Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 262
Filter
1.
Sci Adv ; 10(32): eadn0416, 2024 Aug 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39110810

ABSTRACT

Reactivating place cells during sharp-wave ripples in the hippocampus is important for memory consolidation. However, whether hippocampal reactivation is affected by the values of events experienced by the animal is largely unknown. Here, we investigated whether place cells in the dorsal (dHP) and intermediate hippocampus (iHP) of rats are differentially reactivated depending on the value associated with a place during the learning of places associated with higher-value rewards in a T-maze. Place cells in the iHP representing the high-value location were reactivated significantly more frequently than those representing the low-value location, characteristics not observed in the dHP. In contrast, the activities of place cells in the dHP coding the routes leading to high-value locations were replayed more than those in the iHP. Our findings suggest that value-based differential reactivation patterns along the septotemporal axis of the hippocampus may play essential roles in optimizing goal-directed spatial learning for maximal reward.


Subject(s)
Hippocampus , Animals , Hippocampus/physiology , Rats , Male , Reward , Maze Learning/physiology , Place Cells/physiology
2.
Adv Neurobiol ; 38: 195-214, 2024.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39008017

ABSTRACT

The hippocampus is indispensable for episodic memories, but its particular role in the process is still unclear. This chapter briefly overviews past studies focusing on place cells and memory engrams, highlighting their potential roles in spatial navigation. Future work reconciling these two lines of studies would provide a comprehensive view of the specific contribution of the hippocampus and a better understanding of how memory engrams support memory.


Subject(s)
Hippocampus , Memory, Episodic , Spatial Navigation , Hippocampus/physiology , Spatial Navigation/physiology , Humans , Animals , Place Cells/physiology
3.
Elife ; 122024 Jul 22.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39037765

ABSTRACT

Hippocampal place cells in freely moving rodents display both theta phase precession and procession, which is thought to play important roles in cognition, but the neural mechanism for producing theta phase shift remains largely unknown. Here, we show that firing rate adaptation within a continuous attractor neural network causes the neural activity bump to oscillate around the external input, resembling theta sweeps of decoded position during locomotion. These forward and backward sweeps naturally account for theta phase precession and procession of individual neurons, respectively. By tuning the adaptation strength, our model explains the difference between 'bimodal cells' showing interleaved phase precession and procession, and 'unimodal cells' in which phase precession predominates. Our model also explains the constant cycling of theta sweeps along different arms in a T-maze environment, the speed modulation of place cells' firing frequency, and the continued phase shift after transient silencing of the hippocampus. We hope that this study will aid an understanding of the neural mechanism supporting theta phase coding in the brain.


Subject(s)
Action Potentials , Place Cells , Theta Rhythm , Animals , Theta Rhythm/physiology , Place Cells/physiology , Action Potentials/physiology , Models, Neurological , Hippocampus/physiology , Hippocampus/cytology , Adaptation, Physiological , Rats
4.
Cereb Cortex ; 34(7)2024 Jul 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39016432

ABSTRACT

Sound is an important navigational cue for mammals. During spatial navigation, hippocampal place cells encode spatial representations of the environment based on visual information, but to what extent audiospatial information can enable reliable place cell mapping is largely unknown. We assessed this by recording from CA1 place cells in the dark, under circumstances where reliable visual, tactile, or olfactory information was unavailable. Male rats were exposed to auditory cues of different frequencies that were delivered from local or distal spatial locations. We observed that distal, but not local cue presentation, enables and supports stable place fields, regardless of the sound frequency used. Our data suggest that a context dependency exists regarding the relevance of auditory information for place field mapping: whereas locally available auditory cues do not serve as a salient spatial basis for the anchoring of place fields, auditory cue localization supports spatial representations by place cells when available in the form of distal information. Furthermore, our results demonstrate that CA1 neurons can effectively use auditory stimuli to generate place fields, and that hippocampal pyramidal neurons are not solely dependent on visual cues for the generation of place field representations based on allocentric reference frames.


Subject(s)
Acoustic Stimulation , Cues , Place Cells , Rats, Long-Evans , Space Perception , Animals , Male , Place Cells/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , CA1 Region, Hippocampal/physiology , CA1 Region, Hippocampal/cytology , Rats , Auditory Perception/physiology , Action Potentials/physiology , Spatial Navigation/physiology
5.
Sci Rep ; 14(1): 16714, 2024 07 19.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39030197

ABSTRACT

Studies on the neural correlates of navigation in 3D environments are plagued by several issues that need to be solved. For example, experimental studies show markedly different place cell responses in rats and bats, both navigating in 3D environments. In this study, we focus on modelling the spatial cells in rodents in a 3D environment. We propose a deep autoencoder network to model the place and grid cells in a simulated agent navigating in a 3D environment. The input layer to the autoencoder network model is the HD layer, which encodes the agent's HD in terms of azimuth (θ) and pitch angles (ϕ). The output of this layer is given as input to the Path Integration (PI) layer, which computes displacement in all the preferred directions. The bottleneck layer of the autoencoder model encodes the spatial cell-like responses. Both grid cell and place cell-like responses are observed. The proposed model is verified using two experimental studies with two 3D environments. This model paves the way for a holistic approach using deep neural networks to model spatial cells in 3D navigation.


Subject(s)
Hippocampus , Animals , Hippocampus/physiology , Hippocampus/cytology , Rats , Models, Neurological , Place Cells/physiology , Neural Networks, Computer , Spatial Navigation/physiology , Grid Cells/physiology , Rodentia
6.
Nat Neurosci ; 27(8): 1599-1608, 2024 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38937582

ABSTRACT

Hippocampal place cells are influenced by both self-motion (idiothetic) signals and external sensory landmarks as an animal navigates its environment. To continuously update a position signal on an internal 'cognitive map', the hippocampal system integrates self-motion signals over time, a process that relies on a finely calibrated path integration gain that relates movement in physical space to movement on the cognitive map. It is unclear whether idiothetic cues alone, such as optic flow, exert sufficient influence on the cognitive map to enable recalibration of path integration, or if polarizing position information provided by landmarks is essential for this recalibration. Here, we demonstrate both recalibration of path integration gain and systematic control of place fields by pure optic flow information in freely moving rats. These findings demonstrate that the brain continuously rebalances the influence of conflicting idiothetic cues to fine-tune the neural dynamics of path integration, and that this recalibration process does not require a top-down, unambiguous position signal from landmarks.


Subject(s)
Optic Flow , Place Cells , Rats, Long-Evans , Animals , Optic Flow/physiology , Rats , Male , Place Cells/physiology , Cues , Space Perception/physiology , Hippocampus/physiology , Hippocampus/cytology
7.
Curr Biol ; 34(10): 2256-2264.e3, 2024 05 20.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38701787

ABSTRACT

The hippocampal formation contains neurons responsive to an animal's current location and orientation, which together provide the organism with a neural map of space.1,2,3 Spatially tuned neurons rely on external landmark cues and internally generated movement information to estimate position.4,5 An important class of landmark cue are the boundaries delimiting an environment, which can define place cell field position6,7 and stabilize grid cell firing.8 However, the precise nature of the sensory information used to detect boundaries remains unknown. We used 2-dimensional virtual reality (VR)9 to show that visual cues from elevated walls surrounding the environment are both sufficient and necessary to stabilize place and grid cell responses in VR, when only visual and self-motion cues are available. By contrast, flat boundaries formed by the edges of a textured floor did not stabilize place and grid cells, indicating only specific forms of visual boundary stabilize hippocampal spatial firing. Unstable grid cells retain internally coherent, hexagonally arranged firing fields, but these fields "drift" with respect to the virtual environment over periods >5 s. Optic flow from a virtual floor does not slow drift dynamics, emphasizing the importance of boundary-related visual information. Surprisingly, place fields are more stable close to boundaries even with floor and wall cues removed, suggesting invisible boundaries are inferred using the motion of a discrete, separate cue (a beacon signaling reward location). Subsets of place cells show allocentric directional tuning toward the beacon, with strength of tuning correlating with place field stability when boundaries are removed.


Subject(s)
Cues , Grid Cells , Virtual Reality , Animals , Grid Cells/physiology , Male , Hippocampus/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Rats , Place Cells/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Rats, Long-Evans , Orientation/physiology
8.
Nat Commun ; 15(1): 3702, 2024 May 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38697969

ABSTRACT

Hippocampal place cells represent the position of a rodent within an environment. In addition, recent experiments show that the CA1 subfield of a passive observer also represents the position of a conspecific performing a spatial task. However, whether this representation is allocentric, egocentric or mixed is less clear. In this study we investigated the representation of others during free behavior and in a task where female mice learned to follow a conspecific for a reward. We found that most cells represent the position of others relative to self-position (social-vector cells) rather than to the environment, with a prevalence of purely egocentric coding modulated by context and mouse identity. Learning of a pursuit task improved the tuning of social-vector cells, but their number remained invariant. Collectively, our results suggest that the hippocampus flexibly codes the position of others in multiple coordinate systems, albeit favoring the self as a reference point.


Subject(s)
CA1 Region, Hippocampal , Animals , Female , CA1 Region, Hippocampal/physiology , CA1 Region, Hippocampal/cytology , Mice , Mice, Inbred C57BL , Place Cells/physiology , Reward , Behavior, Animal/physiology
9.
Nat Rev Neurosci ; 25(6): 428-448, 2024 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38714834

ABSTRACT

The representation of distinct spaces by hippocampal place cells has been linked to changes in their place fields (the locations in the environment where the place cells discharge strongly), a phenomenon that has been termed 'remapping'. Remapping has been assumed to be accompanied by the reorganization of subsecond cofiring relationships among the place cells, potentially maximizing hippocampal information coding capacity. However, several observations challenge this standard view. For example, place cells exhibit mixed selectivity, encode non-positional variables, can have multiple place fields and exhibit unreliable discharge in fixed environments. Furthermore, recent evidence suggests that, when measured at subsecond timescales, the moment-to-moment cofiring of a pair of cells in one environment is remarkably similar in another environment, despite remapping. Here, I propose that remapping is a misnomer for the changes in place fields across environments and suggest instead that internally organized manifold representations of hippocampal activity are actively registered to different environments to enable navigation, promote memory and organize knowledge.


Subject(s)
Hippocampus , Space Perception , Hippocampus/physiology , Animals , Humans , Space Perception/physiology , Place Cells/physiology
10.
Sheng Wu Yi Xue Gong Cheng Xue Za Zhi ; 41(2): 335-341, 2024 Apr 25.
Article in Chinese | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38686415

ABSTRACT

Place cell with location tuning characteristics play an important role in brain spatial cognition and navigation, but there is relatively little research on place cell screening and its influencing factors. Taking pigeons as model animals, the screening process of pigeon place cell was given by using the spike signal in pigeon hippocampus under free activity. The effects of grid number and filter kernel size on the place field of place cells during the screening process were analyzed. The results from the real and simulation data showed that the proposed place cell screening method presented in this study could effectively screen out place cell, and the research found that the size of place field was basically inversely proportional to the number of grids divided, and was basically proportional to the size of Gaussian filter kernel in the overall trend. This result will not only help to determine the appropriate parameters in the place cell screening process, but also promote the research on the neural mechanism of spatial cognition and navigation of birds such as pigeons.


Subject(s)
Columbidae , Hippocampus , Columbidae/physiology , Animals , Hippocampus/cytology , Hippocampus/physiology , Place Cells/physiology , Spatial Navigation/physiology , Cognition , Action Potentials
11.
Nat Commun ; 15(1): 647, 2024 Jan 20.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38245502

ABSTRACT

The hippocampal subfield CA3 is thought to function as an auto-associative network that stores experiences as memories. Information from these experiences arrives directly from the entorhinal cortex as well as indirectly through the dentate gyrus, which performs sparsification and decorrelation. The computational purpose for these dual input pathways has not been firmly established. We model CA3 as a Hopfield-like network that stores both dense, correlated encodings and sparse, decorrelated encodings. As more memories are stored, the former merge along shared features while the latter remain distinct. We verify our model's prediction in rat CA3 place cells, which exhibit more distinct tuning during theta phases with sparser activity. Finally, we find that neural networks trained in multitask learning benefit from a loss term that promotes both correlated and decorrelated representations. Thus, the complementary encodings we have found in CA3 can provide broad computational advantages for solving complex tasks.


Subject(s)
Hippocampus , Place Cells , Rats , Animals , Learning , Entorhinal Cortex , Neural Networks, Computer , Dentate Gyrus
12.
Neurologia (Engl Ed) ; 39(3): 244-253, 2024 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37442425

ABSTRACT

INTRODUCTION: The relationship between the entorhinal cortex (EC) and the hippocampus has been studied by different authors, who have highlighted the importance of grid cells, place cells, and the trisynaptic circuit in the processes that they regulate: the persistence of spatial, explicit, and recent memory and their possible impairment with ageing. OBJECTIVE: We aimed to determine whether older age causes changes in the size and number of grid cells contained in layer III of the EC and in the granular layer of the dentate gyrus (DG) of the hippocampus. METHODS: We conducted post-mortem studies of the brains of 6 individuals aged 56-87 years. The brain sections containing the DG and the adjacent EC were stained according to the Klüver-Barrera method, then the ImageJ software was used to measure the individual neuronal area, the total neuronal area, and the number of neurons contained in rectangular areas in layer III of the EC and layer II of the DG. Statistical analysis was subsequently performed. RESULTS: We observed an age-related reduction in the cell population of the external pyramidal layer of the EC, and in the number of neurons in the granular layer of the DG. CONCLUSION: Our results indicate that ageing causes a decrease in the size and density of grid cells of the EC and place cells of the DG.


Subject(s)
Entorhinal Cortex , Place Cells , Humans , Entorhinal Cortex/physiology , Dentate Gyrus/physiology , Hippocampus , Neurons
13.
Sci Rep ; 13(1): 21763, 2023 12 08.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38066014

ABSTRACT

Hippocampal Place Cells (PCs) are pyramidal neurons showing spatially localized firing when an animal gets into a specific area within an environment. Because of their obvious and clear relation with specific cognitive functions, Place Cells operations and modulations are intensely studied experimentally. However, although a lot of data have been gathered since their discovery, the cellular processes that interplay to turn a hippocampal pyramidal neuron into a Place Cell are still not completely understood. Here, we used a morphologically and biophysically detailed computational model of a CA1 pyramidal neuron to show how, and under which conditions, it can turn into a neuron coding for a specific cue location, through the self-organization of its synaptic inputs in response to external signals targeting different dendritic layers. Our results show that the model is consistent with experimental findings demonstrating PCs stability within the same spatial context over different trajectories, environment rotations, and place field remapping to adapt to changes in the environment. To date, this is the only biophysically and morphologically accurate cellular model of PCs formation, which can be directly used in physiologically accurate microcircuits and large-scale model networks to study cognitive functions and dysfunctions at cellular level.


Subject(s)
Place Cells , Animals , Neurons/physiology , Pyramidal Cells/physiology , Hippocampus/physiology , Synapses/physiology , CA1 Region, Hippocampal/physiology , Action Potentials/physiology
14.
eNeuro ; 10(12)2023 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37973379

ABSTRACT

Spatial memories are represented by hippocampal place cells during navigation. This spatial code is dynamic, undergoing changes across time, known as representational drift, and across changes in internal state, even while navigating the same spatial environment with consistent behavior. A dynamic code may provide the hippocampus a means to track distinct epochs of experience that occur at different times or during different internal states and update spatial memories. Changes to the spatial code include place fields (PFs) that remap to new locations and place fields that vanish, while others are stable. However, what determines place field fate across epochs remains unclear. We measured the lap-by-lap properties of place cells in mice during navigation for a block of trials in a rewarded virtual environment. We then determined the position of the place fields in another block of trials in the same spatial environment either separated by a day (a distinct temporal epoch) or during the same session but with reward removed to change reward expectation (a distinct internal state epoch). We found that place cells with remapped place fields across epochs tended to have lower spatial precision during navigation in the initial epoch. Place cells with stable or vanished place fields tended to have higher spatial precision. We conclude that place cells with less precise place fields have greater spatial flexibility, allowing them to respond to, and track, distinct epochs of experience in the same spatial environment, while place cells with precise place fields generally preserve spatial information when their fields reappear.


Subject(s)
Hippocampus , Place Cells , Mice , Animals , Spatial Memory , Reward
15.
ACS Sens ; 8(12): 4765-4773, 2023 Dec 22.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38015643

ABSTRACT

The functioning of place cells requires the involvement of multiple neurotransmitters, with dopamine playing a critical role in hippocampal place cell activity. However, the exact mechanisms through which dopamine influences place cell activity remain largely unknown. Herein, we present the development of the integrated three-electrode dual-mode detection chip (ITDDC), which enables simultaneous recording of the place cell activity and dopamine concentration fluctuation. The working electrode, reference electrode, and counter electrode are all integrated within the ITDDC in electrochemical detection, enabling the real-time in situ monitoring of dopamine concentrations in animals in motion. The reference, working, and counter electrodes are surface-modified using PtNPs and polypyrrole, PtNPs and PEDOT:PSS, and PtNPs, respectively. This modification allows for the detection of dopamine concentrations as low as 20 nM. We conducted dual-mode testing on mice in a novel environment and an environment with food rewards. We found distinct dopamine concentration variations along different paths within a novel environment, implying that different dopamine levels may contribute to spatial memory. Moreover, environmental food rewards elevate dopamine significantly, followed by the intense firing of reward place cells, suggesting a crucial role of dopamine in facilitating the encoding of reward-associated locations in animals. The real-time and in situ recording capabilities of ITDDC offer new opportunities to investigate the interplay between electrophysiology and dopamine during animal exploration and reward-based memory and provide a novel glimpse into the correlation between dopamine levels and place cell activity.


Subject(s)
Dopamine , Place Cells , Mice , Animals , Polymers , Pyrroles , Electrodes , Reward
16.
Nat Commun ; 14(1): 7748, 2023 Nov 27.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38012135

ABSTRACT

Episodic memories comprise diverse attributes of experience distributed across neocortical areas. The hippocampus is integral to rapidly binding these diffuse representations, as they occur, to be later reinstated. However, the nature of the information exchanged during this hippocampal-cortical dialogue remains poorly understood. A recent study has shown that the secondary motor cortex carries two types of representations: place cell-like activity, which were impaired by hippocampal lesions, and responses tied to visuo-tactile cues, which became more pronounced following hippocampal lesions. Using two-photon Ca2+ imaging to record neuronal activities in the secondary motor cortex of male Thy1-GCaMP6s mice, we assessed the cortical retrieval of spatial and non-spatial attributes from previous explorations in a virtual environment. We show that, following navigation, spontaneous resting state reactivations convey varying degrees of spatial (trajectory sequences) and non-spatial (visuo-tactile attributes) information, while reactivations of non-spatial attributes tend to precede reactivations of spatial representations surrounding hippocampal sharp-wave ripples.


Subject(s)
Memory, Episodic , Place Cells , Male , Mice , Animals , Hippocampus/physiology , Neurons/physiology , Cues
17.
Elife ; 122023 10 04.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37792453

ABSTRACT

Hippocampal place cell sequences have been hypothesized to serve as diverse purposes as the induction of synaptic plasticity, formation and consolidation of long-term memories, or navigation and planning. During spatial behaviors of rodents, sequential firing of place cells at the theta timescale (known as theta sequences) encodes running trajectories, which can be considered as one-dimensional behavioral sequences of traversed locations. In a two-dimensional space, however, each single location can be visited along arbitrary one-dimensional running trajectories. Thus, a place cell will generally take part in multiple different theta sequences, raising questions about how this two-dimensional topology can be reconciled with the idea of hippocampal sequences underlying memory of (one-dimensional) episodes. Here, we propose a computational model of cornu ammonis 3 (CA3) and dentate gyrus (DG), where sensorimotor input drives the direction-dependent (extrinsic) theta sequences within CA3 reflecting the two-dimensional spatial topology, whereas the intrahippocampal CA3-DG projections concurrently produce intrinsic sequences that are independent of the specific running trajectory. Consistent with experimental data, intrinsic theta sequences are less prominent, but can nevertheless be detected during theta activity, thereby serving as running-direction independent landmark cues. We hypothesize that the intrinsic sequences largely reflect replay and preplay activity during non-theta states.


Subject(s)
Place Cells , Running , Hippocampus , CA3 Region, Hippocampal , Memory, Long-Term , Theta Rhythm , Action Potentials
18.
Cell Rep ; 42(10): 113142, 2023 10 31.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37742193

ABSTRACT

Hippocampus place cell discharge is temporally unreliable across seconds and days, and place fields are multimodal, suggesting an "ensemble cofiring" spatial coding hypothesis with manifold dynamics that does not require reliable spatial tuning, in contrast to hypotheses based on place field (spatial tuning) stability. We imaged mouse CA1 (cornu ammonis 1) ensembles in two environments across three weeks to evaluate these coding hypotheses. While place fields "remap," being more distinct between than within environments, coactivity relationships generally change less. Decoding location and environment from 1-s ensemble location-specific activity is effective and improves with experience. Decoding environment from cell-pair coactivity relationships is also effective and improves with experience, even after removing place tuning. Discriminating environments from 1-s ensemble coactivity relies crucially on the cells with the most anti-coactive cell-pair relationships because activity is internally organized on a low-dimensional manifold of non-linear coactivity relationships that intermittently reregisters to environments according to the anti-cofiring subpopulation activity.


Subject(s)
Hippocampus , Place Cells , Mice , Animals , CA1 Region, Hippocampal
19.
Elife ; 122023 07 19.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37466236

ABSTRACT

Aversive stimuli can cause hippocampal place cells to remap their firing fields, but it is not known whether remapping plays a role in storing memories of aversive experiences. Here, we addressed this question by performing in vivo calcium imaging of CA1 place cells in freely behaving rats (n = 14). Rats were first trained to prefer a short path over a long path for obtaining food reward, then trained to avoid the short path by delivering a mild footshock. Remapping was assessed by comparing place cell population vector similarity before acquisition versus after extinction of avoidance. Some rats received shock after systemic injections of the amnestic drug scopolamine at a dose (1 mg/kg) that impaired avoidance learning but spared spatial tuning and shock-evoked responses of CA1 neurons. Place cells remapped significantly more following remembered than forgotten shocks (drug-free versus scopolamine conditions); shock-induced remapping did not cause place fields to migrate toward or away from the shocked location and was similarly prevalent in cells that were responsive versus non-responsive to shocks. When rats were exposed to a neutral barrier rather than aversive shock, place cells remapped significantly less in response to the barrier. We conclude that place cell remapping occurs in response to events that are remembered rather than merely perceived and forgotten, suggesting that reorganization of hippocampal population codes may play a role in storing memories for aversive events.


The human brain is able to remember experiences that occurred at specific places and times, such as a birthday party held at a particular restaurant. A part of the brain known as the hippocampus helps to store these episodic memories, but how exactly is not fully understood. Within the hippocampus are specialized neurons known as place cells which 'label' locations with unique patterns of brain activity. When we revisit a place, such as the restaurant, place cells recall the stored pattern of brain activity allowing us to recognize the familiar location. It has been shown that a new negative experience at a familiar place ­ for example, if we went back to the restaurant and had a terrible meal ­ triggers place cells to update the brain activity label associated with the location. However, it remains uncertain whether this re-labelling assists in storing the memory of the unpleasant experience. To investigate, Blair et al. used a technique known as calcium imaging to monitor place cells in the hippocampus of freely moving rats. The rats were given a new experience ­ a mild foot shock ­ at a previously explored location. Tiny cameras attached to their heads were then used to record the activity of hundreds of place cells before and after the shock. Initially, the rats remembered the aversive experience and avoided the location where they had been shocked. Over time, the rats began to return to the location; however, their place cells displayed different patterns of activity compared to their previous visits before the shock. To test whether this change in place cell activity corresponded with new memories, another group of rats were administered a mild amnesia-inducing drug before the shock, causing them to forget the experience. These rats did not avoid the shock site or show any changes in place cell activity when they revisited it. These findings imply that new events cause place cells to alter their 'label' for a location only if the event is remembered, not if it is forgotten. This indicates that alterations in place cell activity patterns may play a role in storing memories of unpleasant experiences. Having a better understanding of how episodic memories are stored could lead to better treatments for diseases that impair memory, such as Alzheimer's disease and age-related dementia.


Subject(s)
Place Cells , Rats , Animals , Place Cells/physiology , Hippocampus/physiology , Neurons/physiology , Scopolamine Derivatives , CA1 Region, Hippocampal
20.
Cell Rep ; 42(8): 112871, 2023 08 29.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37494183

ABSTRACT

Learning novel experiences reorganizes hippocampal neuronal circuits, represented as coordinated reactivation patterns in post-experience offline states for memory consolidation. This study examines how awake synchronous events during a novel run are related to post-run reactivation patterns. The disruption of awake sharp-wave ripples inhibited experience-induced increases in the contributions of neurons to post-experience synchronous events. Hippocampal place cells that participate more in awake synchronous events are more strongly reactivated during post-experience synchronous events. Awake synchronous neuronal patterns, in cooperation with place-selective firing patterns, determine cell ensembles that undergo pronounced increases and decreases in their correlated spikes. Taken together, awake synchronous events are fundamental for identifying hippocampal neuronal ensembles to be incorporated into synchronous reactivation during subsequent offline states, thereby facilitating memory consolidation.


Subject(s)
Place Cells , Wakefulness , Wakefulness/physiology , Hippocampus/physiology , Neurons/physiology , Learning , Place Cells/physiology
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL