Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 28.031
Filter
1.
PLoS One ; 19(5): e0298116, 2024.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38722850

ABSTRACT

Spatial navigation is a multi-faceted behaviour drawing on many different aspects of cognition. Visuospatial abilities, such as mental rotation and visuospatial working memory, in particular, may be key factors. A range of tests have been developed to assess visuospatial processing and memory, but how such tests relate to navigation ability remains unclear. This understanding is important to advance tests of navigation for disease monitoring in various disorders (e.g., Alzheimer's disease) where spatial impairment is an early symptom. Here, we report the use of an established mobile gaming app, Sea Hero Quest (SHQ), as a measure of navigation ability in a sample of young, predominantly female university students (N = 78; 20; female = 74.3%; mean age = 20.33 years). We used three separate tests of navigation embedded in SHQ: wayfinding, path integration and spatial memory in a radial arm maze. In the same participants, we also collected measures of mental rotation (Mental Rotation Test), visuospatial processing (Design Organization Test) and visuospatial working memory (Digital Corsi). We found few strong correlations across our measures. Being good at wayfinding in a virtual navigation test does not mean an individual will also be good at path integration, have a superior memory in a radial arm maze, or rate themself as having a strong sense of direction. However, we observed that participants who were good in the wayfinding task of SHQ tended to perform well on the three visuospatial tasks examined here, and to also use a landmark strategy in the radial maze task. These findings help clarify the associations between different abilities involved in spatial navigation.


Subject(s)
Spatial Navigation , Humans , Female , Spatial Navigation/physiology , Male , Young Adult , Adult , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Spatial Memory/physiology , Maze Learning/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Adolescent , Mobile Applications
2.
Curr Biol ; 34(10): 2256-2264.e3, 2024 May 20.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38701787

ABSTRACT

The hippocampal formation contains neurons responsive to an animal's current location and orientation, which together provide the organism with a neural map of space.1,2,3 Spatially tuned neurons rely on external landmark cues and internally generated movement information to estimate position.4,5 An important class of landmark cue are the boundaries delimiting an environment, which can define place cell field position6,7 and stabilize grid cell firing.8 However, the precise nature of the sensory information used to detect boundaries remains unknown. We used 2-dimensional virtual reality (VR)9 to show that visual cues from elevated walls surrounding the environment are both sufficient and necessary to stabilize place and grid cell responses in VR, when only visual and self-motion cues are available. By contrast, flat boundaries formed by the edges of a textured floor did not stabilize place and grid cells, indicating only specific forms of visual boundary stabilize hippocampal spatial firing. Unstable grid cells retain internally coherent, hexagonally arranged firing fields, but these fields "drift" with respect to the virtual environment over periods >5 s. Optic flow from a virtual floor does not slow drift dynamics, emphasizing the importance of boundary-related visual information. Surprisingly, place fields are more stable close to boundaries even with floor and wall cues removed, suggesting invisible boundaries are inferred using the motion of a discrete, separate cue (a beacon signaling reward location). Subsets of place cells show allocentric directional tuning toward the beacon, with strength of tuning correlating with place field stability when boundaries are removed.


Subject(s)
Cues , Grid Cells , Virtual Reality , Animals , Grid Cells/physiology , Male , Hippocampus/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Rats , Place Cells/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Rats, Long-Evans , Orientation/physiology
3.
Neuroimage ; 293: 120634, 2024 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38705431

ABSTRACT

Spatial image transformation of the self-body is a fundamental function of visual perspective-taking. Recent research underscores the significance of intero-exteroceptive information integration to construct representations of our embodied self. This raises the intriguing hypothesis that interoceptive processing might be involved in the spatial image transformation of the self-body. To test this hypothesis, the present study used functional magnetic resonance imaging to measure brain activity during an arm laterality judgment (ALJ) task. In this task, participants were tasked with discerning whether the outstretched arm of a human figure, viewed from the front or back, was the right or left hand. The reaction times for the ALJ task proved longer when the stimulus presented orientations of 0°, 90°, and 270° relative to the upright orientation, and when the front view was presented rather than the back view. Reflecting the increased reaction time, increased brain activity was manifested in a cluster centered on the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), suggesting that the activation reflects the involvement of an embodied simulation in ALJ. Furthermore, this cluster of brain activity exhibited overlap with regions where the difference in activation between the front and back views positively correlated with the participants' interoceptive sensitivity, as assessed through the heartbeat discrimination task, within the pregenual ACC. These results suggest that the ACC plays an important role in integrating intero-exteroceptive cues to spatially transform the image of our self-body.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping , Gyrus Cinguli , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Humans , Gyrus Cinguli/physiology , Gyrus Cinguli/diagnostic imaging , Female , Male , Young Adult , Adult , Brain Mapping/methods , Interoception/physiology , Body Image , Functional Laterality/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Arm/physiology
4.
Sci Rep ; 14(1): 10304, 2024 05 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38705917

ABSTRACT

Understanding neurogenetic mechanisms underlying neuropsychiatric disorders such as schizophrenia and autism is complicated by their inherent clinical and genetic heterogeneity. Williams syndrome (WS), a rare neurodevelopmental condition in which both the genetic alteration (hemideletion of ~ twenty-six 7q11.23 genes) and the cognitive/behavioral profile are well-defined, offers an invaluable opportunity to delineate gene-brain-behavior relationships. People with WS are characterized by increased social drive, including particular interest in faces, together with hallmark difficulty in visuospatial processing. Prior work, primarily in adults with WS, has searched for neural correlates of these characteristics, with reports of altered fusiform gyrus function while viewing socioemotional stimuli such as faces, along with hypoactivation of the intraparietal sulcus during visuospatial processing. Here, we investigated neural function in children and adolescents with WS by using four separate fMRI paradigms, two that probe each of these two cognitive/behavioral domains. During the two visuospatial tasks, but not during the two face processing tasks, we found bilateral intraparietal sulcus hypoactivation in WS. In contrast, during both face processing tasks, but not during the visuospatial tasks, we found fusiform hyperactivation. These data not only demonstrate that previous findings in adults with WS are also present in childhood and adolescence, but also provide a clear example that genetic mechanisms can bias neural circuit function, thereby affecting behavioral traits.


Subject(s)
Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Williams Syndrome , Humans , Williams Syndrome/physiopathology , Williams Syndrome/genetics , Williams Syndrome/diagnostic imaging , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Adolescent , Child , Female , Male , Brain Mapping/methods , Brain/diagnostic imaging , Brain/physiopathology , Face , Facial Recognition/physiology , Parietal Lobe/physiopathology , Parietal Lobe/diagnostic imaging , Space Perception/physiology
5.
Sci Rep ; 14(1): 10377, 2024 05 06.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38710784

ABSTRACT

This study investigated the development of spatiotemporal perceptual interactions in 5-to-7 years old children. Participants reproduced the temporal and spatial interval between sequentially presented visual stimuli. The time and spacing between stimuli were experimentally manipulated. In addition, cognitive capacities were assessed using neuropsychological tests. Results revealed that starting at 5 years old, children exhibited spatial biases in their time estimations and temporal biases in their spatial estimations, pointing at space-time interference. In line with developmental improvement of temporal and spatial abilities, these spatiotemporal biases decreased with age. Importantly, short-term memory capacity was a predictor of space-time interference pointing to shared cognitive mechanisms between time and space processing. Our results support the symmetrical hypothesis that proposes a common neurocognitive mechanism for processing time and space.


Subject(s)
Memory, Short-Term , Space Perception , Humans , Child, Preschool , Female , Child , Male , Space Perception/physiology , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Time Perception/physiology , Child Development/physiology , Cognition/physiology , Neuropsychological Tests , Photic Stimulation/methods
6.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 45(7): e26690, 2024 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38703117

ABSTRACT

One potential application of forensic "brain reading" is to test whether a suspect has previously experienced a crime scene. Here, we investigated whether it is possible to decode real life autobiographic exposure to spatial locations using fMRI. In the first session, participants visited four out of eight possible rooms on a university campus. During a subsequent scanning session, subjects passively viewed pictures and videos from these eight possible rooms (four old, four novel) without giving any responses. A multivariate searchlight analysis was employed that trained a classifier to distinguish between "seen" versus "unseen" stimuli from a subset of six rooms. We found that bilateral precuneus encoded information that can be used to distinguish between previously seen and unseen rooms and that also generalized to the two stimuli left out from training. We conclude that activity in bilateral precuneus is associated with the memory of previously visited rooms, irrespective of the identity of the room, thus supporting a parietal contribution to episodic memory for spatial locations. Importantly, we could decode whether a room was visited in real life without the need of explicit judgments about the rooms. This suggests that recognition is an automatic response that can be decoded from fMRI data, thus potentially supporting forensic applications of concealed information tests for crime scene recognition.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Parietal Lobe , Recognition, Psychology , Humans , Male , Female , Parietal Lobe/physiology , Parietal Lobe/diagnostic imaging , Young Adult , Recognition, Psychology/physiology , Brain Mapping/methods , Adult , Photic Stimulation/methods , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Memory, Episodic
7.
Cereb Cortex ; 34(13): 19-29, 2024 May 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38696600

ABSTRACT

While fronto-posterior underconnectivity has often been reported in autism, it was shown that different contexts may modulate between-group differences in functional connectivity. Here, we assessed how different task paradigms modulate functional connectivity differences in a young autistic sample relative to typically developing children. Twenty-three autistic and 23 typically developing children aged 6 to 15 years underwent functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) scanning while completing a reasoning task with visuospatial versus semantic content. We observed distinct connectivity patterns in autistic versus typical children as a function of task type (visuospatial vs. semantic) and problem complexity (visual matching vs. reasoning), despite similar performance. For semantic reasoning problems, there was no significant between-group differences in connectivity. However, during visuospatial reasoning problems, we observed occipital-occipital, occipital-temporal, and occipital-frontal over-connectivity in autistic children relative to typical children. Also, increasing the complexity of visuospatial problems resulted in increased functional connectivity between occipital, posterior (temporal), and anterior (frontal) brain regions in autistic participants, more so than in typical children. Our results add to several studies now demonstrating that the connectivity alterations in autistic relative to neurotypical individuals are much more complex than previously thought and depend on both task type and task complexity and their respective underlying cognitive processes.


Subject(s)
Autistic Disorder , Brain , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Semantics , Humans , Child , Male , Adolescent , Female , Autistic Disorder/physiopathology , Autistic Disorder/diagnostic imaging , Autistic Disorder/psychology , Brain/diagnostic imaging , Brain/physiopathology , Brain Mapping , Space Perception/physiology , Neural Pathways/physiopathology , Neural Pathways/diagnostic imaging
8.
Conscious Cogn ; 121: 103696, 2024 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38703539

ABSTRACT

A serial reaction time task was used to test whether the representations of a probabilistic second-order sequence structure are (i) stored in an effector-dependent, effector-independent intrinsic or effector-independent visuospatial code and (ii) are inter-manually accessible. Participants were trained either with the dominant or non-dominant hand. Tests were performed with both hands in the practice sequence, a random sequence, and a mirror sequence. Learning did not differ significantly between left and right-hand practice, suggesting symmetric intermanual transfer from the dominant to the non-dominant hand and vice versa. In the posttest, RTs were shorter for the practice sequence than for the random sequence, and longest for the mirror sequence. Participants were unable to freely generate or recognize the practice sequence, indicating implicit knowledge of the probabilistic sequence structure. Because sequence-specific learning did not differ significantly between hands, we conclude that representations of the probabilistic sequence structure are stored in an effector-independent visuospatial code.


Subject(s)
Reaction Time , Space Perception , Transfer, Psychology , Humans , Male , Female , Adult , Reaction Time/physiology , Young Adult , Space Perception/physiology , Transfer, Psychology/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Functional Laterality/physiology , Serial Learning/physiology , Practice, Psychological , Hand/physiology
9.
Nat Commun ; 15(1): 3221, 2024 Apr 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38622129

ABSTRACT

The hippocampus creates a cognitive map of the external environment by encoding spatial and self-motion-related information. However, it is unclear whether hippocampal neurons could also incorporate internal cognitive states reflecting an animal's exploratory intention, which is not driven by rewards or unexpected sensory stimuli. In this study, a subgroup of CA1 neurons was found to encode both spatial information and animals' investigatory intentions in male mice. These neurons became active before the initiation of exploration behaviors at specific locations and were nearly silent when the same fields were traversed without exploration. Interestingly, this neuronal activity could not be explained by object features, rewards, or mismatches in environmental cues. Inhibition of the lateral entorhinal cortex decreased the activity of these cells during exploration. Our findings demonstrate that hippocampal neurons may bridge external and internal signals, indicating a potential connection between spatial representation and intentional states in the construction of internal navigation systems.


Subject(s)
Intention , Spatial Navigation , Male , Mice , Animals , Space Perception/physiology , Hippocampus/physiology , Entorhinal Cortex , Cues , Spatial Navigation/physiology
10.
Memory ; 32(4): 411-430, 2024 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38588665

ABSTRACT

In our lived environments, objects are often semantically organised (e.g., cookware and cutlery are placed close together in the kitchen). Across four experiments, we examined how semantic partitions (that group same-category objects in space) influenced memory for object locations. Participants learned the locations of items in a semantically partitioned display (where each partition contained objects from a single category) as well as a purely visually partitioned display (where each partition contained a scrambled assortment of objects from different categories). Semantic partitions significantly improved location memory accuracy compared to the scrambled display. However, when the correct partition was cued (highlighted) to participants during recall, performance on the semantically partitioned display was similar to the scrambled display. These results suggest that semantic partitions largely benefit memory for location by enhancing the ability to use the given category as a cue for a visually partitioned area (e.g., toys - top left). Our results demonstrate that semantically structured spaces help location memory across partitions, but not items within a partition, providing new insights into the interaction between meaning and memory.


Subject(s)
Cues , Mental Recall , Semantics , Humans , Female , Male , Young Adult , Mental Recall/physiology , Adult , Space Perception/physiology , Spatial Memory/physiology , Memory/physiology
11.
Brain Behav ; 14(5): e3496, 2024 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38688878

ABSTRACT

INTRODUCTION: The internal representation of verticality could be disturbed when a lesion in the central nervous system (CNS) affects the centers where information from the vestibular, visual, and/or somatosensory systems, increasing the risk of falling. OBJECTIVE: The aim was to evaluate the vestibular and somatosensory contribution to the verticality pattern in patients with stroke and other neurological disorders. METHODS: A literature search was performed in PubMed, Scopus, Web of Science, and CINAHL databases. Cross-sectional, case-control, and cohort studies comparing body verticality in patients with stroke or CNS diseases (CNSD) versus healthy controls were selected. Subjective postural vertical (SPV) in roll and pitch planes was used as the primary variable. RESULTS: Ten studies reporting data from 390 subjects were included. The overall effect for CNSD patients showed a misperception of body verticality in roll (standardized mean difference [SMD] = 1.05; 95% confidence interval [CI] .84-1.25) and pitch planes (SMD = 1.03; 95% CI .51-1.55). In subgroup analyses, a high effect was observed in the perception of SPV both in roll and pitch planes in stroke (p = .002) and other CNSD (p < .001). CONCLUSION: These findings suggest a potential misperception of SPV in patients with stroke and other neurological disturbances. Patients with CNSD could present an alteration of vestibular and somatosensory contribution to verticality construction, particularly stroke patients with pusher syndrome (PS), followed by those with PS combined with hemineglect.


Subject(s)
Perceptual Disorders , Humans , Nervous System Diseases/physiopathology , Perceptual Disorders/physiopathology , Perceptual Disorders/etiology , Proprioception/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Stroke/physiopathology , Stroke/psychology
12.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 50(6): 605-625, 2024 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38573695

ABSTRACT

Object-based warping is a visual illusion in which dots appear farther apart from each other when superimposed on an object. Previous research found that the illusion's strength varies with the perceived objecthood of the display. We tested whether objecthood alone determines the strength of the visual illusion or if low-level factors separable from objecthood also play a role. In Experiments 1-2, we varied low-level features to assess their impact on the warping illusion. We found that the warping illusion is equally strong for a variety of shapes but varies with the elements by which shape is defined. Shapes composed of continuous edges produced larger warping effects than shapes defined by disconnected elements. In Experiment 3, we varied a display's objecthood while holding low-level features constant. Displays with matched low-level features produced warping effects of the same size even when the perceived unity of the elements in the display varied. In Experiments 4-6, we tested whether displays with low-level features predicted to be important in spatial warping produced the visual illusion even when the display weakly configured into a single object. Results showed that the presence of low-level features like contour solidity and convexity determined warping effect sizes over and above what could be accounted for by the display's perceived objecthood. Our findings challenge the view that the spatial warping illusion is solely object-based. Other factors like the solidity of contours and contours' position relative to reference dots appear to play separate and important roles in determining warping effect sizes. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).


Subject(s)
Form Perception , Optical Illusions , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Humans , Adult , Young Adult , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Optical Illusions/physiology , Form Perception/physiology , Male , Female , Space Perception/physiology
13.
Appetite ; 198: 107337, 2024 Jul 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38579980

ABSTRACT

Previous research explored the spatial representations of healthy low-calorie and unhealthy high-calorie food items, revealing an association of healthy low-calorie food with left and top sides, and unhealthy/high-calorie food with right and top sides. This association, namely side bias, was limited to these specific categories leaving the representations of healthy high-calorie and unhealthy low-calorie food categories yet to be explored. Present study was designed to examine the spatial representation of four food categories (unhealthy low-calorie, unhealthy high-calorie, healthy low-calorie, healthy high-calorie) using a computerized food placement task. In Experiment 1, participants placed four food items from different categories into eight locations. In Experiment 2, identical task was used with the addition of centrally presented anchor food item to investigate the mental representation of food items in relation to each other. The frequency of placing food items in specific spatial locations were measured. The results of Experiment 1 provided partial support for side bias. However, the use of anchor items in Experiment 2 provided compelling evidence for vertical side bias, demonstrating consistent pattern of placing healthy foods on the upper sides and unhealthy foods on the lower sides. In both experiments, real-life food choices were examined to investigate whether the high-calorie bias would be observed in actual food choice behavior. The results from both experiments indicated strong preference to select high-calorie foods, supporting high-calorie bias. Overall, this study extends the evidence on the spatial representations of distinct food categories.


Subject(s)
Choice Behavior , Food Preferences , Humans , Female , Male , Food Preferences/psychology , Young Adult , Adult , Energy Intake , Adolescent , Diet, Healthy/psychology , Space Perception
14.
Exp Brain Res ; 242(6): 1421-1428, 2024 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38647701

ABSTRACT

Unilateral spatial neglect (USN) results from impaired attentional networks and can affect various sensory modalities, such as visual and somatosensory. The rodent medial agranular cortex (AGm), located in the medial part of the forebrain from rostral to caudal direction, is considered a region associated with spatial attention. The AGm selectively receives multisensory input with the rostral AGm receiving somatosensory input and caudal part receiving visual input. Our previous study showed slower recovery from neglect with anterior AGm lesion using the somatosensory neglect assessment. Conversely, the functional differences in spatial attention across the entire AGm locations (anterior, intermediate, and posterior parts) are unknown. Here, we investigated the relationship between the severity of neglect and various locations across the entire AGm in a mouse stroke model using a newly developed program-based analysis method that does not require human intervention. Among various positions of the lesions, the recovery from USN during recovery periods (postoperative day; POD 10-18) tended to be slower in cases with more rostral lesions in the AGm (r = - 0.302; p = 0.028). Moreover, the total number of arm entries and maximum moving speed did not significantly differ between before and after AGm infarction. According to these results, the anterior lesions may slowly recover from USN-like behavior, and there may be a weak association between the AGm infarct site and recovery rate. In addition, all unilateral focal infarctions in the AGm induced USN-like behavior without motor deficits.


Subject(s)
Disease Models, Animal , Perceptual Disorders , Animals , Perceptual Disorders/physiopathology , Perceptual Disorders/etiology , Male , Mice , Mice, Inbred C57BL , Functional Laterality/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Stroke/physiopathology , Stroke/complications , Cerebral Cortex/physiopathology
15.
Exp Brain Res ; 242(6): 1429-1438, 2024 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38652274

ABSTRACT

The ability to adapt to the environment is linked to the possibility of inhibiting inappropriate behaviours, and this ability can be enhanced by attention. Despite this premise, the scientific literature that assesses how attention can influence inhibition is still limited. This study contributes to this topic by evaluating whether spatial and moving attentional cueing can influence inhibitory control. We employed a task in which subjects viewed a vertical bar on the screen that, from a central position, moved either left or right where two circles were positioned. Subjects were asked to respond by pressing a key when the motion of the bar was interrupted close to the circle (go signal). In about 40% of the trials, following the go signal and after a variable delay, a visual target appeared in either one of the circles, requiring response inhibition (stop signal). In most of the trials the stop signal appeared on the same side as the go signal (valid condition), while in the others, it appeared on the opposite side (invalid condition). We found that spatial and moving cueing facilitates inhibitory control in the valid condition. This facilitation was observed especially for stop signals that appeared within 250ms of the presentation of the go signal, thus suggesting an involvement of exogenous attentional orienting. This work demonstrates that spatial and moving cueing can influence inhibitory control, providing a contribution to the investigation of the relationship between spatial attention and inhibitory control.


Subject(s)
Attention , Cues , Inhibition, Psychological , Psychomotor Performance , Reaction Time , Space Perception , Humans , Attention/physiology , Male , Female , Young Adult , Space Perception/physiology , Adult , Reaction Time/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Photic Stimulation/methods
16.
Acta Psychol (Amst) ; 246: 104246, 2024 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38615594

ABSTRACT

Math difficulties (MDs) occur in about 3-7 % of children and have been associated with academic, health, and occupational challenges. To date, findings about the role of working memory in MDs have been conflicting. The Automated Working Memory Assessment Battery (AWMA), which assesses all components of Baddeley and Hitch's model of working memory, was used to investigate which component of the model was most related to math calculation skills in elementary-school children. Participants were 94 (52 male) children (M age = 9 years 1 month; Range = 6 years 0 months to 11 years 8 months). As hypothesized, math calculation scores were correlated with all four working memory components (phonological loop, visuospatial sketchpad, verbal and visuospatial central executive). After accounting for age, phonological processing, and attention, the visuospatial sketchpad was the only memory component that contributed to the prediction of math calculation scores, explaining an additional 10.2 % of unique variance. Short-term visuospatial memory should be assessed in children having difficulty with math and children could benefit from interventions that include attention to the development of both visuospatial memory and math calculation skills. This study did not use a longitudinal design and so we cannot conclude that weak visuospatial memory impedes the development of math calculation skills. Future research should use longitudinal designs and investigate other types of math skills.


Subject(s)
Mathematics , Memory, Short-Term , Humans , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Male , Female , Child , Neuropsychological Tests/statistics & numerical data , Space Perception/physiology , Attention/physiology
17.
Acta Psychol (Amst) ; 246: 104287, 2024 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38670043

ABSTRACT

Although the SNARC effect in the processing of most magnitude stimuli and sequence stimuli has been reported for the past 30 years, it remains unclear whether this effect is caused by the spatial representation or polarity encoding of stimuli. In the present study, we designed five experiments using a four-way classification task to evaluate the ability of spatial representation theory and polarity encoding theory to explain the SNARC effect in the processing of number and sequence stimuli. In all five experiments in the present study, stimuli (Experiments 1 and 4: four different Arabic numbers, Experiment 2: sequence stimuli, Experiment 3: ordinal sequences relevant to working memory, Experiment 5: Chinese characters without any implicit spatial information) were centrally presented. Participants were asked to respond to specific number or sequence stimuli by pressing the A, S, K, and L keys in consistent trials (or the L, K, S, and A keys in inconsistent trials). The results showed that (1) the SNARC effect occurred in the processing of number and sequence stimuli both when only one specific number was mapped to one specific key (Experiments 1, 2 and 3) and when two numbers were mapped to one specific key (Experiment 4). (2) There was not a SNARC effect when the numbers were replaced with Chinese characters without any implicit spatial information (Experiment 5). The results of these five experiments imply that the SNARC effect in the processing of magnitude stimuli, including numbers and sequences, originates from the spatial representation of stimuli, supporting spatial representation theory.


Subject(s)
Space Perception , Humans , Male , Female , Young Adult , Space Perception/physiology , Adult , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology
18.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 86(4): 1342-1359, 2024 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38561567

ABSTRACT

Atypical orienting of visuospatial attention in autistic individuals or individuals with a high level of autistic-like traits (ALTs) has been well documented and viewed as a core feature underlying the development of autism. However, there has been limited testing of three alternative theoretical positions advanced to explain atypical orienting - difficulty in disengagement, cue indifference, and delay in orienting. Moreover, research commonly has not separated facilitation (reaction time difference between neutral and valid cues) and cost effects (reaction time difference between invalid and neutral cues) in orienting tasks. We addressed these limitations in two experiments that compared groups selected for Low- and High-ALT levels on exogenous and endogenous versions of the Posner cueing paradigm. Experiment 1 showed that High-ALT participants exhibited a significantly reduced cost effect compared to Low-ALT participants in the endogenous cueing task, although the overall orienting effect remained small. In Experiment 2, we increased task difficulty of the endogenous task to augment cueing effects. Results were comparable to Experiment 1 regarding the finding of a reduced cost effect for High-ALT participants on the endogenous cueing task and additionally demonstrated a reduced facilitation effect in High-ALT participants on the same task. No ALT group differences were observed on an exogenous cueing task included in Experiment 2. These findings suggest atypical orienting in High-ALT individuals may be attributable to general cue indifference, which implicates differences in top-down attentional processes between Low- and High-ALT individuals. We discuss how indifference to endogenous cues may contribute to social cognitive differences in autism.


Subject(s)
Attention , Autistic Disorder , Cues , Reaction Time , Humans , Male , Female , Attention/physiology , Young Adult , Autistic Disorder/psychology , Space Perception/physiology , Adult , Adolescent , Orientation , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology
19.
Conscious Cogn ; 121: 103694, 2024 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38657474

ABSTRACT

Mental rotation tasks are frequently used as standard measures of mental imagery. However, aphantasia research has brought such use into question. Here, we assessed a large group of individuals who lack visual imagery (aphantasia) on two mental rotation tasks: a three-dimensional block-shape, and a human manikin rotation task. In both tasks, those with aphantasia had slower, but more accurate responses than controls. Both groups demonstrated classic linear increases in response time and error-rate as functions of angular disparity. In the three-dimensional block-shape rotation task, a within-group speed-accuracy trade-off was found in controls, whereas faster individuals in the aphantasia group were also more accurate. Control participants generally favoured using object-based mental rotation strategies, whereas those with aphantasia favoured analytic strategies. These results suggest that visual imagery is not crucial for successful performance in classical mental rotation tasks, as alternative strategies can be effectively utilised in the absence of holistic mental representations.


Subject(s)
Imagination , Humans , Imagination/physiology , Male , Adult , Female , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Young Adult , Space Perception/physiology , Rotation , Middle Aged , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology
20.
Sci Rep ; 14(1): 9228, 2024 04 22.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38649675

ABSTRACT

Psychophysical studies typically test attentional mechanisms in isolation, but in everyday life they interact to optimize human behavior. We investigated whether spatial and temporal attention interact in two orientation discrimination experiments that vary in task demand. We manipulated temporal and spatial attention separately and conjointly with well-established methods for testing each spatial or temporal attention. We assessed sensitivity (d') and reaction time for every combination of spatial and timing cues, each of which was valid, neutral, or invalid. Spatial attention modulated sensitivity (d') and speed (reaction time) across temporal attention conditions. Temporal attention modulated sensitivity and speed under high- but not low- task demands. Furthermore, spatial and temporal attention interacted for the high-demand task. This study reveals that task demand matters; in a simple task spatial attention suffices to improve performance, whereas in a more demanding task both spatial and temporal attention interact to boost performance, albeit in a subadditive fashion.


Subject(s)
Attention , Reaction Time , Space Perception , Humans , Attention/physiology , Female , Male , Reaction Time/physiology , Adult , Space Perception/physiology , Young Adult , Cues , Task Performance and Analysis
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...