multidisciplinary approach, we present the results of different studies of Gerstmann's syndrome, regarding the relation between numerical ability and other cognitive skills; the different participation of the cerebral hemispheres and the special implication of the parietal lobe in mathematical tasks. CONCLUSIONS: Different cerebral regions are involved in doing mental arithmetic, however simple. This makes one think more in terms of cerebral circuits than in a phrenological idea which would assign the responsibility for arithmetical calculations to a specific region. The similarity between the results analysed leads us to the conclusion that one region is particularly involved in understanding numbers, namely the inferior part of the parietal lobe. Different neuronal circuits are used depending on the type of task to be performed. Finally we describe the most relevant models for the processing of numbers which have been developed during the study.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiopathology , Cognition Disorders/physiopathology , Mathematics , Thinking , Brain/pathology , Cognition Disorders/diagnosis , Functional Laterality/physiology , Gerstmann Syndrome/diagnosis , Gerstmann Syndrome/physiopathology , Humans , Language Disorders/diagnosis , Language Disorders/physiopathology , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Memory Disorders/diagnosis , Memory Disorders/physiopathology , Neuropsychological Tests , Parietal Lobe/physiopathology
19.
Brain Res Cogn Brain Res ; 12(2): 337-40, 2001 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11587904

ABSTRACT

Writing is a uniquely human skill that we utilize nearly everyday. Lesion studies in patients with Gerstmann's syndrome have pointed to the parietal cortex as being critical for writing. Very little information is, however, available about the precise anatomical location of brain regions subserving writing in normal healthy individuals. In this study, we used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to investigate parietal lobe function during writing to dictation. Significant clusters of activation were observed in left superior parietal lobe (SPL) and the dorsal aspects of the inferior parietal cortex (IPC) bordering the SPL. Localized clusters of activation were also observed in the left premotor cortex, sensorimotor cortex and supplementary motor area. No activation cluster was observed in the right hemisphere. These results clearly indicate that writing appears to be primarily organized in the language-dominant hemisphere. Further analysis revealed that within the parietal cortex, activation was significantly greater in the left SPL, compared to left IPC. Together with lesion studies, findings from the present study provide further evidence for the essential role of the left SPL in writing. Deficits to the precise left hemisphere parietal cortex regions identified in the present study may specifically underlie disorders of writing observed in Gerstmann's syndrome and apractic agraphia.


Subject(s)
Dominance, Cerebral/physiology , Handwriting , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Parietal Lobe/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Agraphia/physiopathology , Female , Gerstmann Syndrome/physiopathology , Humans , Male
20.
Brain ; 122 ( Pt 6): 1107-20, 1999 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10356063

ABSTRACT

The four symptoms composing Gerstmann's syndrome were postulated to result from a common cognitive denominator (Grundstörung) by Gerstmann himself. He suggested that it is a disorder of the body schema restricted to the hand and fingers. The existence of a Grundstörung has since been contested. Here we suggest that a common psychoneurological factor does exist, but should be related to transformations of mental images rather than to the body schema. A patient (H.P.) was studied, who presented the four symptoms of Gerstmann's syndrome in the absence of any other neuropsychological disorders. MRI showed a focal ischaemic lesion, situated subcortically in the inferior part of the left angular gyrus and reaching the superior posterior region of T1. The cortical layers were spared and the lesion was seen to extend to the callosal fibres. On the basis of an extensive cognitive investigation, language, praxis, memory and intelligence disorders were excluded. The four remaining symptoms (finger agnosia, agraphia, right-left disorientation and dyscalculia) were investigated thoroughly with the aim of determining any characteristics that they might share. Detailed analyses of the tetrad showed that the impairment was consistently attributable to disorders of a spatial nature. Furthermore, cognitive tests necessitating mental rotation were equally shown to be impaired, confirming the essentially visuospatial origin of the disturbance. In the light of this report, the common cognitive denominator is hypothesized to be an impairment in mental manipulation of images and not in body schema.


Subject(s)
Brain/pathology , Gerstmann Syndrome/diagnosis , Brain Ischemia/diagnosis , Diagnosis, Differential , Gerstmann Syndrome/physiopathology , Gerstmann Syndrome/psychology , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Mental Processes , Middle Aged , Wechsler Scales
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...